Abstract
Recent advances on human dynamics have focused on the normal patterns of human activities, with the quantitative understanding of human behavior under extreme events remaining a crucial missing chapter. This has a wide array of potential applications, ranging from emergency response and detection to traffic control and management. Previous studies have shown that human communications are both temporally and spatially localized following the onset of emergencies, indicating that social propagation is a primary means to propagate situational awareness. We study real anomalous events using country-wide mobile phone data, finding that information flow during emergencies is dominated by repeated communications. We further demonstrate that the observed communication patterns cannot be explained by inherent reciprocity in social networks and are universal across different demographics.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
Much effort has been devoted to the study of human dynamics under regular and stationary situations7,9,10,11,15,16,19,23,28,30,31,32,34,37,40,45. Our quantitative understanding of human behavior under extreme conditions, such as violent conflicts5, life-threatening epidemic outbreaks3,9,10,25 and other large-scale emergencies, remains limited however. Yet, it is essential for a number of practical problems faced by emergency responders29. There is an extraordinary need, therefore, to quantitatively study human dynamics and social interactions under rapidly changing or unfamiliar conditions.
Previous studies2,24 have suggested that mobile phones can act as in situ sensors for human behavior during anomalous events, finding that the occurrence of anomalous events triggers a large spike in the communication activity of those who witnessed the event. More specifically, they found that communication spikes following emergencies are temporally and spatially localized, indicating information flow through the social networks of affected individuals becomes an important means to spread situational awareness and information to the general population.
In this work, we quantify the propagation of real-world emergency information through the contact networks of mobile phone users. We denote the group of users directly affected by an emergency by population G0, while users they contact during the timespan of the emergency that are not in G0 form the population G1. We focus here on how G1 users change their communication patterns following an emergency. To be specific, we address three questions: First, to what magnitude do G1 users change their communication behavior? Do they show the same volume spike as previously observed for G0 users2? Second, what is the origin of the behavior changes of G1 users? What is the dominant feature of these changes? Third, will a G1 user prefer to call back the G0 user, potentially offering comfort, support and seeking pertinent information? Or will they instead call forward to propagate their situational awareness to others. Intuitively we expect that G1 users were chosen for contact by the G0 users due to important relationship(s) between them and they may communicate with each other more often than with other peers even during normal days. It is therefore important for future emergency detection and intervention to know whether or not there is abnormal reciprocal communication during emergencies, compared with ordinary activity levels.
Data and events
In this paper, we use a de-identified dataset from a large mobile phone company in a European country1,4,8,14,16,20,21,27,28,33,36,37,39,42,43,44. The data consist of approximately 10 million users and four years of cell phone activity, including both voice calls and text messages. Each data entry records the user initiating the call or text (caller) and the user receiving it (callee); the cellular tower that routed the call; and the date and time when it occurred. The locations (longitude and latitude) of cellular towers are also recorded, allowing us to infer the location of callers whenever they initiate a communication. Hence, given the spatiotemporal localization of an event, these data offers a unique opportunity to quantify the social response of the affected population.
To study real events covered by this mobile phone data, we need to determine their times and locations. We study the event set identified in previous studies2, where the authors used Google local news (news.google.com) service to search for news stories covering the country and time period of the mobile phone dataset. Keywords such as ‘emergency’, ‘disaster’, ‘concert’, etc. were used to find potential news stories. Important events such as bombings, earthquakes and concerts are prominently covered in the social media. Study of these reports typically gave the precise time and the locations for these events2.
To identify the beginning and the end of an event, tstart and tstop, we adopt the following procedure2. First, we scan all calls in the event region during the day covering the event, giving the event day call volume time series (number of calls per minute) Vevent(t)2. Then, we scan calls for a number of “normal” days, those modulo one week from the event day, exploiting the weekly periodicity of V(t). These normal days' call volume time series are averaged to get 〈Vnormal〉. To smooth the time series, call volumes were binned into 10 minute intervals. The standard deviation σ(Vnormal) as a function of time is then used to compute z(t) = ΔV(t)/σ(Vnormal), where ΔV(t) = Vevent(t) − 〈Vnormal〉 is the call volume change during the event day. Finally, the interval [tstart, tstop] was the longest contiguous run of time intervals where z(t) > zthr, for some fixed cutoff zthr. To be consistent with pervious studies2, we chose zthr = 1.5 for all events.
Results
To extract the contact network between users during an event, we track all outgoing calls in order of occurrence during the event's time interval [tstart, tstop]. We therefore identified the individuals located within the event region (G0), as well as a G1 group consisting of individuals outside the event region but who receive calls from the G0 group during the event, a G2 group that receive calls from G1 and so on.
To determine how unusual the observed activities are, we compare the call volume during the event to the average call volumes of a number of “normal” days (Sec. Data and events). Since a temporal contact network can always be constructed from mobile phone dataset, even when no event occurs, it is necessary to design a proper control for normal days to make the call volumes between event day and normal days comparable.
To design a proper control, we study new “cascades” generated by the same eyewitness users G0 during the same time interval [tstart, tstop] for each normal day and create a different cascade {G0, g1, g2, …} for each normal day. Gi are for event period whereas gi are for normal period. Gi and gi share the same G0, that is by definition G0 = g0. e.g. g1 users are the users who receive cell-phone communication directly from G0 users, gi users (i > 1) are the users who receive cell-phone communication directly from gi−1 users.
The number of Gi users will typically be larger than that of gi users and Gi users may be more active than gi users, so the normal day's call volume time series, V(t|gi), must be rescaled when compared to the event day's call volume time series, V(t|Gi). For this purpose, we multiply V(t|gi) by a constant scaling factor ai,
where both integrals run over the same “calibration interval” δt and τ = 0 is the start of the selection window. For most events, we integrate over a 24-hour period two days before the event window. The factor ai was chosen such that the total number of calls during normal time periods for V(t|Gi) is approximately equal to aiV(t|gi), equalizing the normal-day time series and removing any biases due to |Gi| ≠ |gi|. This control procedure allows us to investigate call volume patterns of different user groups.
To explore the call volume patterns in different user groups, we measure the call volume change
for G0 and G1 groups as a function of time, where V(t|Gi) is the call volume in the event day and 〈aiV(t|gi)〉 is the call volume averaged over selected “normal” days.
Previous work has studied several aspects of communication patterns and found a spike in the volume of phone call activity during an emergency event2. Yet, by using the control mentioned above, we find the call volume change for different groups such as G0 and G1 exhibits different patterns in different events (Fig. 1). More specifically, we observe activity spikes in both G0 and G1 groups for three emergency events, referred to as “Jet Scare”, “Plane Crash” and “Bombing” (Fig. 1a–c). Yet in all other events, there is no volume spike for the G1 group, e.g. “Concert” (Fig. 1d). These results are consistent with previous findings2, showing that the users in the G1 group are triggered to a higher communication level, characterized by a sharp increase in call volumes, during the emergency events. Yet, it is somewhat puzzling that the call volume change of G1 users have a spike, which is instantaneous and shows virtually no delay to the spike of G0 users'. Note the spikes of G0 and G1 users are synchronous qualitatively and sensitive to the time aggregation (see Supplementary Materials). As the activity spikes of G0 users for emergency events are both temporally and spatially localized, the communication of G1 users becomes the most important means of spreading situational awareness.
To quantify the reach of situational awareness, we focus on G1 and study their communication patterns after receiving a phone call or message from G0. As an example, we choose three G0 users (diamonds) and their related G1 users (circles) and G2 users (triangles) in the Bombing event as a sample contact network (Fig. 2a). There are three types of communication behaviors for users in G1: (1) call back to G0 user(s), which are edges in orange, (2) call forward to G2 users, edges in purple and (3) calls to other G1 user(s), edges in green. We denote these three kinds of communication behaviors with, C10, C12 and C11, respectively. We measure the contributions of these three communication modes to the total activities of G1 users, finding C11 constitutes no more than 5% of the total volume of G1 users' communicating activities (Fig. 2b). The observed low volume of C11 among G1 users during emergencies is somewhat unexpected, given the importance of triadic closure in social communications6,17. Hence, the spike observed in G1 users in Fig. 1 is mainly determined by C10 and C12.
The existence of different communication modes in G1 (Fig. 2a) raises an important question: what is the temporal contribution of C10 and C12 to the observed spikes in G1 users' activities? To this end, we decompose ΔV(t|G1) into V(C10) and V(C12) by modifying the rescaling framework
for i = {0, 2}, where V1i is the call volume from G1 (g1) users to Gi (gi) users and a1i is a scaling factor modified from Eq. 1 as
In Fig. 3, we show V(C10) and V(C12) as a function of time. We find that in all three emergency events, V(C10) has evident volume spikes during the event period. And, by comparing Fig. 1 and Fig. 3, we find the peaks of V(C10) are in close temporal vicinity to those of ΔV(t|G1). Yet, for Concert (Fig. 3d), we observed no clear volume spikes. Overall, Fig. 3 demonstrates that C10, as a major contribution to the observed spikes in G1, indicating the call back actions (C10) in emergencies contribute more to the spike of ΔV(t|G1) than the call forward actions (C12). That is, G1 users prefer to interact back with G0 users rather than contacting with new users (G2), a phenomenon that limits the spreading of information. Indeed, C10 measures the reciprocal communications from G1 to G0, representing correspondence and coordination calls between social neighbors. C12, on the other hand, measures the dissemination of situational awareness, corresponding to information cascades that penetrate the underlying social network. Hence, the results in Fig. 3 indicate that during emergencies both dissemination and call-back response of emergency information are important for information flow and they together determine the magnitude of G1 users' communication spikes (Fig. 1).
These preceding results indicate that reciprocal communications play a dominant role in social response during emergency, raising an important question: what is the origin of the observed reciprocal correspondence? There are two possible mechanisms likely at work here. First is the heterogeneous nature of reciprocities: Social ties are characterized by different reciprocities18,22,26,35,38,41, corresponding to different likelihood of reciprocal communications upon receiving a call from others. Therefore, the large increase in reciprocal communications may represent a selection bias introduced by eyewitnesses by communicating with their social neighbors with high reciprocity. The second possible factor is a behavioral change of social neighbors after learning about the event, corresponding to coordination and providing additional information to eyewitnesses. To quantify the competition between these two factors, we measure the reciprocity of communications between any two individuals, during normal periods.
In an unweighted network, a general definition of link reciprocity R is to measure the tendency of two nodes to form mutual connections (A → B and B → A)12,26,46. Hence, R = 1 for a purely bidirectional link and R = 0 for a unidirectional one. Considering the weighted nature of contact networks13,22,41, we define the reciprocity of communications between G0 and G1 users as
where Vx→y is the number of calls from user x to user y within a given period and |·| is the absolute value. With this definition, two users have a reciprocity ranging from 0 to 1, where R = 1 corresponds to reciprocal links and R = 0 for non-reciprocal ones.
In Fig. 4, we show the reciprocity of communications averaged over all pairs of users in G0 and G1 for the four events during event periods. We find that, for Bombing, Plane Crash and Jet Scare, the average reciprocity for each emergency event shows a significant increase, deviating by approximately 1.8, 3 and 6 standard deviations from normal days, respectively. This result indicates that the observed increase in “call-back” actions from G1 to G0 during these emergency events correspond to behavioral changes in communications. If the increased call-back were entirely random, the distribution of reciprocity over the G1 population would be sufficient to explain the resulting call-back, but we do not observe this. For Concert, we observe a decrease in reciprocity comparing to normal periods, with 4.7σ below the averaged reciprocity of normal days, indicating clear distinctions between emergency and non-emergency events.
To test the consistency of our results, we also study reciprocity for other emergency and non-emergency events. As shown in Supplementary Materials Fig. S1, the reciprocities for Blackout and Earthquake are characterized by only modest increase, well within the range of one standard deviation, reassuring the preceding results on correlations between activity spikes in G1 (Fig. 3a in Ref. 2) and their increase in reciprocity.
Finally, to better understand the origin of the observed increase of reciprocity, we measure the contributions to G1 users' behavioral change for different demographics. More specifically, we obtained self-reported gender information, available for 88% of users and consider all communications in which gender information is available for both parties. There are four kinds of coupled pairs between G0 and G1 users: male-male (MM), male-female (MF), female-male (FM) and female-female (FF). For each event, we compute the reciprocities for the four different kinds of pairs separately. We also average the reciprocities of the MF and FM pairs as cross-gender pairs (CG) and the MM and FF pairs as same-gender pairs (SG). Interestingly, we find the collective response from different demographics is almost universal (Fig. 5). That is, for emergency events with significant increases in reciprocity (Jet Scare and Plane Crash), the reciprocities across different gender pairs are all several standard deviations larger than normal periods (Fig. 4 (a) and (b)).
Discussion
Taken together, we have studied cell phone communications during anomalous events and find volume spikes in G1's communication compared to normal days in three emergencies. To uncover the possible origin of the volume spikes, we decomposed G1's communications into call-back (C10) and call-forward (C12) actions. Comparing to non-emergency events, we found that the dominant component of volume spikes is C10 for all three emergency events, indicating the need for correspondence with eyewitnesses is more critical than the dissemination of situational awareness during emergencies. We further demonstrated such communication patterns correspond to a behavioral change in G1 users that cannot be explained by reciprocity or demographics. We believe the empirical findings reported in this paper present relevant information that can be used to benchmark potential models and will play an increasingly important role as large-scale data flourish and our quantitative understanding human behavior deepens.
References
Bagrow, J. P. & Brockmann, D. Natural emergence of clusters and bursts in network evolution. Phys. Rev. X 3, 021016 (2013).
Bagrow, J. P., Wang, D. & Barabási, A.-L. Collective response of human populations to largescale emergencies. PLoS ONE 6, e17680 (2011).
Balcan, D. et al. Multiscale mobility networks and the spatial spreading of infectious diseases. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 106, 21484–21489 (2009).
Barabási, A.-L. The origin of bursts and heavy tails in human dynamics. Nature 435, 207–211 (2005).
Bohorquez, J. C., Gourley, S., Dixon, A. R., Spagat, M. & Johnson, N. F. Common ecology quantifies human insurgency. Nature 462, 911–914 (2009).
Borgatti, S. P., Mehra, A., Brass, D. J. & Labianca, G. Network analysis in the social sciences. Science 323, 892–895 (2009).
Brockmann, D., Hufnagel, L. & Geisel, T. The scaling laws of human travel. Nature 439, 462–465 (2006).
Candia, J. et al. Uncovering individual and collective human dynamics from mobile phone records. J. Phys. A 41, 224015 (2008).
Colizza, V., Barrat, A., Barthélemy, M. & Vespignani, A. The role of the airline transportation network in the prediction and predictability of global epidemics. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 103, 2015–2020 (2006).
Eubank, S. et al. Modelling disease outbreaks in realistic urban social networks. Nature 429, 180–184 (2004).
Gabrielli, A. & Caldarelli, G. Invasion percolation and the time scaling behavior of a queuing model of human dynamics. J. Stat. Mech. Theor. Exp., P02046 (2009).
Garlaschelli, D. & Loffredo, M. I. Patterns of link reciprocity in directed networks. Phys. Rev. Lett. 93, 268701 (2004).
Gavalda, A., Duch, J. & Gómez-Gardeñes, J. Reciprocal interactions out of congestion-free adaptive networks. Phys. Rev. E 85, 026112 (2012).
Goh, K.-I. & Barabási, A.-L. Burstiness and memory in complex systems. EPL (Europhys. Lett) 81, 48002 (2008).
Goncalves, B. & Ramasco, J. J. Human dynamics revealed through web analytics. Phys. Rev. E 78, 026123 (2008).
González, M. C., Hidalgo, C. A. & Barabási, A.-L. Understanding individual human mobility patterns. Nature 453, 779–782 (2008).
Granovetter, M. S. The strength of weak ties. Am. J. Sociol. 78, 1360–1380 (1973).
Hidalgo, C. A. & Rodriguez-Sickert, C. The dynamics of a mobile phone network. Physica A 387, 3017–3024 (2008).
Hufnagel, L., Brockmann, D. & Geisel, T. Forecast and control of epidemics in a globalized world. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 101, 15124–15129 (2004).
Jo, H.-H., Karsai, M., Kertész, J. & Kaski, K. Circadian pattern and burstiness in mobile phone communication. New J. Phys. 14, 013055 (2012).
Karsai, M., Kaski, K., Barabási, A.-L. & Kertész, J. Universal features of correlated bursty behaviour. Sci. Rep. 2, 397 (2012).
Kovanen, L., Saramaki, J. & Kaski, K. Reciprocity of mobile phone calls. Dynamics of Socio-Economic Systems 2, 138–151 (2011).
Lazer, D. et al. Social science: Computational social science. Science 323, 721–723 (2009).
Lu, X., Bengtsson, L. & Holme, P. Predictability of population displacement after the 2010 haiti earthquake. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 109, 11576–11581 (2012).
Meloni, S. et al. Modeling human mobility responses to the large-scale spreading of infectious diseases. Sci. Rep. 1, 62 (2011).
Newman, M. E. J., Forrest, S. & Balthrop, J. Email networks and the spread of computer viruses. Phys. Rev. E 66, 035101 (2002).
Oliveira, J. G. & Barabási, A.-L. Human dynamics: Darwin and einstein correspondence patterns. Nature 437, 1251–1251 (2005).
Onnela, J.-P. et al. Structure and tie strengths in mobile communication networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 104, 7332–7336 (2007).
Petrescu-Prahova, M. & Butts, C. Emergent coordinators in the world trade center disaster. International Journal of Mass Emergencies and Disasters 26, 133168 (2008).
Ratkiewicz, J., Fortunato, S., Flammini, A., Menczer, F. & Vespignani, A. Characterizing and modeling the dynamics of online popularity. Phys. Rev. Lett. 105, 158701 (2010).
Rybski, D., Buldyrev, S. V., Havlin, S., Liljeros, F. & Makse, H. A. Scaling laws of human interaction activity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 106, 12640–12645 (2009).
Rybski, D., Buldyrev, S. V., Havlin, S., Liljeros, F. & Makse, H. A. Communication activity in a social network: relation between long-term correlations and inter-event clustering. Sci. Rep. 2, 560 (2012).
Simini, F., González, M. C., Maritan, A. & Barabási, A.-L. A universal model for mobility and migration patterns. Nature 484, 96–100 (2012).
Singer, H. M., Singer, I. & Herrmann, H. J. Agent-based model for friendship in social networks. Physical Review E 80, 026113 (2009).
Skvoretz, J. & Agneessens, F. Reciprocity, multiplexity and exchange: Measures. Quality & Quantity 41, 341–357 (2007).
Song, C., Koren, T., Wang, P. & Barabási, A.-L. Modelling the scaling properties of human mobility. Nat Phys 6, 818823 (2010).
Song, C., Qu, Z., Blumm, N. & Barabási, A.-L. Limits of predictability in human mobility. Science 327, 1018–1021 (2010).
van Doorn, G. S. & Taborsky, M. The evolution of generalized reciprocity on social interaction networks. Evolution 66, 651–664 (2012).
Vazquez, A. et al. Modeling bursts and heavy tails in human dynamics. Physical Review E 73, 036127 (2006).
Vespignani, A. Predicting the behavior of techno-social systems. Science 325, 425–428 (2009).
Wang, C. et al. Weighted reciprocity in human communication networks. arXiv:1108.2822 (2011).
Wang, D., Pedreschi, D., Song, C., Giannotti, F. & Barabási, A.-L. Human mobility, social ties and link prediction. In: Proceedings of the 17th ACM SIGKDD International Conference on Knowledge Discovery and Data Mining KDD'11, 1100–1108, New York, NY, USA, (2011).
Wang, P. & González, M. C. Understanding spatial connectivity of individuals with non-uniform population density. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. A 367, 3321–3329 (2009).
Wang, P., González, M. C., Hidalgo, C. A. & Barabási, A.-L. Understanding the spreading patterns of mobile phone viruses. Science 324, 1071–1076 (2009).
Wu, F. & Huberman, B. A. Novelty and collective attention. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 104, 17599–17601 (2007).
Zamora-López, G., Zlatić, V., Zhou, C., Štefančić, H. & Kurths, J. Reciprocity of networks with degree correlations and arbitrary degree sequences. Phys. Rev. E 77, 016106 (2008).
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by the Network Science Collaborative Technology Alliance sponsored by the US Army Research Laboratory under Agreement Number W911NF-09-2-0053; the Office of Naval Research under Agreement Number N000141010968; the Defense Threat Reduction Agency awards WMD BRBAA07-J-2-0035 and BRBAA08-Per4-C-2-0033; and the James S. Mc-Donnell Foundation 21st Century Initiative in Studying Complex Systems. L. Gao and Z. Gao thank the financial support from the Major State Basic Research Development Program of China (973 Program) No. 2012CB725400, the China Scholarship Council, the National Natural Science Foundation of China (71101009, 71131001) and the Fundamental Research Funds for the Central Universities No. 2012JBM067.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
L.G., C.S. and D.W. designed research; L.G. and D.W. performed research; L.G., J.B. and D.W. analyzed data; L.G., Z.G., A.B., J.B. and D.W. wrote the paper.
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
Electronic supplementary material
Supplementary Information
Quantifying Information Flow During Emergencies
Rights and permissions
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivs 3.0 Unported License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/3.0/
About this article
Cite this article
Gao, L., Song, C., Gao, Z. et al. Quantifying Information Flow During Emergencies. Sci Rep 4, 3997 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1038/srep03997
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/srep03997
This article is cited by
-
Mobile phone data reveal the effects of violence on internal displacement in Afghanistan
Nature Human Behaviour (2022)
-
Triadic embeddedness structure in family networks predicts mobile communication response to a sudden natural disaster
Nature Communications (2021)
-
Spatiotemporal behaviors of the ridership of a public transportation system during an epidemic outbreak: case of MERS in Seoul
Journal of the Korean Physical Society (2021)
-
Using Mobile Phone Data for Emergency Management: a Systematic Literature Review
Information Systems Frontiers (2020)
-
Universal knowledge discovery from big data using combined dual-cycle
International Journal of Machine Learning and Cybernetics (2018)
Comments
By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.