Introduction

Stress is common and unavoidable, and exposure to stress of various forms can significantly alter brain function and promote cognitive and emotional disorders.1, 2, 3 Effects of stress on cognition are complex and depend on factors including the nature, duration and timing of stress, and the specific cognitive task employed.4, 5, 6 The hippocampus contributes greatly to memory,7,8 and it has been increasingly recognized that processing of sensory signals into memories in rodents involves dorsal hippocampus networks.9,10 In contrast, ventral hippocampus is endowed with distinct afferent/efferent connections9,11 including robust pathways to amygdala,12,13 which may enhance the emotional salience of memories.9,14 Stress and stress hormones influence dorsal and ventral hippocampus differentially: chronic stress attenuates long-term potentiation in dorsal, but not ventral CA1, suggesting that discrete mechanisms exist for the effects of stress mediators on dorsoventral components of the hippocampal formation.15

The effects of stress on memory have received intense investigation, showing that the duration of stress, for example, acute vs chronic, crucially influences its effect on memory function.16, 17, 18, 19, 20, 21 However, the potential importance of the complexity of stress has not been clarified. Regarding chronic stress, habituation occurs to a single stress, and sequential different stresses are required to maintain stress responses.22,23 In the context of hours-long stress, it is unclear if the effects of combined or multimodal stress on learning and memory differ from those of a discrete stress, independent of stress severity. This is important, because modern-life stress often involves multiple concurrent psychological, social and physical stresses. Here, we tested the hypothesis that a relatively short (hours) multimodal stress has more profound impact on memory compared with a unimodal stress of equal duration and severity. We further queried if multimodal and unimodal stresses influenced dorsal and ventral hippocampus differentially. Using novel object recognition which involves hippocampal and parahippocampal structures,24 we found that memory problems of mice exposed to multimodal stress were significantly greater than those of mice exposed to restraint alone, although hormonal and hypothalamic stress responses did not differ following each stress. Analyses of hippocampal dendritic spines suggested that the greater memory impairment might reflect greater loss of synapses in dorsal CA1, a hippocampal region crucially important for long-term memory,25 with relative preservation of ventral synapses. Further, brain structures and networks were differentially activated by the qualitatively different forms of stress, with augmented connectivity of ventral-hippocampus-amygdala circuits after multimodal stress. Together, these data suggest that multimodal stress adversely impacts dorsal versus ventral hippocampal circuits, with distinct consequences for memory function.

Materials and Methods

A complete description is found in the Supplementary Information.

Animals

Three-month-old C57BL/6J male mice were used. Animals were group-housed in a quiet, uncrowded facility on a 12 h light/dark cycle, with ad libitum access to lab chow and water. In addition, to examine if postsynaptic density protein 95 (PSD-95) was a reasonable marker for mature synapses, 3-month-old yellow fluorescent protein-expressing mice (n=3, Jackson Labs, Bar Harbor, ME, USA) were used to visualize the localization of PSD-95-ir puncta on dendritic spines of hippocampal pyramidal cells. Experiments were performed in accordance with the NIH guidelines on laboratory animal welfare and approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee.

Stress paradigms

Mice were assigned to one of several experimental groups: unimodal (restraint only), multimodal or stress-free control. For restraint stress, mice were placed in a restrainer fashioned from a 50 ml plastic tube in an empty, clean cage. For multimodal stress, mice were restrained and placed five per cage (social stress) on a laboratory shaker and jostled in a brightly lit room bathed in loud rap music. Both restraint-only and multimodal stress conditions lasted for 1, 2 or 5 h. For additional behavioral comparisons, another group of mice (n=5) was exposed to the loud music (noise) alone for 5 h. These mice were transported to the lab, but remained in their home cage under dim lights. Following the stress, mice were returned to their home cages (learning and memory experiments), killed by decapitation for plasma corticosterone levels, or anesthetized for perfusion.

Learning/memory testing

Restraint, noise (n=5), multimodal or control mice (n=8 per group) were tested in a novel object recognition task 90 min after the termination of the stress as described previously.26 A 90-minute delay period was chosen because previous studies demonstrated that it sufficed for stressed mice to recover, enabling them to explore the two objects for the same duration as non-stressed cohorts.26 Objects were counterbalanced across groups, and all objects were cleaned with ethanol between trials. Six hours after training, mice were exposed to one of the previously encountered objects and a novel object and allowed to explore for 5 min. Both training and testing phases were video-recorded and scored blindly without the knowledge of stress condition. Recognition memory of the familiar object was assessed using a discrimination index (DI).27

As measures of anxiety following stress, we analyzed exploration times as well as several locomotion parameters in a large open field apparatus (dimensions: 30 × 23 × 21.5 cm). To consider the potential confounding effects of anxiety on learning, the assessment was performed during the training phase of the novel object recognition test. A computerized video tracking system (Noldus Ethovision) was used to calculate the total distance traveled, as well as the time spent in, and frequency of entries into, the inner ‘anxiogenic’ regions of the apparatus.

Quantification of hippocampal synapses

A separate cohort of restraint stress, multimodal stress and control mice (n=5–9 mice per group) were used to assess the effects of each stress on the number of hippocampal synapses. The postsynaptic density 95 protein (PSD-95), which is localized primarily to dendritic spine heads, was used as a reliable marker for mature synapses.28, 29, 30 Mice were perfused immediately after the stress, and brains were postfixed and processed for immunohistochemistry as described in previous studies.31 Briefly, brains were sectioned coronally (for dorsal hippocampus) or horizontally into 20 μm thick slices. Immunohistochemistry was performed on free-floating sections and mouse anti-PSD-95 antibody (1:2000; Affinity BioReagents, Golden, CO, USA) was used. Antibody binding was visualized with anti-mouse IgG conjugated to Alexa Fluor 568 (1:200, Molecular Probes, Eugene, OR, USA). The restraint experiments and the multimodal stress experiments were run separately and each included a control group. Sections from each stress group and its control group were run concurrently in the same conditions, and analyzed without knowledge of treatment group (blindly). Deconvolution analysis was performed on wide-field three-dimensional images as described previously.26,31,32

Radioimmunoassay

Plasma corticosterone levels were measured in a subset of mice at one (n=3 per group) or five hours (n=4 per group) from stress onset, compared with stress-free controls (n=9), using a commercial kit.33 Assay sensitivity was 0.16 μg dl−1, and all samples were run in a single assay.

Neuronal activation

A separate cohort of restraint and multimodal stress mice (n=5 per group) were anesthetized after termination of the 5-hour stress (or under stress-free conditions, n=5) and perfused for Fos analyses. To obviate the possibility that the 5-hour time-point was not representative of patterns of Fos expression during earlier time points, additional cohorts of mice were perfused following 2 h of restraint or multimodal stress. Immunohistochemistry was carried out and sections from all experimental groups were run concurrently.

Statistical analysis

Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS 20.0 and GraphPad Prism 5.0 software. For DI values, anxiety measures, corticosterone, PSD-95-ir, and Fos-ir cells, separate one-way analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were performed, followed by Bonferroni post hoc comparisons. To study the linear relationship of Fos-activation among 12 selected brain regions, we calculated pair-wise Pearson correlation coefficients (r) separately for each stress group, correlating numbers of Fos-ir cells within each region to other regions. To identify significant differences in the strengths of these correlations, Fisher r to z transformations were used to directly compare r-coefficients between restraint and multimodal stress conditions. Mean DIs for each group were compared with chance levels using one-sample t-tests.

Results

Multimodal stress affects learning and memory to a greater degree than a unimodal stress

To study if the complexity of stress influences brain function, we developed a model of multimodal stress.26,34 Mice were subjected to concurrent psychological and physical stresses including restraint, jostling, bright light and unpredictable loud noise (rap music) for several hours (Figure 1a).35 The effects of this multimodal stress on memory were compared with those of a typical restraint paradigm or of rap music, of equal duration (five hours). We compared learning and memory of groups of mice subjected to either of these paradigms with memory of unstressed controls using a novel object recognition task.24,26 This task, which involves both hippocampal and cortical circuits,24,36,37 is advantageous because it requires little training, is not stressful in itself, and can be assessed in a relatively short time frame.38 Although restraint stress led to modest impairment in this task (Figure 1b), and the loud noise stress had no appreciable effect (Supplementary Figure 1a), the multimodal stress resulted in significantly worse memory of a familiar object. Specifically, after a recovery period of 90 min, the exploration time of all stress groups was similar to that of unstressed mice (one-way ANOVA, F2,21=0.01, P=0.99; Supplementary Figure 2a) and each group explored two identical objects for equal durations during the training phase (one-sample t-tests, all P>0.05; Supplementary Figure 2b). However, when tested for memory of these objects 6 h later, the discrimination index, reflecting differential object exploration while adjusting for potential differences in total exploration duration, demonstrated overall group differences in object recognition (F2,21=13.04, P<0.001). Post hoc comparisons revealed that multimodal stress reduced DI compared with both control- and restraint-stressed mice (all P<0.01; Figure 1b). Although restraint alone tended to reduce DI, these mice were not significantly worse than controls (P>0.05) and their object recognition index was above chance (t7=8.7, P<0.001; Figure 1b). As mentioned, the group of mice (n=5) exposed to the loud noise (rap music) alone for 5 h performed as well as the non-stressed control group (Supplementary Figure 1). Together, these memory tests in the restraint, loud noise and the multimodal stress groups demonstrate distinct impacts on learning and memory of hours-long exposure to a combined, multimodal stress as compared with more discrete (‘uni-modal’) stresses. They suggest that the underlying neural circuits of memory functions may be differentially impacted by a combined vs single stress.

Figure 1
figure 1

A multimodal stress impacted memory differently than a more discrete stress. (a) Mice exposed to five hours of restraint stress were compared with those exposed to the same duration of a multimodal stress consisting of restraint, light, loud music and jostling. (b) During the testing phase of the novel object recognition task, the Discrimination index (DI) demonstrated that mice exposed to multimodal stress failed to distinguish the familiar object from a novel one. (c) Hormonal stress responses were comparable following restraint or multimodal stress. The levels and trajectories of plasma corticosterone at several time points after a restraint or a multimodal stress were indistinguishable. This was the case both at 1 h after the onset of stress, and at the 5-h time point (when stresses were terminated). n=3–5 per group per time point. (d) The total numbers of activated (Fos-ir) cells within the hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus were similar following restraint or multimodal stress, either 2 h from the onset of stress or at its termination (n=3–5 per group per time point). (e) Photomicrographs showing typical Fos staining within the PVN are quantified in d. Together, these data indicate that the two stress paradigms stimulate the neuroendocrine stress response system to a similar degree, that is, they do not differ in overall intensity. * signifies compared with stress-free controls, # signifies compared with restraint. 3v, third ventricle; f, fornix; PVN, paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus.

PowerPoint slide

Stronger memory effects of multimodal stress are not associated with differential stress or anxiety responses

Differences in memory between the two stress paradigms might result if the multimodal paradigm simply elicited a much stronger physiological or behavioral stress response. To test for a potentially larger hormonal stress response following multimodal stress, plasma levels of the stress hormone corticosterone were measured at several time points following either stress, and the pattern and trajectory of corticosterone levels were compared. Restraint and multimodal stress elicited similar corticosterone levels (Figure 1c), both at one hour after the onset of the stress and at the 5 h time point when stress was terminated (all P>0.05), suggesting that the two stress paradigms stimulated the physiological stress response to a similar degree. Corticosterone release is driven by the activation of corticotropin-releasing hormone-expressing cells within the hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus (PVN) and subsequent release of hypothalamic corticotropin-releasing hormone and pituitary adrenocorticotropic hormone. In line with the comparable plasma corticosterone levels, the numbers of Fos-immunoreactive (ir) cells in PVN, either two hours from the onset of stress39 or at its termination (Figures 1d and e), did not distinguish multimodal from unimodal stress (restraint vs multimodal). These data indicate that the two forms of stress stimulated physiological responses governed by the neuroendocrine hypothalamic–pituitary adrenal stress system to a comparable degree, and suggest that the two stress paradigms did not differ appreciably in their overall intensity or magnitude.

In addition, to examine if the stress paradigms provoked different levels of anxiety during the learning task, we analyzed several independent measures of anxiety. First, exploration durations during the training phase (90 min post-stress termination) did not differ appreciably between the restraint and multimodal stresses (Supplementary Figure 2a). These findings excluded anxiety-induced poor exploration as a source of the memory defects. In addition, we examined locomotion in a large, open-field arena. We assessed distance traveled and the classical anxiety measures of the duration of time spent in the inner region of the apparatus and the frequency of entries into the inner region (Supplementary Figure 3). There were no differences across stress conditions in total distance traveled during the training phase that followed the stresses, as well as the duration of time spent in the inner region of the apparatus or the frequency of entries into the inner region (one-way ANOVAs, main effect of stress: all P>0.05). Although there was a main effect of stress on total exploration time during the testing period (7.5 h after stress termination, F2,21=4.94, P<0.05), there was no difference between the two stress groups (P>05; Supplementary Figure 2c). Thus, hours-long multimodal and restraint stress did not lead to differential levels of anxiety-like behavior when the animals learned the task.

Greater memory impairment is associated with differential loss of hippocampal excitatory synapses following multimodal versus restraint stress

What might be the mechanisms underlying the more severe memory impairment provoked by short multimodal stress? Stress has been shown to influence the number of excitatory synapses on dendritic spines already within hours26,40 in brain regions including the hippocampus.34,41,42 A differential loss of hippocampal synapses would be a plausible mechanism for the effects of multimodal stress on memory.

We tested this hypothesis by quantifying PSD-95, a marker for mature synapses, in hippocampal regions harboring the apical dendrites of CA3 and CA1 pyramidal cells.28, 29, 30 We used wide-field imaging followed by 3-dimensional deconvolution tomography, a method enabling analysis of hundreds of thousands of synapses.43,44 We sampled systematically dorsal (septal) and ventral (temporal) hippocampus, and compared the effects of restraint and multimodal stresses (Figures 2a and d). PSD-95 localized to the heads of GFP-expressing dendritic spines of hippocampal pyramidal neurons, (Figure 2c), so that counts of PSD-95 puncta provided a measure of dendritic spines and synapses.31

Figure 2
figure 2

A differential loss of synapses in the hippocampus following restraint vs multimodal stress. (a) Diagramatic representation of the analyzed sections: serial sections (20 μm) were collected from the dorsal hippocampus (commencing at 400 μm from the septal pole) and ventral hippocampus at the coronal and horizontal plan, respectively, with intersection intervals of 100 μm. (b) Diagram illustrating the location of regions analyzed for synapse and spine density. Boxed areas in the stratum radiatum of CA1, CA3 were imaged for the deconvolution tomography analysis of PSD-95 (postsynaptic density protein 95)-ir puncta. *, hippocampal fissure. DG, dentate gyrus; SLM, stratum lacunosum-moleculare; SO, stratum oriens; SP, stratum pyramidale; SR, stratum radiatum. (c) Illustration of combined methodologies employed to establish that PSD-95 puncta are reasonable markers of dendritic spine heads (arrows). This is shown by confocal microscopy of dendrite from a 3-month-old YFP-expressing mouse, subjected to immunohistochemistry for the synaptic marker. The thickness of the optical section is 0.2 μm. (d) Quantification of the number of PSD-95-ir puncta as measures of excitatory synapses. Restraint stress resulted in a modest reduction of PSD-95 in dorsal CA3, but not in ventral CA3 nor in dorsal or ventral CA1. In contrast, multimodal stress provoked a robust loss of PSD-95-ir puncta in the CA3 area and a selective loss in the dorsal CA1, a hippocampal area implicated as crucial for long-term memory (see McQuown et al.25). Scale bar, C, 1 μm. * signifies compared with stress-free controls, # signifies compared with restraint.

PowerPoint slide

There were significant differences in the effects of restraint and multimodal stresses on overall hippocampal synaptic integrity, which varied along the dorsoventral hippocampal axis. Restraint stress reduced PSD-95-ir puncta only in the dorsal CA3 (P<0.05), sparing the ventral CA3 synapses (P>0.05); restraint stress had no significant effects on either the dorsal or the ventral CA1 synapses (all P>0.05; Figure 2d). In contrast, multimodal stress provoked spine loss throughout CA3 (dorsal: P<0.05; ventral: P<0.01; Figure 2d). Remarkably, within hippocampal area CA1, synapse loss varied dramatically along the dorsoventral axis of the hippocampus; multimodal stress reduced PSD-95 puncta counts drastically, but only in the dorsal CA1 (P<0.001), a region crucial to normal memory function,25 with preserved synapses within the ventral CA1 (P>0.05). Of note, in the dorsal CA1, multimodal stress led to significantly more synapse loss compared with restraint (P<0.05).

Unimodal and multimodal stresses of comparable magnitude lead to distinct neuronal activation patterns

The data above suggest that the perception and processing within the brain of multimodal and restraint stresses of similar magnitudes might differ. Therefore, we employed Fos, a widely used marker, to map neuronal activation throughout the brain after the two types of stress.16,17,19,45 We analyzed Fos expression at the time point when differential learning and memory effects of the two stress paradigms were observed (end of the five-hour stress).

Analysis of Fos expression throughout the brain revealed that most regions were similarly activated by restraint or multimodal stress (Supplementary Table 1), consistent with the comparable overall intensity of the two stresses. However, differential patterns of activation were observed within key brain structures involved in stress processing and memory networks. Specifically, within the central amygdala nucleus (CEA) (F2,12=71.9, P<0.001) and lateral-posterior region of the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (BST) (F2,12=10.4, P=0.002), there were more Fos-ir cells following multimodal compared to restraint stress (Figures 3a and b; Supplementary Table 1). The CEA is the principal output of the amygdala, a region involved in emotional functions including anxiety and fear,46 and BST is afferent to stress-sensitive hypothalamic neurons.19,47 Interestingly, the opposite pattern was observed in the lateral (F2,12=34.4, P<0.001) and medial (F2,12=74.2, P<0.001) septum, where restraint stress led to higher numbers of Fos-expressing cells than multimodal stress (Figure 3c; Supplementary Table 1). The septum is a primary link from hippocampus to both cortical and subcortical regions48 and may gate sensory input to/from hippocampus, contributing to memory.

Figure 3
figure 3

Brain regions preferentially activated by the multimodal vs restraint stress. Within the central amygdala (a) and lateral-posterior region of the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (b), there were more Fos-ir cells following a 5 h multimodal compared with restraint stress. (c) The lateral and the medial nuclei of the septum contained more Fos-ir cells following the restraint stress (note different scales for the two subdivisions). (d) Hippocampal activation by restraint vs multimodal stress. Within the pyramidal cell layer and dentate gyrus, restraint or multimodal stress led to similar numbers of Fos-ir cells. Of note, the pyramidal cell layers of the ventral hippocampus were activated to a much greater degree than those in the dorsal hippocampus (Restraint: dorsal=41.8±4.3; ventral=251.9±28.1; t4=7.39, P<0.001; multimodal: dorsal=56.2±12.4; ventral=287.4±35.8; t8=6.1, P<0.001). Activation of the dorsal dentate gyrus was suppressed by either stress. * signifies compared with stress-free controls, # signifies compared with restraint. ac, anterior commissure; BLA, basolateral amygdala; BST, bed nucleus of the stria terminalis; BSTlp, lateral-posterior BST; BSTlv, lateral-ventral BST; BSTma, medial-anterior BST; BSTmv, medial-ventral BST; CC, corpus callosum; CEA, central amygdala; ec, external capsule; LS, lateral septum; LV, lateral ventricle; MS, medial septum.

PowerPoint slide

Restraint and multimodal stresses activated similar numbers of cells within the hippocampal pyramidal cell layers (all P>0.05; Figure 3d; Supplementary Table 1), suggesting that the hippocampus itself may not distinguish the complexity of stress. Fos-ir neurons were strikingly more numerous in ventral compared with dorsal hippocampal pyramidal cell layer of stressed mice (Figure 3d; note different y axes). These findings are consistent with the functional distinctions between dorsal and ventral hippocampus,9 and the involvement of the latter in stress-sensitive ‘emotional’ networks.49 Interestingly, Fos expression within dentate gyrus, the origin of afferents to the CA3 pyramidal cells appeared to be suppressed by both stresses (F2,12=6.7, P=0.011; Figure 3d).

Measures of functional connectivity of the hippocampus differ after restraint versus multimodal stress

The initial analysis of Fos expression described above established the general patterns of neuronal activation across stress-activated and information-processing circuits (Figure 4a). However, these data did not provide information about interactions among these regions. Indeed, rather than working independently, these structures are organized into functional networks50 that interact and contribute to the perception and processing of sensory input and subsequent emotional, behavioral and neurohormonal responses to stress.51,52 We employed Pearson cross-correlation matrices of regional Fos activation as surrogate parameters for functional connectivity among selected brain regions (Table 1).53 Pair-wise correlation values between two regions (ranging from −1 to 1) provided an index of the strength and nature of interactions among them in response to either multimodal or restraint stresses.

Figure 4
figure 4

Patterns of network activation by restraint or multimodal stress. (a) Analysis of Fos expression throughout the brain revealed distinct patterns of neural activation, summarized in the schematic. Although most regions were equally activated by restraint or multimodal stress, differential patterns of activation were observed within central amygdala, BST and septum, suggesting that these regions may discriminate the relative complexity of stress. (b) Measures of functional network connectivity differ following restraint or multimodal stress. Cross-correlation matrices of regional Fos activation levels were generated for each stress condition (Restraint, top; multimodal, bottom). Pearson r-values are color coded to reflect the direction and strength of each correlation, and significant correlations are indicated with an asterisk. These analyses revealed that both stress paradigms resulted in significant pair-wise correlations in activity and that the patterns of these correlations were influenced by stress complexity. (c) Focusing on the hippocampus, restraint stress led to strong correlations with the septum and thalamic paraventricular nucleus (left), whereas the multimodal stress shifted functional connectivity measures of the hippocampus away from the septum/thalamus, and increased correlations between hippocampus and amygdala and BST (right). Bold lines in (b) indicate significant correlations. BLA, basolateral amygdala; BST, bed nucleus of the stria terminalis; CA, pyramidal cell layer of hippocampus; CEA, central amygdala; d CA, dorsal CA; DG, dentate gyrus of hippocampus; d DG, dorsal DG; DMH, dorsomedial hypothalamus; EW, Edinger–Westphal nucleus; LH, lateral hypothalamus; LS, lateral septum; MEA, medial amygdala; MS, medial septum; PAG, periaqueductal gray; PIT, pituitary gland; PVN, paraventricular nucleus of hypothalamus; PVT, paraventricular thalamic nucleus; RN, raphe nucleus; Thal, thalamus; v CA, ventral CA; v DG, ventral DG; VMH, ventromedial hypothalamus.

PowerPoint slide

Table 1 Pearson correlation matrices for activation levels following restraint or multimodal stress

Both multimodal and restraint stress resulted in several significant pair-wise correlations of activation throughout brain structures as would be expected in a functional network (Figure 4b; Table 1). However, the patterns of these correlations, and in particular the significant correlations of the hippocampus, were influenced by the complexity of the stress. Following restraint stress, hippocampal activation was strongly correlated to activation within septum and the paraventricular region of the thalamus (PVT) (Figure 4b, top; 4c, left). Following multimodal stress of comparable duration and magnitude, the pattern of hippocampal connectivity shifted, reducing correlations with septum and PVT, and increasing correlation among hippocampus, amygdala and BST (Figure 4b, bottom; 4c, right). This shift in measures of functional connectivity was supported by directly comparing Pearson coefficients between the stress conditions (Table 1). These results suggest that hippocampal network-connectivity patterns distinguish between the two forms of stress.

Discussion

The principal findings of this study are (a) hours-long, multimodal stress provokes severe memory problems compared with restraint or to loud, unpredictable noise stress of comparable duration and hypothalamic–pituitary adrenal stress system axis stimulation; (b) the memory defects may be partially explained by the greater loss of synapses in hippocampus, and particularly in dorsal hippocampal CA1, following multimodal stress; (c) Fos mapping demonstrates that the brain distinguishes a stress that combines multiple components from a more discrete restraint stress. Together, these data indicate that the immediate effects of short stress on cognitive function are influenced by the complexity of the stress: combined psychological, social and physical stress lasting merely hours may lead to unexpectedly severe memory defects, with important implications to the function of individuals experiencing emotional trauma together with physical or social duress. The findings are also important to our understanding of stress-related disorders, such as posttraumatic stress disorder, where memory processes are deranged.

More severe memory problems follow hours-long multimodal vs restraint stress

Because of the prevalence of stress, its effects on cognitive function including memory have been extensively studied.54, 55, 56, 57 A major body of work has demonstrated the importance of stress duration. In general, acute stress, lasting seconds to minutes, enhances learning and memory.58, 59, 60, 61, 62, 63, 64, 65 By contrast, chronic stress, lasting weeks, generally impairs these processes.3,5,66, 67, 68, 69, 70, 71 In addition, distinct effects of specific modalities of stress (physical or psychological) have been well studied.19,20,35,72,73

Given that many stressful experiences are not unitary or discrete, this study sought to understand the effects of multiple concurrent stresses, and to determine whether the effects on the brain are distinguishable from those following single stress of similar intensity. We chose a duration of hours, because many stressful episodes may last hours rather than seconds or weeks, and are thus neither acute (minutes) nor chronic (days/weeks). Surprisingly, we found highly divergent effects of the multimodal compared with less complex stresses such as restraint or loud unpredictable noise on memory: these had only modest effects on object memory, whereas an equal duration of the multimodal stress abrogated object recognition.

Differential loss of hippocampal apical dendritic synapses may underlie impairment of memory after multimodal versus unimodal stress

Because general measures of the severity of the two stresses, including a time-course of plasma corticosterone and activation of stress-integrating centers in the PVN,73 did not differ appreciably (Figure 1), we examined alternative sources for the profound differential effects of these stresses on memory. We found major distinctions between multimodal and unimodal stress on excitatory synapses in hippocampal areas CA3 and CA1. Notably, the overall degree of synapse loss was far greater after the multimodal stress. There were also important variations between CA regions, as well as the dorsal-ventral plane of the hippocampus. Indeed, synaptic loss following restraint stress was mild, consistent with modest memory defects observed after this stress (Figure 1) and was confined to dorsal CA3 neurons, which are known to have high sensitivity to stress.2,23,74 In response to the combined stress, synaptic loss spread throughout the CA3 (dorsal and ventral) and into the dorsal CA1, leaving ventral CA1 intact.

These data seem to suggest a progressive loss of synapses from CA3 to CA1, corresponding with increased stress complexity. Interestingly, these data are in line with the greater role of memory functions subserved by dorsal hippocampus, and in particular by CA1 region25,75 and suggest that synapses within these circuits are specifically targeted in mice exposed to multimodal stress. This differential loss of dorsal CA1 synapses may lead to disrupted function of the memory networks associated with dorsal hippocampus.9,15

The current findings are congruent with previous studies showing that stress can influence the number of synapses already within hours40,55 in brain regions including the hippocampus.26,34,42 Neurotransmitters, including norepinephrine,76, 77, 78 serotonin79, 80, 81 and glutamate,82,83 as well as stress-induced release of neuropeptides26,34,84,85 and steroids,86, 87, 88 can influence synapse number and function. Building on the existing body of information, the current findings suggest that rapid, stress-induced changes in the structure and function of synapses may provide a mechanism for fine-tuning of specific neuronal networks in response to ever-changing environmental conditions.2 Indeed, selective loss of synapses in networks involved in memory may serve an adaptive role, attenuating memories of severe, adverse experiences.

The differential impairment of memory after multimodal vs unimodal stress is accompanied by a differential pattern of regional neuronal activation

Clues into the distinct brain responses that may explain the higher impact on memory of multimodal compared with restraint stress emerge from the activation patterns of specific brain regions and the measures of their interconnectivity. Expression of the immediate early-gene c-fos provides a useful tool to examine activation of neurons and identify brain regions recruited by stress.45,89,90 Indeed, various models of stress have been shown to activate neurons throughout the brain, including hypothalamus, amygdala, hippocampus and cortex.72,90, 91, 92, 93, 94, 95 Much of this work has categorized different forms of stress (physical, psychological, social) and has identified distinct and partially overlapping circuits recruited by different stress modalities.16,18, 19, 20,35,96,97

Here, Fos mapping revealed distinctive activation of specific neuronal populations and networks by a multimodal compared with a more discrete stress. Within the amygdala, we found higher levels of Fos expression in the CEA after the multimodal vs restraint stress. The CEA is rich in stress hormones and their receptors,98, 99, 100, 101 and contributes to processing of stress signals.102, 103, 104 Functionally, CEA has been implicated in learning and expression of fear responses,105, 106, 107 as well as the expression of stress-induced anxiety behaviors.108, 109, 110, 111, 112, 113 As the major outflow of the amygdala complex,46 higher activation of CEA might indicate a convergence of the several stressful components inherent in the multimodal stress and a potential for overwhelming of emotional processing/stress-coping pathways.

In the hippocampal formation, a key region involved in memory,7,32,37 we found specific changes of Fos expression in both afferent structures and outflow targets. First, stress of both types reduced activity in the dentate gyrus, considered a gateway to the hippocampus proper,11 suggesting that sensory signals to the hippocampus may be gated or filtered during either stress. In addition, both types of stress activated ventral pyramidal cells to a much larger degree than dorsal hippocampus counterparts. It is interesting to speculate about the function of hippocampal neuron activation by stress: like every experience, stressful experiences are accompanied by learning. It is conceivable that the type of learning evoked by the restraint vs multimodal stress might differ, as a larger number of hippocampal pyramidal cell-layer neurons were activated by the latter.

Here, we employed Fos mapping, a powerful tool for delineating stress-induced neural activation. There are known limitations to this technique.89,90,114 Most notably the fact that not all cell types express the gene product, and thus the absence of Fos does not necessarily mean a cell was not activated or involved in stress processing. Therefore, there has been a need to validate this method, and other immediate early gene products have been used to map stress responses; in many, but not all, of these studies there is considerable overlap in activated areas.90,114 In the context of hours-long stress, we have previously examined pCREB activation115 and found that although the distribution of activated cells was similar to that labeled by Fos, pCREB activation followed a rapid and transient time-course which is not optimal for the hours-long stress models employed here. More recently, the activity-regulated cytoskeletal-associated protein (Arc) has emerged as an alternative tool to examine activity-dependent transcription, with particular relevance for mechanisms of plasticity. However, Arc has a much more restricted pattern of expression than Fos in response to stress: Arc appears to be expressed primarily within telencephalic areas116 and is therefore problematic for mapping global network patterns of activation, including hypothalamic and midbrain structures. Arc might be an interesting future target of studies in the context of hippocampal plasticity following multimodal stress.

Multimodal and unimodal stress influence dorsal and ventral hippocampal connectivity differentially

Distinct functions and connectivity of ventral versus dorsal hippocampus are being increasingly recognized.9 Dorsal hippocampus in the rodent, and primarily CA1, has emerged as the principal location of memory encoding and processing, whereas ventral hippocampus contributes to a network involved in salience and emotionality.9,10,25,117 These ideas are in line with the differing connectivity of hippocampal subregions: dorsal hippocampus primarily projects to cortical structures, whereas outflow from ventral hippocampus targets subcortical regions including amygdala and hypothalamus.9,11,13 In addition, stress and glucocorticoids affect synaptic plasticity differentially along the hippocampal dorsoventral axis, impairing long-term potentiation in dorsal hippocampus, but enhancing this process in ventral hippocampus.15,49,118 The current results further support the distinct roles of dorsal and ventral hippocampus and indicate that multimodal stress may influence them selectively in a manner unique from that of restraint stress.

Specifically, restraint stress enhanced functional connectivity of hippocampus to septum and to the PVT. The dorsal hippocampus-septal network contributes to cognitive functions,119,120 and PVT is considered a ‘stress memory’ center.22 Thus, the restraint effects may reflect processes of memory designed to aid in coping with future stress. In contrast, multimodal stress reduced correlation of hippocampus and septum and increased significantly correlations with amygdala and BST. Amygdala-hippocampal networks, mediated primarily via ventral hippocampal pathways, have been strongly implicated in emotional responses to stress, including pathological anxiety and stress-related disorders.104,121 Hence, these changes may reflect reduced ability for objective memory and increased emotional salience of the stress.103 Previous animal studies have shown that stress can alter relative activation patterns within the limbic system,122 as well as modulate the nature of interactions among brain regions.123,124 Similar shifts in functional connectivity have also been observed in humans in response to emotionally salient stimuli.77,125

Overall, these data indicate that increased stress complexity causes a shift in network connectivity from cognitive processing of basic sensory input toward emotional and stress-coping circuits. Functional connectivity results from activity of synapses, and is hence modulated by the number of synapses and the robustness of their function.126,127 The current studies find both an apparent connectivity shift towards ventral hippocampus and a preferential loss of synapses in dorsal-hippocampus pyramidal cells, a fact that will lessen dorsal-hippocampus septum connections while maintaining ventral-hippocampus amygdala connectivity (Supplementary Figure 4). Although the precise causal relationships of these two phenomena are not fully clear at this point, both of these processes might contribute to the augmented memory impairment following short, multimodal stress.