Introduction

Do animals see the world as we see it? In the last thirty years, neuroscientists have made amazing progress in understanding the functioning of our visual system. One of the most important findings is that some features of the visual system, for example, the molecular machinery that underlies visual phototransduction, are extraordinarily similar in organisms ranging from jellyfish to humans1. Neurobiologists have found that, in some cases, even the neural circuitry that processes visual information has similar organizational principles, as shown by classical investigations of the visual cortex of humans, cats and macaques2,3.

Our visual system, however, does much more than simply reproducing the outside world: it is an active process that recreates a peculiar reality for each brain. The two-dimensional image projected on our retina is decomposed into distinct streams (e.g., orientation, shapes, color and motion features) on which the visual areas build a plausible three-dimension reconstruction of the visual environment. Our perception of a visual stimulus can, therefore, differ considerably from its physical counterpart. For this reason, even the most striking similarity between the visual systems does not guarantee that an animal perceives a visual scene as we do. In this respect, visual illusions can be an open window into how an animal perceives the world, a powerful non-invasive tool that complements neurobiological investigation4. Research on illusions has shown, for example, that the visual systems of mammals, birds and insects follows similar principles for the segregation of objects from the background5,6.

Motion illusion is a type of visual illusion in which we perceive motion that is different (in direction, strength, etc.7,8,9,10,11,12) from that present in the physical stimulus; or, even more strikingly, the perception of motion is induced by a completely static pattern13,14,15. As regards this latter family of stimuli, neurobiological and behavioral studies showed that both fixational eye movements16,17,18 and cortical processing19,20,21 are responsible for the perception of motion, but some outstanding questions remain unsolved. In particular, there is considerable debate among investigators as to whether gaze instability is strictly necessary to the illusory motion perception or, rather, if fixational eye movements only improve the strength of the illusion. Interestingly, not all subjects report perceiving illusory motion22,23. The origin of this polymorphic response is not entirely clear, but Fraser and Wilcox22 reported a significant parent-offspring correlation in the response to a motion illusion indicating either a genetic causation or a common environment effect. A subsequent analysis based on monozygotic-dizygotic twin comparison supported the strong genetic nature of this polymorphism. Recently Gori et al.24 showed that a specific genetic variance significantly reduce the ability of perceive illusory motion.

The literature reported motion after effects (small misperceptions of motion induced by prior exposure to opposite direction motion) or illusory reversal in motion direction due to flicker in different animals (mammals, birds and insects)25,26,27,28, but these effects can also be explained in humans without complex cortical elaboration. On the contrary, the global illusory motion experienced, for example, in the Rotating Snakes illusion (RSI)13 is a very different effect. This complex and large illusory motion may share the name “illusory motion” with other small effects, but it is, indeed, different in quality, quantity and, more importantly, in the brain structures that seemed to be necessary to perceive it. Surprisingly, despite long-standing interest on global motion illusion by psychologists, physiologists and neuroscientists there have been no studies reporting in non-human animals an illusion able to produce global motion induced by static patterns. A partial exception is a study of Conway et al.20 reporting that direction-selective neurons in the macaque visual cortex gave directional responses when monkeys saw a static sequence of colors characterizing the RSI (Fig 1a) that induces motion perception in humans. However, this study did not show if the recorded neuronal response reaches the conscious level because the monkeys did not perform a behavioral task of motion discrimination. Due to the extreme similarity in many aspects of vision, it would not be unexpected to discover that non-human animals, which present several stages of cortical elaboration of visual motion input, may perceive illusory motion as humans. Less obvious is to predict whether lower vertebrates, such as fish, that completely lack cortical elaboration would respond to such a complex motion illusion.

Figure 1
figure 1

The nine-circle version of the Rotating Snakes Illusion (RSI) (a) used in the test phase and its static control stimulus (b).

Those stimuli were created by the authors of this article based on the original pattern by Kitaoka and Ashida13.

Even though fish have no cortex, they need to accurately process motion stimuli to hunt, escape predators or swim in a shoal as the accurate processing of motion information could mean the difference between life and death. On the other hand, research has shown that fish can perceive first-order (luminance-defined or Fourier) motion as well as second-order (non-Fourier) motion29. The Fourier motion is a motion characterized by a change in luminance recorded on the retina. The non-Fourier motion is defined as a motion in which the moving contour is characterized by the change in some aspects (e.g., contrast, texture, or flicker) that do not result in differences in luminance. The ability to perceive non-Fourier motion is usually attributed to higher-level processing in the primate visual cortex29. Second-order motion perceived by the fish suggests that very efficient motion processing is already present in the retina29. These results, therefore, show that lower levels in the visual pathway already processed complex types of motion before the cortex appearance in mammals29. The challenge here is to discover if motion illusions like the global rotation we perceive in the static pattern of the RSI can also be processed without a cortex.

To address these issues, in the present study we investigated the response to the RSI in two species of teleost fishes. The RSI is one of the most celebrated phenomena that show global illusory motion in a static pattern. It is a powerful variation of Fraser and Wilcox's pattern22 and is characterized by the repetition of the following luminance sequence: black-dark gray-white-light gray-black, in a circular configuration that creates a vivid impression of slow rotation. Using computer displays, individual guppies (Poecilia reticulata) and zebrafish (Danio rerio) were presented with pairs of identical geometrical figures, one static and one physically rotating and were trained to select the moving target in order to get a food reward. In the test phase, fish underwent non-rewarded probe trials in which they were presented with new pairs of stimuli, a RSI stimulus (Figure 1a) and a control stimulus that was identical to the illusory figure except that the luminance relationship was flipped between adjacent local texture elements (Figure 1b ) so that the pattern does not induce illusory motion in humans16. In this situation, an animal perceiving a global rotation in the RSI stimulus is expected to select it rather than the counterpart that does not induce the motion illusion.

Figure 2
figure 2

Experimental apparatus.

Fish were first trained with a food reward to discriminate between moving and non-moving targets displayed on two computer screens and hence tested in extinction with RSI stimulus and its static counterpart.

Results

Zebrafish

In the training phase, zebrafish showed a significant preference for the moving stimuli (one sample t-test, t(11) = 7.27, p < 0.001). In the test phase, during which they were subjected to a series of non-rewarded probe trials (extinction trials) with the RSI and the control stimulus, zebrafish showed a clear choice of RSI compared with the control figure (one sample t-test, t(11) = 8.31, p < 0.001) (Figure 3). For 10 out of 12 fish (83%), the performance did not differ between illusory and real motion. For the remaining two fish, real motion was significantly more effective as illusory motion. Accuracy did not differ with respect to the two variants of RSI: four circle RSI (mean ± SD: 0.61 ± 0.06), nine circle RSI (0.61 ± 0.07); paired t-test, t(11) = 0.18, p = 0.853.

Figure 3
figure 3

Proportion of time spent by the fish (N = 12 for each species) near the motion stimuli (training phase, 4 probe trials) and the RSI (test phase,12 probe trials).

* = significant departure from chance level (0.5), p < 0.05.

Guppy

Similar results were observed in guppies. In the training phase, guppies showed a significant preference for the moving stimuli (one sample t-test, t(11) = 7.90, p < 0.001). In the test phase, they showed a significant choice of RSI compared with the control figure (one sample t-test, t(11) = 2.24, p = 0.046) (Figure 3). For 8 out of 12 fish (67%), illusory motion was equally effective as real motion. Real motion was significantly more effective for the four remaining fish.

Fish performance did not differ with respect to the two variants of RSI: four-circle RSI (mean ± SD: 0.54 ± 0.29), nine-circle RSI (0.62 ± 0.15); paired t-test, t(11) = 0.719, p = 0.487.

No difference in the choice of RSI stimulus was found between the two species (independent t-test, t(22) = 0.30, p = 0.767). See Table 1 to see the individual performance of each subject.

Table 1 Individual performance in the time spent near the moving target (training) and the Rotating Snake Illusion (test). The performance of 10 out of 12 zebrafish (83%) and that of 8 out of 12 guppies (67%) did not significantly differ between training and test phase

Discussion

Our results demonstrated, for the first time, that a non-human species does perceive global rotary illusory motion that is elicited by a static pattern. Although fish diverged from land vertebrates approximately 450 million years ago and their visual system evolved in a very different ecological context, they are fooled by the RSI in a qualitative fashion that appears to be consistent with humans' perception of the same static figure.

In humans, two main physiological mechanisms seem to underlie the perception of motion observed in the RSI. Some studies reported convincing results supporting the role of fixational eye movements in triggering motion illusion16,17,18, while others stressed that the illusory effect originated in the visual cortex. Conway et al.20 reported that the differences in response latency to different contrast elements are responsible for the illusion and provided the first evidence that pairs of stimuli with the luminance sequence that characterizes the RSI can generate motion signals in the primary visual cortex (V1) and MT direction-selective (DS) neurons. The two fMRI studies that directly investigated the RSI30,31 recorded an increase in neural activity in the motion-sensitive area of the human visual cortex during the observation of the RSI, suggesting that local motion signals in response to asymmetric spatial patterns at the level of V1 are integrated in the motion-sensitive cortical area (V5-MT complex) that creates the vivid rotatory motion perception. Global rotation is a peculiar type of motion that requires specifically V5-MT activity, no matter whether it is real32 or illusory21. The V5-MT complex possess neurons with large receptive fields that are able to integrate, into a global motion, the local motion signals processed by the small receptive fields of the DS neurons populating V1.

In sum, both neurobiological and behavioral studies suggest a critical role of the visual cortex in the perception of motion in the RSI. Fish show eye movements that, together with head and body movements, probably produce motion signals on their retina that are comparable to those experienced in humans during eye movements33,34 but clearly do not have a cortex and visual signals are processed by the optic tectum (OT). Therefore, different processes should be responsible for RSI in these organisms.

The motion percept is possibly elicited by stimulus processing in DS visual neurons in fish, too. These units respond optimally to stimulus motion in one (preferred) direction, but not in the opposite direction. DS cells were first described in the OT of the goldfish35 and also rabbits possess DS cells in their retina36. The DS ganglion cells (GC) in the retina are pure ON or OFF type and are characterized by small receptive fields. DS GC project to the OT. The DS neurons of the OT possess large receptive fields and collect signals from the retinal DS units of both ON and OFF subtypes with the same preferred direction37. One may argue that the fundamental mechanism that governs motion processing may have appeared early in vertebrate evolution and may still characterize today's fish. With appearance of the neocortex in the mammalian lineage, these mechanisms might have been co-opted by the visual cortical areas. A recent study, however, suggests that these old systems may still be in place, showing that these alternative pathways become apparent when the visual cortex is significantly damaged38. Blindsight patients, which are characterized by damage of the primary visual areas, were found to be able to perceive apparent motion by seemingly relying on a retinocollicular pathway, which bypasses V1 and directly reaches the MT/V5 areas38. Studying animals that have no cortex but exhibit perceptual behavior similar to human observers could be very useful to investigate mechanisms that were conserved in humans but are normally masked by the main modality of visual motion processing through the retino-geniculate-striate pathway.

In humans, across all ages, only 84% of the observers reported perceiving the RSI and 75% reported perceived motion in the previous pattern devised by Fraser and Wilcox22,23. It is interesting to note that comparable percentages of subjects were found here to perceive illusory motion in both guppies and zebrafish. In humans, this trait is highly heritable22,24; it will be interesting to see if this polymorphism has a genetic basis in fish as well. A genetic contribution to individual differences in post-retinal neural processing has been shown for several other aspects of vision, including binocular rivalry39 and flicker fusion thresholds40 and contrast sensitivity41 variations in visual processing (perception and attention) have been associated with developmental disorders such as dyslexia and autism spectrum disorders42,43,44,45,46,47,48. Both guppies and zebrafish have been widely used for behavioral genetic studies. The zebrafish genome was one of the first to be sequenced; resources are available for the identification of gene function49 and the genome of the guppy is currently being sequenced50. Numerous tools are available, especially for the former species, including several mutations that involve defects in the visual system51. The discovery that fish respond to motion illusions adds to other evidence that fish and humans share many features of the visual system and makes these organisms an ideal model to study the neurobiology of motion perception.

From a phylogenetic point of view, guppies are distantly related to zebrafish. According to recent estimates52, zebrafish and guppies belong to clades (Ostariophysi and Acanthopterygii, respectively), which diverged more than 250 million years ago. This suggests that the RSI perception might be a general feature of a teleost visual system. Comparative data from other vertebrate and invertebrate classes are now needed in order to determine whether this is a convergent trait that evolved independently in fish and primate lineages or whether all vertebrates and even some invertebrates, share the same basic processes of motion perception.

In conclusion, our evidence demonstrates that the visual system of fish is clearly equipped with neuronal mechanisms capable of mediating RSI and challenges the idea that higher-level, cortical mechanisms are strictly necessary to elicit illusory motion. It seems plausible that the large receptive fields of the ON/OFF DS visual neurons, populating the OT, could serve as the neural substrate to process global illusory motion in the absence of a cortex. However, our results do not necessarily contradict the literature showing that humans need a cortex to perceive global illusory motion. Rather, we suggest that our findings and those from blindsight patients38 agree in indicating that, as shown for numerous other visual functions, the general principles that underlie motion perception appeared early in vertebrate evolution and have been conserved in the mammalian lineage even after many visual functions became localized in the neocortex. If confirmed, this would open up the possibility of developing useful animal models to study the genetic and neural bases of motion perception.

Methods

Subjects

Two species of teleosts were used in this study: zebrafish (Danio rerio), a leading model organism for studies in neurobiology of vision and neurodevelopmental genetics51 and guppy (Poecilia reticulata), a leading model for behavioral studies in fish53. Twelve adult female zebrafish and twelve adult female guppies were tested in our experiment. The two species were maintained separately in 150 one-stock aquaria (approximately 15 individuals per aquarium). Aquaria were provided with natural gravel, an air filter and live plants. Both stock aquaria and experimental tanks were maintained at a constant temperature of 25 ± 1°C and a 14:10 h light:dark (L:D) photoperiod with an 18-W fluorescent light. Before the experiment, fish were fed twice daily to satiation with commercial food flakes and live brine shrimp (Artemia salina).

Apparatus and Stimuli

We use a modification of the procedures that have been traditionally adopted for investigating visual discrimination in fishes54, a two-alternative spatial forced-choice paradigm that was recently developed by Agrillo et al.55,56 to study numerical competence in fish.

The experimental apparatus was composed by a 50 × 19 × 32 cm tank. It was filled with gravel and 24 cm of water. The long walls were covered with green plastic material and the short walls were covered with white plastic material. To reduce the potential effects of social isolation57, two mirrors (29 × 5 cm) were placed in the middle of the tank, 3 cm away from the long walls. At the two short ends of the tank, two identical monitors (19 inches) were used to present the stimuli.

The stimuli employed in the training phase were pairs of identical geometrical figures (12 × 12 cm) presented on a computer screen. Each array contained circles, stars, triangles and rectangles presented in two different conditions: in the former (‘moving target’), the items rotated in a clockwise or counterclockwise direction at three different speeds (5/15/21 rpm); in the latter condition, the same figures were presented without any movement (non-moving target). To avoid any issue related to potential difference in color vision of the two species, all stimuli were converted to grayscale (as the RSI and the control stimulus presented in the test phase).

A total of 16 different couples of stimuli were employed and alternated in a pseudo-random order. In particular in the initial training, arrays containing a single figure (either static or dynamic) was presented. Subsequently, in order to familiarize fish with the complex pattern of the RSI, multiple figures were presented within each array (see Supplemental Material for examples). Moving and non-moving targets were created and presented through Adobe Flash CS4®.

In test trials, we presented the Rotating Snakes Illusion13 (Figure 1a). A grey-scale version of the RSI13 and its control (both encompassed in a 12 × 12 cm square) were presented in the test phase. The latter stimulus was identical to the illusory figure, except that the luminance relationship was flipped between adjacent local texture elements (Figure 1a and b). This pattern does not induce illusory motion in humans16. Two different versions were created for both stimuli: 4 or 9 circles.

Six identical experimental tanks were employed. They were placed close to each other on the same table and lit by two fluorescent lamps (36 W). A video camera was suspended about 1 meter above the experimental tanks and used to record the position of the subjects during the tests.

Procedure

The experiment was divided into two different phases: training and test.

Training

To familiarize the fish with the novel environment, in the nine days preceding the training, six subjects were simultaneously inserted in a tank similar to that used in the experiment but enlarged in size (60 × 40 × 35 cm). During the first day of this pre-training phase, subjects were exposed together to a continuous change of the monitors' background color (from black to white alternatively). This lasted for 6 hours, during which the background color changed every hour. The second day, fish were moved in couples into the experimental tanks and underwent the same treatment. After this phase, the subjects returned to their home tank for 5 days; then the same pre-training steps were repeated, one day in the enlarged tank and another day in the experimental tanks.

After this period, the fish started the training phase. During this phase, they received four trials per day (three consecutive days, for a total of 12 trials). Soon after the stimuli appeared on the screens, the experimenter used a Pasteur pipette to release the food reward (brine shrimps) in correspondence with the moving stimulus; an identical syringe was used to simultaneously insert pure water close to the still stimulus. Subjects were left free to feed for seven minutes. After this time, the stimuli disappeared from the screen. The inter-trial interval lasted three hours. Stimuli were presented in a pseudo-random sequence and the left-right positions of the stimuli were counterbalanced over trials.

In order to assess task learning, on days 4 and 5, two probe trials were alternated each day with two reinforced trials (four probe trials in total). In the probe trials, stimuli appeared on the screen for four minutes; no reinforcement was provided (extinction procedure) and the time spent by fish within 12 cm (‘choice area’, depicted as a dotted line in Figure 2) in correspondence to the stimuli was recorded as a measure of their capacity to discriminate between them. Reinforced trials were identical to those described for days 1 to 3. To avoid the possibility of fish using the local/spatial cues of their tank, each subject was moved from one tank to another at the end of each day.

Test

After a two-day interval, three probe trials were presented each day for four consecutive days (days 8–11). The RSI was contrasted with its control stimulus. The inter-trial interval lasted three hours. The three probe trials were alternated with two reinforced trials in which we presented the same stimuli of the training phase (true moving vs. static targets), according to the following sequence: probe trial/reinforced trial/probe trial/reinforced trial/probe trial. The proportion of time spent near the RSI during probe trials was recorded as the dependent variable. Frequencies were arcsine (square root)-transformed58.

In order to test if there could be other cues in the RSI stimulus, not related to motion, that might attract the subjects to this stimulus; or, vice versa, any other cue in the static control stimulus that might repel fish, we also ran a control experiment. Twelve naïve fish, six guppies and six zebrafish, were singly housed in tanks similar to those used in the main experiment and acclimated for 24 h. Fish were then simultaneously exposed to the RSI and the control stimuli and we recorded the position for 20 minutes. After a 2-hour interval, we repeated the test, switching the position of the stimuli. We computed the time spent within 20 cm from the stimuli. Zebrafish spent 51.9 ± 4.9% and guppies 51.8 ± 8.5% near the RSI stimulus. There was no significant preference/repulsion for any of the stimuli (Repeated Measure ANOVA, F(1,10) = 0.411, P = 0.536), no species difference: (F(1,10) = 3.224, P = 0.103) or interaction (F(1,10) = 0.053, P = 0.823).

The experiments comply with all laws of the country (Italy) in which they were performed (D.M. 116192) and the study was approved by the ‘Ministero della Salute’ (permit number: 6726-2011). The methods were carried out in accordance with the approved guidelines.