Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Article
  • Published:

Rats maintain an overhead binocular field at the expense of constant fusion

Subjects

Abstract

Fusing left and right eye images into a single view is dependent on precise ocular alignment, which relies on coordinated eye movements. During movements of the head this alignment is maintained by numerous reflexes. Although rodents share with other mammals the key components of eye movement control, the coordination of eye movements in freely moving rodents is unknown. Here we show that movements of the two eyes in freely moving rats differ fundamentally from the precisely controlled eye movements used by other mammals to maintain continuous binocular fusion. The observed eye movements serve to keep the visual fields of the two eyes continuously overlapping above the animal during free movement, but not continuously aligned. Overhead visual stimuli presented to rats freely exploring an open arena evoke an immediate shelter-seeking behaviour, but are ineffective when presented beside the arena. We suggest that continuously overlapping visual fields overhead would be of evolutionary benefit for predator detection by minimizing blind spots.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Rent or buy this article

Prices vary by article type

from$1.95

to$39.95

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Figure 1: Eye movements in freely exploring rats.
Figure 2: Eye movements are dictated by head movement and position in freely moving animals.
Figure 3: Asymmetrical eye movements in freely moving rats.
Figure 4: Eye movements in freely moving animals are not consistent with those needed for binocular fusion.
Figure 5: Overhead binocular overlap.
Figure 6: Shapes moving overhead selectively evoke shelter-seeking behaviour.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Hughes, A. A schematic eye for the rat. Vision Res. 19, 569–588 (1979)

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  2. Legg, C. R. & Lambert, S. Distance estimation in the hooded rat: experimental evidence for the role of motion cues. Behav. Brain Res. 41, 11–20 (1990)

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  3. Berardi, N. & Maffei, L. From visual experience to visual function: roles of neurotrophins. J. Neurobiol. 41, 119–126 (1999)

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  4. Zoccolan, D., Oertelt, N., DiCarlo, J. J. & Cox, D. D. A rodent model for the study of invariant visual object recognition. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 106, 8748–8753 (2009)

    Article  ADS  CAS  Google Scholar 

  5. Chelazzi, L., Rossi, F., Tempia, F., Ghirardi, M. & Strata, P. Saccadic eye movements and gaze holding in the head-restrained pigmented rat. Eur. J. Neurosci. 1, 639–646 (1989)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Hess, B. J. & Dieringer, N. Spatial organization of the maculo-ocular reflex of the rat: responses during off-vertical axis rotation. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2, 909–919 (1990)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Quinn, K. J., Rude, S. A., Brettler, S. C. & Baker, J. F. Chronic recording of the vestibulo-ocular reflex in the restrained rat using a permanently implanted scleral search coil. J. Neurosci. Methods 80, 201–208 (1998)

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  8. Russell, J. T. Depth discrimination in the rat. Pedagog. Semin. J. Gen. Psychol. 40, 136–161 (1932)

    Google Scholar 

  9. Morris, P. Rats in the diet of the barn owl (Tyto alba). J. Zool. 189, 540–545 (1979)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Doncanster, C. P., Dickman, C. R. & Macdonald, D. W. Feeding ecology of red foxes (Vulpes vulpes) in the city of Oxford, England. J. Mamm. 71, 188–194 (1990)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Fuller, J. H. Eye and head movements in the pigmented rat. Vision Res. 25, 1121–1128 (1985)

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  12. Prusky, G. T., Silver, B. D., Tschetter, W. W., Alam, N. M. & Douglas, R. M. Experience-dependent plasticity from eye opening enables lasting, visual cortex-dependent enhancement of motion vision. J. Neurosci. 28, 9817–9827 (2008)

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  13. Euler, T. & Wassle, H. Immunocytochemical identification of cone bipolar cells in the rat retina. J. Comp. Neurol. 361, 461–478 (1995)

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  14. Hughes, A. in Handbook of Sensory Physiology Vol. VII (ed. Crescitelli, F. ) 613–756 (Springer, 1977)

    Google Scholar 

  15. Leigh, R. J. & Zee, D. S. The Neurology of Eye Movement 3rd edn (Oxford Univ. Press, 1999)

    Google Scholar 

  16. Duwaer, A. L. & van den Brink, G. What is the diplopia threshold? Percept. Psychophys. 29, 295–309 (1981)

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  17. Lyle, T. K. & Foley, J. Subnormal binocular vision with special reference to peripheral fusion. Br. J. Ophthalmol. 39, 474–487 (1955)

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  18. Dell’Osso, L. F. & Daroff, R. B. Two additional scenarios for see-saw nystagmus: achiasma and hemichiasma. J. Neuro-Ophthalmol. 18, 112–113 (1998)

    Google Scholar 

  19. Douglas, R. M. et al. Independent visual threshold measurements in the two eyes of freely moving rats and mice using a virtual-reality optokinetic system. Vis. Neurosci. 22, 677–684 (2005)

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  20. Hughes, A. The refractive state of the rat eye. Vision Res. 17, 927–939 (1977)

    Article  ADS  CAS  Google Scholar 

  21. Anjum, F., Turni, H., Mulder, P. G., van der Burg, J. & Brecht, M. Tactile guidance of prey capture in Etruscan shrews. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 103, 16544–16549 (2006)

    Article  ADS  CAS  Google Scholar 

  22. Wallace, D. G., Gorny, B. & Whishaw, I. Q. Rats can track odors, other rats, and themselves: implications for the study of spatial behavior. Behav. Brain Res. 131, 185–192 (2002)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Munz, M., Brecht, M. & Wolfe, J. Active touch during shrew prey capture. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 4, 191 (2010)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Artal, P., Herreros de Tejada, P., Munoz Tedo, C. & Green, D. G. Retinal image quality in the rodent eye. Vis. Neurosci. 15, 597–605 (1998)

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  25. Fernandez-Juricic, E. et al. Testing the terrain hypothesis: Canada geese see their world laterally and obliquely. Brain Behav. Evol. 77, 147–158 (2011)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Dawkins, M. S. What are birds looking at? Head movements and eye use in chickens. Anim. Behav. 63, 991–998 (2002)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Wurtz, R. H. Neuronal mechanisms of visual stability. Vision Res. 48, 2070–2089 (2008)

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank W. Denk, R. Hahnloser, K. Kirchfeld, K. Martin, A. Schwartz and F. Wolf for comments on earlier versions of this manuscript and M. Rictis for help with electronics fabrication. We also thank A. Benali, A. Brauer, U. Czubayko, M.-L. Silva, V. Pawlak, V. Ramachandra and T. Senkova from the Network Imaging Group for data processing, A. Schaefer and M. Köllö for loan of the spherical treadmill, and F. Wolf for advice on the modelling and discussions throughout the project. We thank N. Logothetis for support and C. Sakmann for insights. G.N.’s salary was financed by the German Federal Ministry of Education and Research (BMBF; FKZ: 01GQ1002). The Max Planck Society financed research. We apologize to all authors whom we have not been able to cite because of space restrictions.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Experimental design was by D.J.W., D.S.G., J.S. and J.N.D.K., hardware and software development by D.S.G. and J.S., data collection by D.J.W., D.S.G., J.S. and S.R., analysis design and implementation by D.J.W., D.S.G., G.N., S.R. and J.N.D.K., methods text by D.J.W., D.S.G., J.S. and G.N. and composition of the main text by D.J.W. and J.N.D.K.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jason N. D. Kerr.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

Supplementary information

Supplementary Information

This file contains Supplementary Text (see contents pages for details), Supplementary References and Supplementary Figures 1-14. (PDF 8348 kb)

Eye movements in a freely moving rat

Video-oculography of the right and left eyes of the animal (left) and kinetic traces of the recorded eye movements (right) for the same segment of data shown in Fig. 1b. In the kinetic traces, the horizontal position of the marker represents the horizontal position of the pupil, while the color of the marker represents the vertical position. The time axis points downwards. (MP4 1038 kb)

Example of ocular torsion in a freely moving rat

Video of the right eye of one animal, showing the tracked margin of the pupil (blue) and a reference line (red) to aid in visualization of the torsional rotation. (MP4 993 kb)

Eye movements in a head-restrained rat

Video-oculography of the right and left eyes (left) and kinetic traces of the recorded eye movements (right) for the same segment of data as shown in Fig. 1e. The data shown in this video and that in supplementary video 2 are from the same animal. In the kinetic traces, the horizontal position of the marker represents the horizontal position of the pupil, while the color of the marker represents the vertical position. The time axis points downwards. (MP4 455 kb)

Example of eye movements resulting from pitch and roll in of a head-restrained animal

Overhead view showing both eyes of a head-restrained animal, and the eye movements resulting from nose-up pitch, nose-down pitch, rightward roll and leftward roll (each rotation is made from 0 to 60 degrees, then returned to 0). (MP4 6728 kb)

Example showing the constantly changing ocular alignment occurring during free movement

Video-oculography images of the right and left eye (upper) and corresponding ocular alignment represented as gaze vector difference (lower plot, right eye gaze vector – left eye gaze vector) for a short image sequence. The un-yoked movements of the right and left eyes result in continuously changing ocular alignment. The tracked pupil positions in the upper images are represented by the solid white circles, and the tracked eye corner positions used for eliminating artifacts due to camera movement are shown as solid red circles. The image sequence is presented in slow motion, with relative time from the beginning of the sequence shown below the left eye image. (MP4 1001 kb)

“Rat’s eye view” reconstructed from data from one animal during a jump across the gap in the visually-based gap-crossing task

Image sequence showing the left eye (left) and right eye (right) fields of view of one animal as it jumped across the gap. Images were rendered using a spherical projection of the jumping track and surrounding environment, using head position as the optical center and the left or right gaze vector as the optical axis. The projection was based on the actual head and eye positions measured during the jump, and assumed a full field of view for each eye of 180˚. (MP4 1314 kb)

Behavioral responses to visual stimuli presented beside the animal or overhead

Video showing rat behavior in response to stimuli presented beside (on monitors located to the right side, behind and to the left of the arena) or overhead (monitor located above the arena). Stimuli presented beside the arena evoke no obvious behavioral responses, while stimuli presented overhead evoke a strong shelter-seeking response. (MP4 4748 kb)

Example of camera movement and eye corner tracking methodology

Video-oculography (upper images) and corresponding tracked pupil positions (lower plots) from an image sequence containing a gross movement of the right eye camera caused by the animal bumping the camera into the edge of the track. The tracked pupil positions in the upper images are represented by the solid white circles, and the tracked eye corner positions used for eliminating artifacts due to camera movement are shown as solid red circles. The image sequence is presented in slow motion, with relative time from the beginning of the sequence shown to the right of the left eye image. The camera movement in the right eye image occurs at t = 0.310 s. Note that the large camera movement is eliminated from the tracked position of the right pupil. (MP4 416 kb)

PowerPoint slides

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Wallace, D., Greenberg, D., Sawinski, J. et al. Rats maintain an overhead binocular field at the expense of constant fusion. Nature 498, 65–69 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1038/nature12153

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/nature12153

This article is cited by

Comments

By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing