Introduction

Prevalence of the fragile X gene (FMR1) premutation is ~1:150 in females and ~1:450 in males of the general population1. A portion of older FMR1 premutation carriers (~40% of males, ~16% of females) develop fragile X-associated tremor/ataxia syndrome (FXTAS), with intention tremor, cerebellar ataxia, polyneuropathy and cognitive deficits—particularly executive dysfunction—as the primary symptoms2. Currently, medical intervention for FXTAS is limited to symptom management2,3 (Hagerman et al. 2013).

Pronounced cognitive deficits in executive functioning, processing speed and working memory are common in FXTAS4,5. Attention deficits modulated by FMR1 premutation are frequently self-reported by premutation carriers6. Moreover, the premutation has been associated with Autism and ADHD7,8. Inhibition and working memory are also impaired in carriers asymptomatic of FXTAS9,10. Radiological and neuropathological studies in FXTAS revealed abnormalities in prefrontal and fronto-parietal regions crucial for top-down attentional control and working memory11,12,13.

Cellular neuropathological studies have demonstrated abnormal neuronal response to glutamate in the FMR1 premutation14,15. In human induced pluripotent stem cell (iPSC)-derived neurons carrying the premutation, Liu and colleagues documented an increased response to glutamate and higher amplitude and more frequent calcium spiking activity16.

Memantine, an uncompetitive NMDA glutamate receptor antagonist approved for the treatment of moderate to severe Alzheimer’s disease17, has thus been tested in the first randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled clinical trial conducted in FXTAS. The main analyses showed no significant treatment effects on the primary outcome measures of intention tremor and executive dysfunction18. However, in a substudy utilizing cognitive event-related potentials (ERPs), memantine-associated benefits on both cued-recall memory and N400 repetition effect (an electrophysiological marker of semantic priming and verbal memory processes) were demonstrated19, indicating that ERPs offered a more sensitive measure of changes in cognitive processes compared to standard behavioral and neuropsychological tests. This notion is consistent with the suggestions that ERPs can provide a non-invasive and reliable approach to studying the effects of pharmacological interventions on neural processes20,21. In the present study, effects of memantine treatment on attentional processes that are fundamental to executive function/dysfunction, the cardinal cognitive deficit in FXTAS, were investigated using ERPs obtained from an auditory “oddball” task, an extensively-studied paradigm engaging attentional processes22,23. In the auditory oddball paradigm utilized in the current experiment, patients were instructed to detect the infrequent “oddball” tone embedded in a train of non-target standard tones. Subjects were instructed to press a button to each target detected and also keep a mental count of the number of targets in that experimental block.

Our prior studies in premutation carriers using the same “oddball” paradigm have demonstrated an altered frontal P300 (P3) ERP component in FXTAS patients9,24, which tracks their executive dysfunction. The earlier abnormalities of prolonged N100 latency and reduced P200 (P2) amplitude were also found in a predominately male FXTAS group24 but not in female premutation carriers asymptomatic of FXTAS9. Based on both the NMDA modulation effects on auditory ERPs in animals25,26,27,28 and humans29,30,31,32 and our prior findings in FXTAS9,24, we hypothesized that 1-year of memantine treatment would improve attentional abilities as indexed by P2 and P3 measures in the present study.

Results

Table 1 summarizes the group characteristics at baseline for 24 patients from each group (i.e., memantine and placebo) analyzed in the present study.

Table 1 Group characteristics at Baseline: Mean ± SD.

Behavioral performance

The response time (RT) and accuracy data for target detection at two visits and the change over one year (1-year minus baseline) are summarized in Table 2, with no statistically significant differences found for the RT or target detection accuracy between two groups (p’s ≥ 0.26). ANOVA of d′, the sensitivity for target detection calculated using signal detection theory (d′ = normalized hit rate minus false alarm rate33), showed an insignificant trend of treatment × visit interaction (p = 0.13), with an increase in the memantine group, but a slight decrease in the placebo group. The marginially enhanced sensitivity for target detection as reflected by the d′ suggests a beneficial effect from the memantine treatment.

Table 2 Behavioral performance during auditory “oddball” task: Mean ± SD.

There was no group difference on the |count-hit| discrepancies (a behavioral attention/working memory measure obtained from the dual-response oddball task, contrasting the total correct button-presses and the reported mental count of targets) at either the baseline or follow-up visit (p = 0.79), however, ANOVA revealed a significant visit × treatment interaction (F1,46 = 8.64, p = 0.005, effect size η2 = 0.16). Follow-up within-group paired-samples t-tests showed that the |count-hit| discrepancy significantly decreased after 1-year treatment for the memantine group (t23 = 3.0, p = 0.006), but not for the placebo group which showed an insignificant increase of the |count-hit| discrepancy (t23 = −1.32, p = 0.20). This result reflects improved attention and/or working memory after treatment with memantine.

ERP Results

Figures 1 and 2 depict the grand average ERPs to standard tones at the vertex (Cz) and the ERPs to target tones at 3 midline electrodes (i.e., Fz, Cz and Pz), respectively. In repeated-measures ANOVAs of the P2 data, a significant visit × treatment interaction was found for P2 amplitude (F1,44 = 4.26, p = 0.045, η2 = 0.88). Post-hoc analyses found significantly larger increases of the composite P2 amplitudes (averaged from the fronto-central cluster of 4 electrodes Fz, Cz and FC1/2) in the memantine group compared to the placebo group (1-year – baseline: memantine group = 0.79 ± 1.3 μV, placebo group = −0.01 ± 1.2 μV; t46 = 2.19, p = 0.03). Post-hoc within-group paired-samples t-tests showed that the P2 amplitude was significantly enhanced after 1-year intervention for the memantine group (t23 = −2.88, p = 0.008), but not for the placebo group (t23 = 0.05, p = 0.96). No significant treatment effects were found for other ERP measures analyzed.

Figure 1
figure 1

ERPs to standard tones.

(A) ERPs to standards at the vertex (Cz). (B) Topographic maps of P2 amplitude to standard tones, 160–260 ms post-stimulus.

Figure 2
figure 2

ERPs to targets.

ERPs to targets at 3 midline electrodes (left panel) and topographic maps to targets across the N2 (upper) and P3 (lower) time windows (right panel).

The ANOVA of P2 habituation effect (i.e., smaller response to later stimuli) found a significant treatment × visit × trial position interaction (F1, 46 = 4.17, p = 0.049). Paired-samples t-tests revealed larger P2 amplitude in response to the first 30 standard tones at the 1-year follow-up than at the baseline in the treated group (Fig. 3, mean increase across 4 electrodes: 1-year − baseline = 1.53 μV, t23 = −3.56, p = 0.002), but not in the placebo group (mean decrease: 1-year − baseline = −0.02 μV, t23 = −0.63, p = 0.54). Furthermore, FXTAS patients who received the memantine treatment exhibited a normalized trend of habituation at the 1-year follow-up (1st 30 tones − last 30 tones = 0.82 μV) comparable to that in a group of 16 age-matched normal controls (0.51 μV). In contrast, FXTAS patients without an active memantine treatment (i.e., the placebo group as well as the treated group at baseline prior to the start of memantine) showed an insignificant trend of a reduced P2 amplitude as well as a smaller and inversed P2 habituation effect compared to normal controls: P2 raw amplitude (averaged across the 4 electrodes) in FXTAS = 0.89 μV, controls = 1.65 μV (t = 1.69, p = 0.095); P2 habituation effect in FXTAS = −0.82 μV, NC = 0.51 μV; t = 1.78, p = 0.08), indicating that the increased P2 amplitude and habituation effect in FXTAS after memantine treatment reflect beneficial changes towards normalized brain responses.

Figure 3
figure 3

P2 amplitudes (μV) to the first 30 standard tones and the last 30 standard tones within the first block of each EEG study, showing a reduced amplitude of responses to the later stimuli (i.e., a habituation effect) in the memantine-treated FXTAS group (solid green) and a normal control group (blue), but increased amplitudes to later stimuli in the other groups.

(**p = 0.002).

Correlation Results

Correlations between CGG repeat length and the P2 measures were tested using linear regression. Across all patients, CGG repeat length was inversely associated with the composite P2 peak amplitude at the baseline visit (B = −0.046, r = −0.39, p = 0.014), suggesting a link between the FMR1 premutation and P2.

Across all participants, a significant correlation (B = −0.178, r = −0.32, p = 0.025) was observed between the 1-year increase in the composite P2 amplitude (1-year − baseline) and the improvement (decrease from baseline to 1-year) on the behavioral measure of attention/working memory during the oddball task (i.e., the |count-hit| discrepancy). A similar trend of correlation was found within the memantine group (B = −0.231, r = −0.35, p = 0.09), but not in the placebo group (B = −0.60, r = −0.12, p = 0.56).

Discussion

No targeted treatment has yet been proven effective for FXTAS, a progressive adult-onset neurodegenerative disorder affecting many older FMR1 premutation carriers. The first report made based upon the larger cohort from which the current study was drawn18 concluded that memantine, an NMDA glutamate uncompetitive antagonist, showed no significant effects in FXTAS on the primary clinical and neuropsychological outcome measures of intention tremor severity and executive function. However, herein we have reported that patients with FXTAS administered memantine for one year showed enhanced neural correlates of attentional processes at the stimulus identification/discrimination stage (i.e., the P2 ERP component obtained from an auditory oddball experiment). Increases in P2 amplitude correlated with improved behavioral performance on an attention/working memory measure obtained from the oddball task (i.e., the |count-hit| discrepancy). These improvements were not observed among the placebo group, which exhibited a slight decrement in attention/working memory performance after one year. Thus, we interpret these results as showing that memantine achieved “target engagement” and the P2 can provide a “surrogate marker”21,34 for treatment response with improvements in cognitive domains of attention and working memory. However, one should also note that the cognitive effects of memantine were modest, highly significant for count-hit discrepancy, with insignificant trends towards improvement on response time (RT) and target detection accuracy. Additionally, across all patients, the P2 amplitude at baseline was negatively correlated with CGG repeat length. Although we also expected a treatment effect on the P3 measures, the results did not provide support for it. Our prior studies in FMR1 premutation carriers demonstrated reduced P2 amplitude and an elevated |count-hit| discrepancy in a predominately male FXTAS group24, as well as a higher than normal |count-hit| discrepancy in older female FMR1 premutation carriers without FXTAS9. The findings of the present study indicate that 1-year of chronic treatment with memantine might reverse some of the attentional processing deficits in the premutation carriers with FXTAS.

The precise nature of the auditory P2 is not fully understood, but it is generally agreed that the P2 is sensitive to processes beyond sensation and that attention has significant modulatory effects on this component35,36. P2 sensory gating has been related to brain mechanisms that filter potentially interfering stimuli in attention allocation. Studies using selective attention tasks suggested that the P2 reflects inhibitory control of automatic access to distractors37, or allocation of marginal attentional resources needed for non-target stimuli to be identified and rejected38,39. Enhanced P2 amplitudes after training have been linked to increased distractor inhibition40,41. Consistent with these prior studies, the memantine-related P2 amplitude increase in the current study likely represents improved distractor inhibition and/or similar processes at an earlier stage of stimulus identification/discrimination. The improved P2-indexed activities subsequently benefited the attentional processing of targets, resulting in treatment-related gains in attention/working memory performance as measured by the reduced |count-hit| discrepancy, the trend of better sensitivity (d′) for target detection and normalized habituation of P2 amplitude. The improvements in these behavioral and neural measures, as well as the positive correlation between changes in P2 amplitude and |count-hit| discrepancy during the oddball task, indicate that the memantine-associated changes obtained in the current study represent beneficial treatment effects for FXTAS. Thus, our present findings support the hypothesis that 1-year of memantine treatment has positive effects on some fundamental components of executive function/dysfunction42,43, which are thought to comprise the core cognitive deficits in FXTAS5.

Unlike most ERP components, P2 amplitude has been found to increase with normal aging, a phenomenon interpreted as an inappropriate orienting response to irrelevant stimuli and a deficient capacity to withdraw attentional resources from irrelevant stimuli36,39 (but also see Polich44 for a lack of age effect on P2). The aging-related amplitude increase might have cancelled the FXTAS-associated decrease24 and explains that no P2 amplitude reduction was found for the carriers on placebo for one year.

The primary generators of the auditory P2 have been localized in auditory cortex including the lateral Heschl’s gyrus36. It is also thought that the thalamo-midbrain reticular activating system and insula are important for P2 generation45,46. Notably, radiological studies in FXTAS have found alterations in several putative P2 generators including insula, superior temporal lobe and reticular activating system structures (e.g., thalamus and cerebral peduncle)47,48. Thus, it is plausible that the NMDA antagonist memantine ameliorates neuronal abnormalities in one or several of the putative P2 generators that have a high density of NMDA receptors, such as the reticular activating system.

The middle and late-latency cortical ERPs in rodents have shorter latencies but very similar morphology and at least partially shared functional significance when compared to human auditory ERP components27. The finding of a specific effect of memantine on the P2 among several auditory ERP components is in line with a number of prior pharmacologic studies primarily in mice, which have implicated glutamate as having a central role in modulations of auditory P2. For instance, early postnatal treatment of mice with NMDA antagonist phencyclidine (PCP) has been shown to significantly disrupt auditory gating of the P2, but not N1 or P1 amplitude49. Depending on the specific type and dosage of the NMDA drug administered, P2 amplitude can be either enhanced or attenuated. Tikhonravov and colleagues25 have found that low-dose memantine increased the P2 and N2 amplitudes to deviants. The NMDA antagonist ketamine has been shown to reduce the auditory P2 amplitude50. NMDA receptor subunit NR1 knock-out mice demonstrated reduced NMDA signaling, as well as a surprising absence of the auditory P251. Thus, along with the recent report showing effects of memantine on cued memory retrieval and the N400 ERP repetition effect19, our studies support the hypothesis that memantine might reduce FMR1 premutation-associated abnormalities in glutamatergic signalling and improve cognition in patients with FXTAS.

In summary, our ERP and behavioral data suggest that memantine treatment has a beneficial effect primarily on cognitive abilities, rather than on the more obvious and characteristic motor abnormalities in FXTAS, although occasional improvements have been noticed on motor symptoms with memantine treatment of some patients with FXTAS. For example, one case study reported that memantine ameliorated FXTAS symptoms including tremor, ataxia, neuropathy, depression and anxiety when combined with venlafaxine in a 65-year old female52. Our ERP results demonstrate evidence of target engagement for cognitive function and may warrant further investigations into the effects of memantine in larger FXTAS samples. It would also be of interest to test whether the improvement in P2 amplitude associated with memantine treatment persists after washing-out the drug, a finding which would support a neuroprotective effect.

One should note that our findings of subtle beneficial effects on cognition may not be strong enough to justify the clinical use of memantine in patients with FXTAS, especially without independent replication. A limitation of this ERP substudy is that we analysed ERP data from study completers with technically adequate data at both timepoints. A potential selection bias is thus introduced, favoring patients who may have had more benign clinical courses. However, it should be noted that the proportion of patients with adequate longitudinal ERP data was identical in both treatment arms and most of the reasons for not completing had very similar prevalences in both groups. Thus, like Seritan et al.18, we detected no systematic difference in drop-out rates between groups and note that memantine was well tolerated. It also remains uncertain whether chronic memantine treatment at earlier stages of FXTAS might affect disease onset or progression, or whether extended treatment will show continued improvement of cognitive function, or eventually, quality of life. However, considering the lack of any proven therapy to date, the cognitive effects of memantine in FXTAS, as revealed by our ERP measures, provide important and therapeutically relevant information that could further the development of specific cognitive or disease-modifying therapies for patients with FXTAS and the premutation carriers affected by other disorders such as fragile X-associated primary ovarian insufficiency.

In addition to its effects on NMDA receptors, memantine also binds to dopamine D2 receptors53. Since atypical motor parkinsonism is very common in FXTAS2,54, it is plausible that some of the cognitive effects we observed could be attributed to memantine’s dopaminergic agonist properties. For example, a MEG study55 has demonstrated that dopamine antagonists interfere with involuntary attention shifting and both prolong and diminish the P3a, a positive component whose latency window is close to the P2. Studies of memantine in several neuropsychiatric disorders other than dementia have found positive and promising results (e.g., overall improvements in ADHD in a preliminary open-label study and mood-stabilizing effects in bipolar disorder patients not responsive to standard treatments)56,57. Therefore, the effects of memantine on psychiatric symptoms in FXTAS should be examined more closely. Furthermore, clinical trials testing additive or synergistic effects using memantine in combination with other therapeutic agents is another area which may be worthy of further exploration.

Patients and Methods

Participants

Participants included patients with FXTAS enrolled in a comprehensive research study at the M.I.N.D. Institute, University of California Davis, between 2007 and 201218. The study was carried out in accordance with the approved guidelines/protocols. All the protocols were approved by the University of California Davis Institutional Review Board. Informed written consent was obtained from all subjects prior to their participation. All the evaluations were performed at the M.I.N.D Institute in Sacramento, California, except for the ERP experiments conducted at the Center for Mind and Brain in Davis, California. Further details about the primary clinical trial can be seen in the published report18.

From the 88 patients who started the allocated intervention (43 on memantine, 45 on placebo)18, 76 (38 in each arm) participated in the ERP substudy. Eleven (11) participants performed the baseline ERP experiment but discontinued the intervention trial because of illness or similar reasons (5 on memantine, 6 on placebo; detailed reasons reported in Seritan et al.18) and therefore did not have longitudinal ERP data. There were 9 additional subjects who did not complete the follow-up ERP studies (5 on memantine, 4 on placebo) because of transportation issues or inability to schedule an ERP session when the lab was available (participants were from all over the United States and each went through intensive testing and travel generally over a 3-day visit). Therefore, 56 patients (28 in each arm) completed the auditory oddball ERP experiment at both visits (i.e., at the baseline visit right before the intervention and at the follow-up after staying in the trial for one year). In the memantine group, 4 participants were excluded from further analyses because of either unusable EEG data with excessive artifacts (described below, n = 3) or extended gap between two visits (n = 1; interval = 715 days, compared to the group mean of 356 days). In the placebo group, 4 patients were excluded due to unusable EEG data with excessive artifacts (n = 3) or having a deep-brain stimulator implanted (n = 1). Thus, data from 24 patients from each group were analyzed in the present study (Table 1).

Intervention

Tablets identical in appearance containing either 5 mg of memantine or placebo were provided by Forest Pharmaceuticals. Titration started with 5 mg/day by mouth for one week and dosage was increased by 5 mg each week, until the full maintenance dose of 10 mg twice daily (i.e., 20 mg/day) was achieved by day 22. Participants were instructed to keep all other medications unchanged for the duration of the study.

EEG/ERP data collection

EEG during a two-stimulus auditory oddball experiment was recorded in a sound-attenuated, dimly-lit chamber. Lower (113 Hz) and higher (200 Hz) frequency pure tones were presented at 40 dB above individual hearing level in 4 blocks, each containing 100 tones, with a stimulus onset asynchrony jittered from 1.0–1.5 seconds. Prior to each block, subjects were instructed to respond to the infrequent (probability = 25%) “oddball” tones (high or low target tones, counterbalanced across blocks). A dual task was employed in which subjects were instructed to press a button to each target tone and to also keep a mental count of the number of targets in each block. The mental count of target tones was reported immediately following completion after each block. 32-channel EEG was recorded with a Nicolet-SM-2000 amplifier (band-pass = 0.016–100 Hz, sampled at 250 Hz). (See Olichney et al.58 for more details of the EEG montage used).

Data Analysis

The |count-hit| discrepancy in each block (i.e., the absolute value of the difference between correct button-presses and mental count to target tones within a block) was calculated for each participant, as an inverse measure (i.e., a lower value represents better performance) of attention/working memory performance during the oddball task9,24.

Event-locked EEG segments contaminated with blinks, eye movements, excessive muscle activity, or amplifier blocking were rejected using a semi-automated computer algorithm. Artifact-free EEG segments of 1024 ms (with a 100 ms pre-stimulus baseline period and 924 ms post-stimulus onset) were averaged by experimental condition to obtain the ERPs.

Based on our prior findings using the same paradigm9,24, mean amplitude and local peak latency of 4 ERP components were quantified in the following time windows: N100 (N1, 70–150 ms), P2 (160–260 ms), N200 (N2, 170–300 ms) and P3 (300–650 ms). The waveforms to both target and standard tones were used to measure N1. The P2 was measured from ERPs to standard tones. The N2 component was defined from the difference wave (ERPs to targets minus standards). The P3 was measured from both the difference wave and the ERP waveform to targets.

ERP measures were submitted to repeated-measures ANOVAs (SPSS 22, IBM) with the between-subjects factor of treatment and the within-subjects factors of visit and electrode. Analyses of N1 and P2 included 4 fronto-central electrodes (Fz, Cz, FC1/2). Five central channels (Cz, FC1/2, CP1/2) were used for the N2 analyses. P3 analyses were carried out with 26 scalp electrodes (all except FP1/2). The Greenhouse-Geiser correction was used to adjust for violations of sphericity, where appropriate.

To further characterize the modulatory effects of memantine on the P2 component, a habituation analysis was conducted for P2 amplitude. P2 mean amplitude in response to the first 30 standard tones was compared to the amplitude of response to the last 30 standard tones within the first block of each study, with the between-subjects factor of treatment and the within-subjects factors of visit, trial position, and electrode. Data from a group of 16 age-matched normal controls (mean age = 59.7 years, each of whom only underwent one ERP recording) was used to demonstrate the normal habituation effect.

Linear regression was used to examine the correlations between changes (1-year follow-up minus baseline) in the |count-hit| discrepancy and in ERP measures for which significant treatment effects were shown. Correlations between local peak amplitudes of P2 (measured after application of a 30 Hz low-pass filter) and CGG repeats were also tested.

Additional Information

How to cite this article: Yang, J.-C. et al. Memantine Improves Attentional Processes in Fragile X-Associated Tremor/Ataxia Syndrome: Electrophysiological Evidence from a Randomized Controlled Trial. Sci. Rep. 6, 21719; doi: 10.1038/srep21719 (2016).