Abstract
Researchers examining conflict between intimate partners believe that the experience and expression of emotion drives aggressive behaviour. Intra-personally, increases in negative affect make aggression more likely. Inter-personally, theoretical models suggest that each individuals’ perception of their partners’ emotion also influences aggression, potentially creating a Violence Escalation Cycle. Here, using a lab-based aggression task across a primary study (n = 104, number of trials = 3095) and a replication (n = 58, number of trials = 3167), we show that both intra- and inter-personal experiences of negative emotion predict reactive aggression within couples, revealing retaliation but not escalation. Critically, analyses of facial affect reveal that prototypic displays of negative emotions have a compounding effect, leading to dramatic changes in aggression depending on whether one, both, or neither partner expressed negative emotion. We propose a mechanism by which temporal delays (i.e., experimentally imposed forced breaks) reduce aggression by decreasing negative emotional arousal and limiting impulsive action. Our results show that both forced breaks and elective breaks (i.e., extra participant-initiated extensions of the forced break time) reduce aggression, providing exciting evidence that interventions focused on preventing impulsive action when people are in a provoked state can reduce aggression within couples.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
Intimate Partner Aggression is widespread, with around a third of dating couples at universities engaging in some form of physical aggression during conflict1. As a result, researchers and practitioners are increasingly focused on understanding the social and emotional interactions that occur between partners during everyday conflict, and how these predict the escalation towards aggression2. Aggression between partners is frequently reactive in nature—that is, it is impulsive3,4, and is characterised by strong negative emotional arousal5,6,7. Critically, emotional arousal and impulsivity are linked, such that acute negative affect (e.g., an angry state) is associated with failures of self-control. As a personality trait, the tendency to act rashly when upset is called “negative urgency“3,5,6,7,8, and it has been implicated as a central contributor to intimate partner aggression. By contrast, the systematic pattern of abuse sometimes known as ‘intimate terrorism’ or “coercive controlling violence’ is less strongly associated with impulsivity and we therefore use the term ‘intimate partner aggression’, rather than ‘intimate partner violence’, to describe our research focus9.
Within dyads, theoretical accounts such as the General Aggression Model10, and the I3 model11, further argue that emotions from both one’s self (intra-personal) and one’s partner (inter-personal) influences one’s aggressive behaviour. Consequently, if reactive aggression between couples requires negative arousal (influenced by both partners) and impulsivity, we reasoned that it should be possible to reduce behavioural aggression by experimentally blocking impulsive action while participants are in a provoked state. In principle, impulsive action can be prevented by introducing a ‘forced break’ period (e.g., a brief experimentally imposed delay) between provocation and the opportunity for aggression. If impulsivity during a provoked state is critical, we would expect a forced break to limit aggression by blocking impulsive action and reducing negative emotion. In essence, assessing the consequences of an experimentally imposed forced break on the dynamics of dyadic conflict provides a strong test of the influence of both negative affect and impulsivity during provocation, allowing us to examine current accounts of Intimate Partner Aggression.
Negative urgency is believed to reflect a failure to appropriately regulate negative emotional arousal. As noted above, negative emotional arousal (and poor emotion regulation ability) has been robustly shown to increase the likelihood of aggressive behaviour within individuals12,13,14,15,16. Indeed, three recent meta-analyses all conclude that the inability to regulate negative emotions correlates with reactive aggression14,15,16. Across these studies, emotional arousal has been assessed using a range of methods, including self-report ratings17,18, heart rate19, and the analysis of facial expressions.20, where prototypic displays of negative emotion (specifically anger and disgust, the so-called ‘moral emotions’21,22) are particularly relevant to conflict and aggression. More recently, researchers have suggested that emotional arousal and regulation between partners (known as co-regulation) may also help to explain aggression in couples16,20,23,24, though robust experimental evidence is lacking due to a paucity of research using face-to-face dyadic designs (i.e., where two people engage in an aggression task together). Research involving couples is clearly essential to fully understand the impact of dyadic affective processes on conflict, particularly the impact of one partner’s emotional expression on the other partner’s behaviour. Consequently, in the present study we examine behaviour using real couples engaging in competitive interactions face-to-face. One advantage of employing a dyadic aggression task is that this approach can be combined with real-time estimates of emotional arousal, such as monitoring facial expressions, allowing the temporal dynamics of conflict to be assessed.
While experimental studies of dyadic conflict are rare, studies using self-report measures routinely suggest that most couples’ aggression found in community samples (i.e., not clinical, forensic, or offender populations) is bidirectional, meaning that both partners act aggressively during conflict, 25,26,27. Given the bidirectionality of most couples’ conflict, the Violence Escalation Cycle28 predicts that aggression within dyads will involve retaliation that escalates over the course of a conflict. Specifically, the Violence Escalation Cycle posits that when a person aggresses against their partner, the partner will perceive an injustice and retaliate with slightly more aggression, wherein the original aggressor will perceive an injustice and retaliate as well, resulting in a cyclic pattern of escalation. Despite prominent theories10,28 (and some research)28 attesting to the escalatory nature of couples conflict, to date, there is little experimental evidence pertaining to retaliation and escalation, at least in part due to the paucity of experimental, dyadic research noted above.
In response, the current study asks three questions relating to the role of dyadic affective and behavioural processes in reactive aggression between romantic partners: First, does the introduction of a forced break (i.e., a brief experimentally manipulated delay ranging from five to 15 s) between provocation and the opportunity for aggression prevent impulsive action and reduce negative emotions, thereby decreasing aggression? If the answer to this first question is “yes”, then do longer breaks produce greater reductions? Second, do measures of emotional expression from both partners (assessed via automated machine learning assisted affect coding of videos taken during dyadic interactions) provide evidence of co-regulatory interaction, modulating an aggressor’s actions? Third, does aggressive behaviour between romantic partners exhibit escalating retaliation over the time course of a conflict when couples are engaged in an on-going competitive interaction? Overall, therefore, our aim is to assess the ways in which emotions experienced by both partners during a conflict are predictive of aggressive behaviour.
To answer these questions we adapted a common aggression paradigm (the Competitive Reaction Time Task; CRTT; similar to the Taylor Aggression Paradigm (TAP))29 to allow intimate partners (adults in romantic relationships; chosen for our focus on intimate partner conflict) to compete face-to-face in a multi-round reaction time ‘game’ (see Fig. 1a). To provide an ethically acceptable opportunity for participants to exhibit aggressive behaviour, the paradigm requires the winner of each round to select the volume of a noxious noise (a ‘sound blast’) to send to their losing partner’s headphones (see Fig. 1b). In the ‘immediate response’ control condition (which functions as our baseline), the winner is allowed to select a blast level immediately after winning, whereas in the ‘forced break’ experimental conditions, the winner is blocked from selecting a blast level for 5, 10, or 15 s after winning. We include multiple break lengths to potentially identify the most effective delay (i.e., the largest decrease in aggression with the lowest time cost).
As well as using trial-to-trial changes in blast level as an empirical measure of aggressive behaviour, all participants completed a self-report measure of trait aggression (BPAQ-SF: the Buss and Perry Aggression Questionnaire, Short Form). In addition, we used OpenFace 2.030 (a machine learning program for automated Facial Action Coding31) to identify prototypic displays of emotion during the blast initiation response time window (blast initiation ±1 s; see Fig. 1b), which allows us to assess the impact of each participant’s emotional experience on behaviour (see Fig. 1c). The differences between positive and negative emotions expressed during the experiment are visible in composite images (created with PsychoMorph32; see Fig. 1d). Importantly, because we measured facial expressions continuously throughout the task, we were able to examine how behaviour changed as a function of the emotions experienced by each partner. Further, and whenever possible, we use trial-level data in our analysis to maximize power. The use of trial-level data enables us to examine the rich relationship between each partner’s blast levels and affect over time, and potentially solve a historic problem in the field (i.e., how to study aggression in lab settings when the level of aggressive behaviour that can be ethically elicited is minimal). A solution here is possible because trial-level data enables us to extract instances of aggression within a context of overall non-aggressiveness (i.e., an ethically permissible lab-based aggression paradigm). From a theoretical perspective, therefore, our paradigm includes the features that are expected to produce behavioural aggression according to the I3 model and Perfect Storm Theory2 (instigation/provocation, an impellent, and dis-inhibition, see Fig. 2). Finally, in addition to our primary study, we also present a replication that provides additional support for the initial findings and includes qualitative reports of the impact of forced breaks on our participants, illuminating the mechanism behind forced breaks.
Methods
Primary study
Participants
A total of 104 participants (60 women, 3 non-binary participants; assessed using the item “What is your gender?” in the post-experiment questionnaire) were recruited from a participant pool at the University of St Andrews in Scotland. Participants were romantic couples (of any orientation) who participated together, and each partner was given £12.50 in compensation. Participants’ mean age was 21.1 years (SD = 3.3) with a minimum of 18 and a maximum of 39. Most participants were full time students (72.8%) and white (70.3%). Most participants were not cohabiting (66.1%), and the average duration of a relationship was roughly eight months. This study was approved by the School of Psychology and Neuroscience Ethics Committee in December 2022 (approval code PS16636) before data collection commenced. Consent from participants was obtained in person with physical consent forms; procedure described below.
Design
We used a between-groups design. Couples were assigned randomly upon arrival (and without their knowledge) either to the immediate response (n = 23) or one of the forced break conditions (5 s: n = 32; 10 s: n = 23; and 15 s: n = 26). The study was not pre-registered.
Materials
Trait Aggression
We assessed trait aggression using the short form of the Buss and Perry Aggression Questionnaire (BPAQ)33. The BPAQ-SF is a self-report measure that asks participants to rate statements (e.g., “I have threatened people I know”, “I have trouble controlling my temper”, etc.), judging how well the statement describes them on a five-point scale from “very unlike me” to “very like me”. Higher scores indicate higher trait aggression.
Acute Aggressive Behaviour
Acute aggressive behaviour was measured using a modified version of the Competitive Reaction Time Task (CRTT)34. In the classic CRTT, a participant plays a competitive reaction time game against a fictitious virtual opponent, and the (rigged) winner of each round sends a noxious white noise “sound blast” to the loser (where the winner chooses the volume, duration, or both). Extending Anderson and colleagues’28 modification where two real participants unknown to each other played virtually from separate rooms, we brought romantic partners into a single room, together, and had them play face-to-face28. According to the recommendations of Elson and colleagues35, participants could modify only the loudness of the noise blasts (and not the duration) on an eight point scale ranging from 75db to 110db, played for 2 s (with an instantaneous rise time)35.
Our Face-To-Face CRTT (FTF-CRTT) was partially rigged. When participants’ reaction times were within 100 ms of each other, a winner was selected at random. When one participant’s reaction time was more than 100 ms later than their partner, the faster participant won the round. This was designed to avoid suspicion of rigging (pilot testing showed that participants could auditorily distinguish whose button press reaction time was faster when there was more than a 100 ms gap) while keeping the win-loss rate of each participant close to 0.5 (so that all players had roughly equal opportunity to aggress). Higher scores (i.e., where the winner chooses a louder blast to play to the loser of the round) are indicative of more aggression. For the purpose of comparing trial level blasts over time, and to allow graphical representations of the interaction between couples, blast levels during win streaks (i.e., where a participant wins two or more rounds in a row) were compressed and the mean of the streak represents the blast level for the given time interval (and the mean of negative expression represents negativity expressed across the win streak).
FACS and OpenFace
FTF-CRTT gameplay was recorded using an Insta360 camera placed in-between participants below face height (so couples could see each other unobstructed). Videos were then exported twice as standard mp4 files (each focusing on one partner). Action units of the Facial Action Coding System (FACS)36,37 were extracted automatically by OpenFace38, a machine learning program designed to automate FACS scoring. Once action units were extracted, the scoring for specific emotions was coded using R39 according to the FACS and Emotion FACS (EMFACS) researcher manuals (not publicly available). OpenFace provides both binary read outs of action units (i.e., present or absent) and intensity readouts on a scale from zero to five (based on the FACS intensity scoring system). To maintain homogeneity with the FACS, we used the intensity readouts in our analysis. Higher intensity scores indicate stronger prototypic emotion expression, which is assumed to index experienced emotional arousal. Prototypic emotional displays were coded according to the core combinations introduced by Ekman and Friesen36,37, modified to fit EMFACS coding rules and use with OpenFace (see Table SI1 of the Supplementary Information for details). Negative emotion included prototypic displays of anger and disgust, whilst positive emotion included happiness. FACS scoring was conducted by AGM, who is certified to use the FACS and the EMFACS through the Paul Ekman Group.
Having identified the intensity of negative or positive emotion during the blast initiation response time window (blast initiation ± 1 s) for each player, throughout the task, we used selective averaging procedures to examine blast level selections as a function of emotion. High levels of negative affect were operationally defined as trials where negative expressive power was greater than one standard deviation above the mean (with all other trials categorised as low). High levels of positive affect was defined as trials where positive expressive power was greater than one standard deviation above the mean (with all other trials categorised as low) and negative affect was low. To examine compounding effects, blast level selections in the ‘immediate response’ condition were categorised according to whether each member of each couple exhibited a high level of negative affect (defined as above, examined independently in each player).
Procedure
The study was advertised using the University of St Andrews’ internal participant recruitment and memo systems as a study of competitive reaction time (omitting our focus on aggression). Interested participants completed an online pre-screen to assess inclusion criteria (i.e., over the age of majority in Scotland, currently in a romantic relationship). Following form submission, an experimental session was scheduled and both partners came to the lab together. Upon arrival, participants read information sheets and signed consent forms. After being given an opportunity to ask questions, participants were shown a demonstration of the game, where they were able to hear the lowest, medium, and highest blast levels (1, 4, and 8, played at 75 db, 90 db, and 110 db, respectively). Participants were then prompted to put on their headphones and stand in front of their monitors. Each participants had a set of headphones, a large arcade button, and a keyboard (see Fig. 1a). The participant stations were set up such that participants stood facing each other, with no visual obstructions. Our headphones were not noise isolating, and partners could communicate verbally. The 360° camera was positioned between participants, just above the level of the monitors and below the eyeline. Video was recorded in 5.7k at 30 fps.
Once participants were ready to begin, our custom FTF-CRTT interface (coded in Python40) appeared on participants’ screens. They were prompted to enter their names and begin the game. Each round started with a black screen displaying “Ready” in white text. One second later, “Set” appeared, and then 0–8 s later (time length was randomized to increase task attention), “GO!!” appeared, signalling the participants to hit their large arcade buttons as quickly as possible. The names of the winner and loser of the round were displayed. The winner was then prompted to enter their selected blast level on their keyboard and the blast was sent to the loser’s headphones. The next round would begin immediately once the blast ended, and the game continued for 30 rounds. In the control “immediate response” condition (which functions as our baseline), participants were able to select the blast level straight away, whereas in the experimental “forced break” conditions, participants were unable to select a blast level for 5, 10, or 15 s after the winner was announced. We used a between subject design and multiple forced break lengths to assess whether the impact of forced breaks varied by their length.
Once all 30 rounds were complete, participants filled in a post-experimental questionnaire containing the BPAQ-SF and custom measures of self- and other- emotion and aggression. We also prompted participants to (optionally) indicate how they felt about their experience overall in a free text answer box as part of the debrief. Participants were then debriefed and told of the true intentions of the study (to examine aggression). All participants were given the opportunity to ask questions about the study and directed to support resources that would be helpful in case of distress. We then recorded their email addresses so that we could deliver compensation, thanked them, and released them.
We did not show participants the blast levels chosen by their partner, but we also did not prevent them from peeking at their partner’s keyboard or talking to each other about the discrete numbers selected. Because the loser received whichever blast level the winner chose, we had no reason to try to hide this from the loser, and no practical way to prevent participants from discussing it. In fact, we chose to use non-noise isolating headphones specifically so that partners could talk to each other during the game, to increase ecological validity, and so that the winner could hear (from sound leaked from the loser’s headphones) that they were really in control of the blast level.
Follow-on study
Participants
A total of 58 participants (29 women, 1 non-binary participant; assessed by the item “what is your gender?” in the post-experiment questionnaire) were recruited from a participant pool at the University of St Andrews in Scotland. Participants were romantic couples (of any sexual orientation) who participated together, and each partner was given £12.50 in compensation. Participants’ mean age was 21.9 years (SD = 7.4) with a minimum of 18 and a maximum of 62. Most participants were full time students (80.0%) and white (83.3%). Most participants were not cohabiting (70.0%), and the average duration of a relationship was roughly fifteen months. This study was approved by the School of Psychology and Neuroscience Ethics Committee (as an amendment) in January 2024 (approval code PS16636) before data collection commenced. Consent from participants was obtained in person with physical consent forms; procedure described below.
Design
The follow-on study employed a complex design including both within and between participant manipulations. Couples were assigned randomly upon arrival (and without their knowledge) to one of two conditions that either started with the immediate response task (n = 28, 14 couples), or started with the forced break task (n = 30, 15 couples). In each case a second task was performed, to allow a within participants examination of the effects of task instruction (to be reported separately). Examining behaviour from the first task performed by each group therefore provides a direct replication of the between participants comparison of immediate response and forced break conditions in the primary study. The task and procedures (e.g., instructions, number of rounds, etc.) were matched to the original study, with the exception that in the follow up study we only included a 10 s break condition (as opposed to 5 s, 10 s, and 15 s as in the original study). Here, for the sake of brevity, we only include data from the follow-on study that provides a replication of the original experiment. For the replication data we report all measures and experimental conditions in the present article and the associated reporting summary. The study was not pre-registered.
Materials
All materials used in the primary study were used in the follow-on study. In addition, the follow-on study included two additional measures; a test of emotional facial expression recognition ability (used as a filler in between the first and second experimental tasks, data to be reported separately), and several open-ended qualitative questions (presented after the second experimental task was completed, as described below). Given the order of testing we can be confident that neither of these additional measures can have influenced the replication.
Qualitative Questions
In a survey completed at the end of the experimental session, we asked participants to rate three relationship quality factors on a Likert scale from one (low) to seven (high), as in previous research5. Further, participants were provided with an optional free-text answer box where they were asked to describe their thoughts and feelings during the game and the forced breaks.
Procedure
Procedure in our follow-on study is identical to the primary study with minor modifications. Specifically, participants completed two games (30 rounds each) of the CRTT, separated by a filler task. Following completion of the first game (as described above), participants were each given a laptop on which to complete the filler task (which took approximately seven minutes). Once both participants completed the filler task they were brought back over to the CRTT station to play another 30 rounds. Once the second game finished, participants completed the post-experimental questionnaire, and were then debriefed and excused in the same manner as the primary study.
Statistical analysis (both studies)
Null hypothesis significant testing statistics
All analysis was performed in R using two-tailed analysis. All regressions were performed using the Linear Least Squares method. All other comparison of means testing used Wilcoxon Rank Sum tests with a Continuity Correction because Shapiro-Wilk Tests for Normality revealed that all data was significantly non-normal (see Table SI2 of the Supplemental Information); equal variance was assumed. Cronbach’s Alpha41,42 indicated that internal consistency was “robust” or better43 for each measure (FTF-CRTT, 30 items, α = 0.925; negativity, 30 items, α = 0.866; BPAQ-SF, 12 items, α = 0.835). The effect size, r, was calculated from the Wilcoxon tests using the wilcox_effsize function of the rstatix package (version 0.7.2). Standard summary statistics are available in Table 1 and full statistical information is available in Table 2. Where possible, analysis was conducted with trial-level data to extract as much information from the data recorded as possible and avoid biasing results with artificially normal (averaged) data with deceptively low variance44. Finally, Multi-Level Modelling and Actor-Partner Interdependency Modelling were conducted, using the gls function from the nlme package (version 3.1–164) and the lmer function from the lme4 package (version 1.1–35.3), respectively. Normality of residuals for all models was assumed.
Bayesian statistics
All analysis was performed in R using the BayesFactor package (version 0.9.12–4.6). For comparison of means testing, the ttestBF function was used with Jeffreys prior applied to the variance of the normal prior sample (i.e., a minimally informative prior) and a Cauchy prior applied to the effect estimate45,46. For correlation analysis, the regressionBF function was used with the same arrangement of priors. All grey-shaded regions shown in plots represent standard errors. All boxplot elements represent their defaults in ggplot2 (i.e., centre line, median; upper and lower box boundaries, Inter Quartile Range (IQR); upper and lower whiskers, smallest or largest value within 1.5× the corresponding IQR; dots, outliers).
Reporting summary
Further information on research design is available in the Nature Portfolio Reporting Summary linked to this article.
Results
Primary study
As in previous studies47 the BPAQ-SF (max score = 60cf. 33) shows that a convenience sample of mostly undergraduate students scores lower (m = 13.86, sd = 9.97) on trait aggression than the general population. Similarly, as in previous research using the CRTT48,49, the standard experimental measure of aggression (all blast levels, averaged across trials for each participant) did not significantly correlate with participants’ self-reported trait aggression (see Fig. 3a: t(105) = 1.05, p = 0.29, r = 0.10, 95% CI = −0.09–0.29, bf = 0.40). Poor correlations between the BPAQ and standard versions of the CRTT have been widely reported and discussed in the field34,35,50 and we will return to this in the Discussion.
However, our facial affect data (see Fig. 3b) potentially explains the inconsistent CRTT findings in the present literature: prototypic displays of positive affect (happiness20,21,51,52) were far more frequent and intense than prototypic displays of negative affect (anger and disgust, both associated with conflict behaviour20,21,51,52.; mean positive affect = 0.83, SD = 0.77; mean negative affect = 0.12, SD = 0.21; difference: w = 8e + 6, p > 2e-16, r = 0.50, 95% CI = 0.61–0.68, bf = 7e + 484). In other words, participants appear to find the CRTT task to be more fun than upsetting, a pattern also reflected in debrief feedback from participants. High negative emotion was present sufficiently often, however, (16% of trials) that its impact on aggression could be examined using selective averaging procedures, as shown below.
Negative emotion drives aggression and forced breaks reduce it
Using the facial affect data, we isolated trials associated with high levels of positive or negative expressions of emotion, operationally defined as trials where positive or negative expressive power (mean intensity) was greater than one standard deviation above the mean during the blast initiation response time window (blast initiation ±1 s). Within the “immediate response” condition, participants used significantly more “highly” aggressive behaviour (blast levels 6, 7, and 8) on high negative affect trials compared to high positive affect trials (illustrated as a density plot in Fig. 4a: w = 2871, p = 7e-3, r = 0.23, 95% CI = 4e-5–2.0, bf = 5.4). Among trials with high levels of negative affect, we observed significantly lower blast levels in all the “forced break” conditions, compared to the immediate response condition (illustrated in Fig. 4b: w = 8002, p = 5e-4, r = 0.34, 95% CI = 1.0–3.0, bf = 2e + 5). Pairwise comparisons across our three forced break conditions revealed no significant differences in blast levels as a function of delay length (a full breakdown of all forced break conditions is provided in Tables SI3, 4, and 5, and Figure SI1 of the Supplemental Information). Consequently, for simplicity our analysis presented in the main text combines all forced break conditions together. Consistent with the reduction in aggressive responding, analysis shows a positive association between negative expressive intensity during the blast initiation response window (blast initiation ±1 s) and blast level selections in the immediate response condition (t(345) = 6.6, p = 2e-10, r = 0.33, 95% CI = 0.23–0.42, bf = 5e + 7), but not in the forced break condition (t(1201) = 0.5, p = 0.61, r = 0.01, 95% CI = −0.04–0.07, bf = 0.07).
To assess whether forced breaks influenced behaviour more broadly, we also examined the distribution of blasts when positive emotions were expressed. We found no evidence of a reliable difference between the immediate response and forced break conditions on trials with high levels of positive affect, demonstrating the selective nature of the delay effect (Fig. 4c: w = 5180, p = 0.60, r = 0.03, 95% CI = −9e-1 − 8e-7, bf = 0.19). Similarly, we found no evidence that forced breaks elicited a change in behaviour on trials with low levels of negative emotion, where lower blast levels dominated (Fig. 4d: w = 144438, p = 0.93, r = 1e-3, 95% CI = −2e-5–6e-5, bf = 0.08). Finally, we also examined whether there was a reduction in the intensity of the winner’s expressed negative affect in the forced break condition between the time the winner was announced (Time 1) and their blast selection after the forced break (Time 2), testing whether the reduction in aggressive responding (illustrated in Fig. 4b) was actually associated with reduced negative affect. Analysis confirmed a significant reduction in negative emotion over time in the forced break condition (Fig. 4e: w = 24519, p > 2e-16, r = 0.64, 95% CI = 0.28–0.35, bf = 6e + 24), corresponding to the lower blast level selections observed following a forced break. Moreover, there is a significant difference in negativity change between the forced break and immediate response condition (where the T1–T2 difference in the Immediate Response condition comprises the time taken to input a blast level once the winner was announced; Fig. 4f: w = 1274, p = 3e-7, r = 0.18, 95% CI = −0.37 to −0.17, bf = 7e + 29). As well as showing that high levels of negative emotion drive aggressive behaviour within couples, these data confirm that the introduction of a forced break between provocation and the opportunity for aggression mitigates negative affect and results in a significant reduction in aggression.
Further to the above analysis, our data also allows us to examine the impact of couples who, of their own accord, elected to wait longer before choosing a blast level. Our experimental paradigm enforced a minimum break length by blocking blast selection entries for a set period, but participants were not forced to respond immediately after their break was over. As a result, we were able to calculate the amount of extra time participants elected to take after their forced break was over. Analysis of these extra (additional) breaks reveals that voluntarily electing to wait longer than required further reduced both negative expressive intensity (Fig. 4g: t(226) = −4.2, p = 5e-13, r = −0.18, 95% CI = −0.23 to −0.13, bf = 9e + 9) and blast level selections (Fig. 4h: t(226) = −4.5, p = 9e-6, r = −0.29, 95% CI = −0.40 to −0.17, bf = 1802). These results suggest that a break was effective at reducing negative emotionality and aggression regardless of whether it was forced or voluntary.
Negative emotion compounds, increasing aggression within couples
The preceding analysis examined behaviour solely as a function of the intra-personal emotions experienced by the aggressor (the winner of each round). Based on the assumption that inter-personal affective processing also matters, we predicted that negative affective displays would compound, causing aggression to be greater when both partners displayed high levels of negative affect. To test our prediction, the distribution of blast levels in the “immediate response” condition was examined as a function of whether neither, one, or both members of each couple exhibited a high level of negative emotion (see Fig. 5). When neither partner displayed high levels of negative emotion (white) blast levels were generally low. By contrast, there was a significant increase in aggression when either the winner (red: w = 2735.5, p = 5e-5, r = 0.25, 95% CI = −3.0 to −1.0, bf = 1e + 4) or loser (black: w = 5407.5, p = 0.01, r = 0.17, 95% CI = −1.0 to −3e-5, bf = 21.53) displayed a high level of negative emotion, reflecting a clear shift towards more aggressive blasts. Notably, however, aggression levels were similar regardless of which member of the pair expressed negative emotion (w = 1181, p = 0.11, r = 0.15, 95% CI = −2.0 – 7e-5, bf = 0.69; though, as our Bayes Factor indicates, we lack credible evidence for or against the null hypothesis). More importantly, a further increase in blast levels occurred when both players showed high negative affect (burgundy) compared to neither player (w = 1118.5, p = 2e-8, r = 0.34, 95% CI = −4.0 to −2.0, bf = 5e + 8), the loser only (w = 548.5, p = 6e-4, r = 0.35, 95% CI = −3.0 to −1.0, bf = 47.13), and the winner only (w = 455, p = 0.047, r = 0.24, 95% CI = -2.0 – 2.6, bf = 1.01), demonstrating that negative emotion compounds within couples.
The dynamics of aggression reveal retaliation, but not escalation, over time
Based on previous research28 and theory10,28, we predicted that blast levels in later rounds would be higher than blast levels in earlier rounds (indicating escalation over time). Counter to our expectations, analysis of blast levels over time revealed no evidence of escalation (see Fig. 6a; t(1548) = 1.1, p = 0.27, r = 0.03, 95% CI = −0.03–0.12, bf = 0.10). Comparison of grand mean blast level data between partners revealed a significant and large correlation however, indicating that partners tended to match each other’s overall levels of aggression (see Fig. 6b; t(56) = 12.6, p < 2e-16, r = 0.84, 95% CI = 0.78 – 0.91, bf = 5e + 14). Consequently, we further examined the relationship between each blast level selection (round i) and the following blast level selection (Round i + 1) to assess the dynamics of behaviour. Analysis of single-trial blast level data revealed a significant correlation between each blast and the subsequent blast, providing evidence of immediate retaliation (see Fig. 6c; t(750) = 13.3, p < 2e-16, r = 0.44, 95% CI = 0.38–0.49, bf = 7e + 29). Importantly, the pattern of trial level matching can be seen within individual couples, revealing a striking pattern of matching blast levels between individuals over successive rounds of the game despite clear between couple variability. Data from three separate couples are illustrated in Fig. 6d–f (note that ‘win streaks’ were collapsed, as outlined in methods), revealing an oscillating pattern of win–loss for each player.
The presence of matching led us to examine patterns of escalation at a trial level, in addition to the game level described above (and shown to be absent in Fig. 6a). For this, we calculated the difference between each player’s blast selection (blast on round i (Ri)) and their partner’s previous blast selection (blast Ri-1), resulting in escalation/de-escalation coefficients ranging from a possible −7 to 7. We found that participants chose blast levels higher than their opponent’s last selection (i.e., they escalated) on 46% of trials, they de-escalated on 39% of trials, and they matched on 15% (see Fig. 6g). Indeed, within each couple, all three patterns of behaviour were typically visible over time (illustrated in an example couple shown in Fig. 6h). There was, however, notable between-participant variation in escalation tendencies (i.e., many participants never matched their partner exactly, while a small portion matched 75% of the time; as illustrated by the distributions shown in Fig. 6g). Finally, therefore, we also examined the relationship between escalation and de-escalation tendencies within each couple (Fig. 6i), revealing a negative relationship between the escalation tendency of the higher-escalation partner and the lower-escalation partner (t(50) = −12.5, p < 2e-16, r = −0.87, 95% CI = −0.92 to −0.78, bf = 6e + 13). Asymmetry in escalation tendencies within couples suggests that one partner tended to consistently choose blast levels that were higher than their partner’s last selection, while the other partner consistently chose lower blast levels, exemplified by the couple shown in Fig. 6h. Nonetheless, as described above (and illustrated in Fig. 6b, c), even with this escalation tendency asymmetry, most couples still matched each other’s mean blast levels.
Putting it all together: modelling a mechanism
The above analysis has demonstrated that forced breaks reduce negative affect and blast levels (behavioural aggression) under conditions of high negative affect; however, the potential mechanism behind forced breaks can be further illuminated by statistical modelling of the data. Specifically, we employed multi-level, longitudinal Actor-Partner Interdependence Modelling53,54 (APIM) to examine the interplay of both partner’s emotions over time. APIM allows us to assess the impact of the previous winner’s and loser’s negative affect (at Ri-1) on the current winner’s and loser’s negative affect (at Ri), remembering that each player oscillates between winning and losing over time (as described above). Specific model parameters and full statistics for all following models are provided in Table 3.
In the Immediate Response condition, the multi-level longitudinal APIM model (Fig. 7a reveals that the current winner’s negative affect (at Ri) is affected by both their previous negative affect (as a loser, at Ri-1; an “actor effect”; t(326) = 4.6, p < 0.0001, β = 0.29, 95% CI = 0.16 – 0.29) and their partner’s previous negative affect (as the winner/potential aggressor, at Ri-1; a “partner effect”; t(326) = 2.3, p = 0.02, 95% CI = 0.02–0.17), suggesting that the winner of each round is influenced by the negative affect of both players (corroborating the compounding effects shown in Fig. 5). Further, because the model is longitudinal, we expect the pattern to oscillate on each iteration (illustrated with a fading continuation of the model with ellipses), potentially explaining the oscillating pattern of matching shown in Fig. 6d–f. Finally, examination of the impact of both player’s negative affect on blast selection using multi-level regression further corroborates our findings that both partners’ negative affect drives aggression (t(345) = 6.57, p = 1.8e-10, r = 0.33, 95% CI = 0.02–0.42, bf = 4.5e + 7) as shown in Fig. 7b (mean effect shown in red, pair-level illustrated in grey).
We also provide a second model displaying the same relationships for the forced break condition (illustrated in Fig. 7c). As expected, each player’s negative affect at Ri influences their negativity at Ri-1, but there was no significant impact of the Ri-1 loser’s negativity on the Ri winner’s negativity in the Forced Break condition. Given that APIM models are non-Bayesian the null finding must be interpreted as a lack of evidence for an effect, rather than as evidence of the absence of an effect. Nonetheless, Bayesian multi-level regression provides extremely strong evidence that the impact of both partner’s negative affect does not predict higher blast levels in the forced break condition (t(1201) = 0.51, p = 0.61, r = 0.01, 95% CI = −0.04 –0.07, bf = 0.07), as is illustrated in Fig. 7d.
Finally, to further illustrate the potential mechanism of forced breaks, we conducted a simple mediation analysis55,56, to examine the direct and indirect impact of the forced break condition on blast level selection (behavioural aggression) through winner negativity. The mediation analysis (Fig. 8; Table 4) shows that forced breaks (in isolation) are predicted to reduce blast level selections by 0.36 on average (t(1548) = −2.6, p = 0.010, β = −0.36, 95% CI = −0.64 to −0.09) and negativity by 0.11 on average (t(1548)= −8.73, p < 2e-16, β = −0.11, 95% CI = −0.13 to −0.08). When accounting for winner negativity, however, the impact of forced breaks on blast level becomes non-significant (t(1547)= −1.6, p = 0.11, β = −0.23, 95% CI = −0.51–0.05) and negativity solely predicts blast (t(1547) = 4.4, p = 1e-5, β = 1.26, 95% CI = 0.70 – 1,82), suggesting a complete mediation effect (i.e., the impact of forced breaks on blast level is entirely dependent on its impact on negativity).
Replication analysis
Having established a link between the expression of negative emotion and aggressive behaviour using our face-to-face Competitive Reaction Time Task we carried out a follow-on study (as outlined in the methods) that allowed us to ask whether the effects were replicable. As in the primary study, results of the replication provide strong evidence for the alternative hypothesis57,58 (Table 5). First, we show again that forced breaks reduce negativity from time one (when the winner is announced) to time two (when the winner picks a blast level; Fig. 9a; w = 2791, p = 2e-14, r = 0.73, 95% CI = 0.33–0.45, bf = 1.6e + 11). Second, when negativity is high, we see lower blast levels in the forced break condition (Panel b; w = 34585, p = 2.7e-4, r = 0.12, 95% CI = 4.5e-5 – 2.0, bf = 52.8). Third, negativity from both players predicts higher blast levels (in a multi-level model, grey regression lines represent the pair-level; Panel c; t(386) = 3.14, p = 0.002, r = 0.16, 95% CI = 0.06–0.25, bf = 12.4). Fourth, players matched each other’s blast levels closely (shown on the trial level; Panel d; t(20) = 9.25, p = 1.2e-8, r = 0.90, 95% CI = 0.77–0.98, bf = 347396). In sum, therefore, the follow up study provides a clear replication of our original findings.
Finally, we examined the qualitative responses provided in the follow-up study, as they pertain to our participant’s perceptions of the impact of forced breaks on their cognition, emotion, and behaviour. Adopting a simplified thematic analysis approach (cf. Braun and Clarke, 200659, 201360) individual participant responses were assessed independently by AGM to identify common and repeated themes within the comments provided, with additional comparison made across individuals to confirm the consistency of the themes. Initial themes were discussed with all authors, leading to a) further refinement, b) the identification of representative quotes to illustrate each theme, and c) a count of the frequency of each theme occurring within the response data. The quotes and frequency counts are presented in Table 6, highlighting the participants keen awareness of responding to their partner’s behaviour (matching/retaliation) and feeling uncomfortable about doing so (guilt/discomfort). Despite not being asked explicitly to do so, participants also reported awareness of the influence of both their own and their partner’s negativity on their behaviour. Given the qualitative nature of this data we do not present any further statistical analysis or make claims based on it alone, using it solely to inform discussion of our principal findings.
Discussion
We adapted a widely used aggression paradigm (the Competitive Reaction Time Task; CRTT61) for use with couples, allowing aggressive behaviour between partners to be examined experimentally. As predicted by the General Aggression Model10 and the I3 Model11, we showed that aggression within couples is driven by both inter- and intra-personal experiences of negative emotional arousal, confirming that emotional experiences of both partners can influence an aggressor’s actions. Indeed, we observed an 86% increase in mean aggression when both partners were expressing negative emotion compared to when neither partner was doing so. In addition, by examining the dynamic interaction between partners we were able to reveal evidence that couples match each other’s aggression over time. Taken together, therefore, these findings substantiate emotional co-regulation (i.e., partners’ emotional impact on each other) as a central feature of intimate partner conflict—something long suspected23,24,62,63, and partially demonstrated3,20,64,65, but impossible to show with traditional experimental paradigms (i.e., single player or computer opponent versions of the competitive reaction time task).
Most importantly, perhaps, we also showed that the introduction of a short delay (a forced break) between provocation and the opportunity for aggression led to a decrease in aggression. This finding builds on previous research66, highlighting the possibility of developing practical interventions aimed at reducing intimate partner aggression. Although our findings reveal that forced breaks decrease negative emotion and aggression, we did not find evidence that the length of forced break impacted results (i.e., our 5, 10, and 15 s conditions resulted in similar reductions in emotional negativity and aggression). Nonetheless, we were able to show that participants who elected to take extra time before responding benefited from a further decrease in negative emotion and exhibited less aggression. This finding is noteworthy because it shows that breaks do not need to be forced to be effective, suggesting that training individuals to take voluntary breaks may be effective as an aggression reduction strategy.
One obvious difficulty for experimental investigations of conflict between couples is the inherent ethical constraint around causing harm, which has often led to the use of relatively innocuous forms of aggression (such as criticizing another person’s writing, giving a fictitious opponent hot sauce to eat, and assigning difficult puzzles to a competitor)67, many of which have been criticized for poor external validity50. In the present study, we employed a noxious auditory sound blast (available here: https://shorturl.at/ejvUZ) that was designed to be unpleasant, but not harmful. Whilst our noxious sound is quite unlike acts of physical aggression seen in real-world settings, participants reported it as aversive during debriefing, even expressing fear of receiving blasts and guilt after sending them (see Table 6), suggests that delivering sound blasts is a reasonable experimental proxy for aggressive behaviour. In addition, (rather than examining the presence or absence of aggression) we used participants’ choice of blast level (i.e., auditory volume) to provide a variable measure of behavioural aggression.
Here, because we employed a variable measure of aggression, we were able to show that couples closely matched each other’s blast levels in both the grand mean and from trial to trial (i.e., if Player 1 selected a blast level of five, Player 2 was likely to select a blast level near five on their next win). Despite providing evidence of retaliation, one clear feature of our findings is the absence of escalation over time, contrary to claims that a Violence Escalation Cycle occurs when two people are in conflict28. We note, however, that the present results come from a convenience sample of couples (with relatively low levels of trait aggression), and it therefore remains possible that escalation does occur in couples with histories of severe aggression or abuse. Equally, future studies are required to determine whether factors such as the length or seriousness of a relationship, or a previous history of aggressive behaviour, alter the pattern of behaviour reported here. More generally, although the General Aggression Model68,69 suggest that similar forms of aggressive interactions should occur for all kinds of dyadic relationships, it would be interesting to discover whether the present findings replicate or differ with other types of dyads (e.g., friends, siblings, teammates, etc.). Regardless, we found that some individuals had a greater tendency to choose blast levels larger than their partner’s last blast choice, evidence of an inter-personal asymmetry in escalation tendency at a trial level (see Fig. 9g, h, i), though partners still tended to match each other overall (see Fig. 6b, c). An important avenue for future research will be to investigate how asymmetries in the escalation tendencies within a couple relate to the overall severity of conflict that results.
As well as being limited by ethical constraints around the experimental inducement of aggression, previous understandings of couples’ conflict has also been limited by the lack of truly dyadic, face-to-face research. Indeed, studies of intimate partner aggression have typically used questionnaire measures administered individually70, or have employed ‘sham’ dyadic designs in which participants are falsely told they may select difficult yoga poses66 for their partner to hold in a separate room, for instance. Previous studies have approached the present design, bringing couples into a lab to play a fake version of the CRTT in separate rooms (usually they are told that their blasts/shock selections will be delivered to their partner, and vice versa, but they are not)3,65, or bringing strangers into a lab and having them really send each other blasts, but from separate rooms28. By examining couples together, face-to-face and in-person, the present study offers a significant methodological improvement to the standard CRTT, increasing its realism while maintaining its experimental control. Although we employed a modest sample size (53 couples in the primary study and 29 in the replication) our approach also allowed us to maximise power by focusing on trial level data (which gave us thousands of observations for most analyses). Critically, we also employed an innovative and underused approach to the analysis of behaviour (discussed below).
Rather than examining aggressive behaviour in isolation, we recorded participants’ facial expressions, which allowed behaviour to be examined as a function of trial-to-trial changes in emotional arousal, illuminating the dynamics of dyadic affective and behavioural processes during conflict. This allows us to identify and examine both essential components of reactive aggression, behavioural aggression and negative emotional arousal, rather than assuming reactive aggression from behaviour alone (a method used successfully in previous studies71). To our initial surprise, assessment of the emotional expressions elicited during performance of the task highlighted that participants were most often experiencing positive emotion (i.e., happiness) suggesting that they enjoyed their participation. Nonetheless, by combining high time-resolution affect coding and selective averaging procedures, we were able to show that high levels of negative affect (indexed by prototypic displays of anger and disgust20,21,51,52) predict increased levels of aggression, regardless of which member of the couple was expressing the negative emotion.
Given the dyadic nature of conflict, it has long been theorized that an aggressor’s behaviour is influenced both by negative emotions in the aggressor62,63, and by the aggressor’s perception of their target’s emotion10,24,28,62,64,72. Our finding that the emotions of both partners impacts the behaviour of an aggressor is notable because it provides experimental support for this claim (rather than relying on retrospective self-reports or observation), adding to a growing body of evidence highlighting the importance of emotional co-regulation as a driver of aggression3,16,20,23,24,64,72. For example, two recent studies using face-to-face dyadic designs found that partners’ negative emotional expressions correlated with each other23 and with aggressive behaviour during conflict20. The present findings are distinctive, however, in showing that, as well as compounding between individuals, aggression is also more likely when only the non-aggressing member of a couple expresses negative emotion. One implication of this finding is that an individuals’ aggressive behaviour can be influenced by the emotional expression of those they interact with. For example, if the compounding of emotion seen here extends beyond dyadic interactions to larger social groupings, the present results may help explain why violence can erupt so readily within crowds73,74,75—where an individual may be simultaneously exposed to multiple expressions of negative emotion. We stress, however, that within the context of intimate partner aggression, it is important not to misinterpret our findings as suggesting that a victim/target of aggression is responsible for the behaviour of the aggressor (cf. victim blaming76). Nonetheless, a co-regulatory account of intimate partner aggression does suggest that each partner within a couple will influence the other. In particular, the current findings suggest that effectively regulating one’s own negative emotion should be an effective strategy for managing conflict in both one-self and one’s partner.
More broadly, the present findings confirm that it is possible to reduce aggression within intimate dyadic interactions. Here, we used short involuntary time delays (of 5, 10, or 15 s) to influence behaviour, revealing a marked reduction in negative expression and aggressive behaviour following forced breaks (of all lengths), and we were able to replicate these findings in a follow-on study. Within the theoretical aggression literature10,11, it has been posited that both inter- and intra- personal affective processes, as well as impulsivity3,8, impact aggression. Based on the current findings we propose that forced breaks reduce aggression by first blocking the expression of impulsive behavioural aggression while participants are in a provoked state. Then, because negative affect reduces over time, when a response is expressed, it will tend to be less aggressive (assuming no further provocation has occurred). We expect, of course, that a range of other factors, including individual traits and situational variables (such as the couple’s escalation/de-escalation asymmetry), will also shape the outcomes of couples’ conflict. Nonetheless, in practice, our results provide strong evidence for the role of impulsivity and negative affect in intimate partner aggression, at least during the kind of short-term, acute conflict present in our experiment.
Limitations
Although we believe that the findings of the present studies are exciting, it is important to recognise limitations. As mentioned, because we only studied couples in the experiments reported here, we don’t know how these results would generalise to other forms of dyad, such as friends or team-mates. Fortunately, future studies using our Face-To-Face version of the CRTT can examine how factors such as the type of relationship or the gender of each partner influences aggression within couples. We also used a convenience sample of mostly undergraduates, who, as expected, scored relatively low on our measure of trait aggression (and although our follow-up study contained a larger age range it was still limited in its diversity). Relatedly, use of undergraduates also defined the nature and length of the relationships between our couples. Future work could usefully examine couples more broadly (e.g., longer-term couples, couples who are married or co-habiting, etc.).
A common critique of the CRTT is that there is no non-aggressive response option. Participants must give a sound blast, which makes it difficult to infer aggressive intent from the use of a sound blast. We chose our response format based on recommendations from previous work35, and while participants could not choose “no response”, in practice they did use the full range of blasts available to choose how much discomfort to inflict. Nonetheless it is important to consider the fact that when inflicting discomfort is facilitated (by design) by an experimental setup, it limits the extent to which we can make claims about “real-world” aggression. In addition, it could also be valuable to include a trait measure of negative urgency in future studies on forced breaks, as variability in person level variables between participants might moderate the effect of forced breaks on negative affect and aggression. Equally, it is important to recognise that intimate partner aggression is not a unitary phenomenon and our focus on the role of negative emotion and impulsivity means that our results can only speak to some forms of aggression between partners. Specifically, forced breaks can only be effective in reducing reactive aggression. Forced breaks should not be expected to have any influence on the systematic, premeditated forms of abuse used to exert long-term control over a partner.
Conclusion
The present study involved an innovative task design, alongside physiological and behavioural measures, and focused trial-level analyses, which together allowed us to demonstrate that forced breaks can successfully reduce aggressive behaviour between couples. This finding is noteworthy for highlighting a potential route to intervention, even for couples where both partners express high levels of negative affect. For example, practitioners providing support for couples may be able to train couples in the use of short breaks as a technique for managing conflict. As well as investigating the possible application of forced breaks in applied contexts, one important aim for future research will be to assess whether fully voluntary breaks (i.e., internally counting to ten) in isolation are as effective as forced breaks (i.e., an externally imposed delay) at reducing negative urgency and aggression. Fundamentally, therefore, we are optimistic about the implications of the current findings. Experimental research involving couples can reveal the complex dyadic processes influencing intimate partner aggression20,65,71,77, adding to a growing awareness of the importance of negative emotional arousal and emotional co-regulation as key drivers of aggression, knowledge that should ultimately contribute to the development of effective interventions.
Data availability
De-identified data available at https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.12936112. Raw video/audio data is not available to protect participant anonymity.
Code availability
Data were analyzed in R and the analysis code is available along with the data at the DOI provided above. Custom code to run the experiment was written in Python and is available on GitHub (https://github.com/AnnahGrace01/FTF-CRTT), and archived at https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.12797202. FACS coding was run through the terminal using OpenFace; detailed information and instructions for running OpenFace can be found on the OpenFace GitHub page.
References
Chan, K. L., Straus, M. A., Brownridge, D. A., Tiwari, A. & Leung, W. C. Prevalence of dating partner violence and suicidal ideation among male and female university students worldwide. J. Midwifery Womens Health 53, 529–537 (2008).
Birkley, E. L. & Eckhardt, C. I. Effects of instigation, anger, and emotion regulation on intimate partner aggression: examination of “perfect storm” theory. Psychol. Violence 9, 186–195 (2019).
Bresin, K. Impulsivity and aggression: a meta-analysis using the UPPS model of impulsivity. Aggress. Violent. Behav. 48, 124–140 (2019).
Hoaken, P. N. S., Shaughnessy, V. K. & Pihl, R. O. Executive cognitive functioning and aggression: Is it an issue of impulsivity? Aggress. Behav. 29, 15–30 (2003).
Blake, K. R., Hopkins, R. E., Sprunger, J. G., Eckhardt, C. I. & Denson, T. F. Relationship quality and cognitive reappraisal moderate the effects of negative urgency on behavioral inclinations toward aggression and intimate partner violence. Psychol. Violence 8, 218–228 (2018).
Chester, D. S. et al. How do negative emotions impair self-control? A neural model of negative urgency. NeuroImage 132, 43–50 (2016).
Gagnon, J. & Rochat, L. Relationships between hostile attribution bias, negative urgency, and reactive aggression. J. Individ. Differ. 38, 211–219 (2017).
Hahn, A. M., Simons, R. M., Simons, J. S. & Welker, L. E. Prediction of verbal and physical aggression among young adults: a path analysis of alexithymia, impulsivity, and aggression. Psychiatry Res 273, 653–656 (2019).
Alexander, E. F. & Johnson, M. D. On categorizing intimate partner violence: a systematic review of exploratory clustering and classification studies. J. Fam. Psychol. 37, 743–752 (2023).
Anderson, C. A. & Bushman, B. J. Human aggression. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 53, 27–51 (2002).
Finkel, E. J. Impelling and inhibiting forces in the perpetration of intimate partner violence: Rev. Gen. Psychol. 11, 193–207 (2007).
Berke, D. S., Reidy, D. E., Gentile, B. & Zeichner, A. Masculine discrepancy stress, emotion-regulation difficulties, and intimate partner violence. J. Interpers. Violence 34, 1163–1182 (2019).
Brem, M. J., Stuart, G. L., Cornelius, T. L. & Shorey, R. C. A longitudinal examination of alcohol problems and cyber, psychological, and physical dating abuse: the moderating role of emotion dysregulation. J. Interpers. Violence https://doi.org/10.1177/0886260519876029 (2021).
Maloney, M. A., Eckhardt, C. I. & Oesterle, D. W. Emotion regulation and intimate partner violence perpetration: a meta-analysis. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 100, 102238 (2023).
Navas-Casado, M. L., García-Sancho, E. & Salguero, J. M. Associations between maladaptive and adaptive emotion regulation strategies and aggressive behavior: a systematic review. Aggress. Violent Behav. 71, 101845 (2023).
Neilson, E. C., Gulati, N. K., Stappenbeck, C. A., George, W. H. & Davis, K. C. Emotion regulation and intimate partner violence perpetration in undergraduate samples: a review of the literature. Trauma Violence Abuse 24, 576–596 (2023).
Bell, K. M. & Higgins, L. The impact of childhood emotional abuse and experiential avoidance on maladaptive problem solving and intimate partner violence. Behav. Sci. 5, 154–175 (2015).
Bliton, C. F. et al. Emotion dysregulation, gender, and intimate partner violence perpetration: an exploratory study in college students. J. Fam. Violence 31, 371–377 (2016).
Wascher, C. A. F. Heart rate as a measure of emotional arousal in evolutionary biology. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 376, 20200479 (2021).
Slep, A. M., Heyman, R. E., Lorber, M. F., Tiberio, S. S. & Casillas, K. L. Couples’ anger dynamics during conflict: interpersonal anger regulation, relationship satisfaction, and intimate partner violence. Fam. Process 60, 1280–1294 (2021).
Bondü, R. & Richter, P. Interrelations of justice, rejection, provocation, and moral disgust sensitivity and their links with the hostile attribution bias, trait anger, and aggression. Front. Psychol. 7, 795 (2016).
Haidt, J. The moral emotions. Handbook of Affective Sciences 852–870 (Oxford University Press, 2003).
Ben-Naim, S., Hirschberger, G., Ein-Dor, T. & Mikulincer, M. An experimental study of emotion regulation during relationship conflict interactions: the moderating role of attachment orientations. Emot. Wash. DC 13, 506–519 (2013).
Lee, K. D. M., Rodriguez, L. M., Edwards, K. M. & Neal, A. M. Emotional dysregulation and intimate partner violence: a dyadic perspective. Psychol. Violence 10, 162–171 (2020).
Bates, E. A. Current controversies within intimate partner violence: overlooking bidirectional violence. J. Fam. Violence 31, 937–940 (2016).
Holmes, S. C. et al. Prevalence and predictors of bidirectional violence in survivors of intimate partner violence residing at shelters. J. Interpers. Violence 34, 3492–3515 (2019).
Langhinrichsen-Rohling, J., Misra, T. A., Selwyn, C. & Rohling, M. L. Rates of bidirectional versus unidirectional intimate partner violence across samples, sexual orientations, and race/ethnicities: a comprehensive review. Partn. Abuse 3, 199–230 (2012).
Anderson, C. A., Buckley, K. E. & Carnagey, N. L. Creating your own hostile environment: a laboratory examination of trait aggressiveness and the violence escalation cycle. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 34, 462–473 (2008).
Lobbestael, J. et al. Toward a more valid assessment of behavioral aggression: an open source platform and an empirically derived scoring method for using the Competitive Reaction Time Task (CRTT). Assessment 28, 1065–1079 (2021).
Baltrusaitis, T., Zadeh, A., Lim, Y. C. & Morency, L.-P. OpenFace 2.0: Facial Behavior Analysis Toolkit. In Proc. 13th IEEE International Conference on Automatic Face & Gesture Recognition (FG 2018) 59–66 https://doi.org/10.1109/FG.2018.00019 (2018).
Ekman, P. & Rosenberg, E. L. What the Face Reveals: Basic and Applied Studies of Spontaneous Expression Using the Facial Action Coding System (FACS). (Oxford University Press, 1997).
Tiddeman, B. P. Blending Textured Images Using a Non-parametric Multiscale MRF Method (2004).
Buss, A. H. & Perry, M. The aggression questionnaire. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 63, 452–459 (1992).
Warburton, W. A. & Bushman, B. J. The competitive reaction time task: the development and scientific utility of a flexible laboratory aggression paradigm. Aggress. Behav. 45, 389–396 (2019).
Elson, M., Mohseni, M. R., Breuer, J., Scharkow, M. & Quandt, T. Press CRTT to measure aggressive behavior: the unstandardized use of the competitive reaction time task in aggression research. Psychol. Assess. 26, 419–432 (2014).
Ekman, P., Levenson, R. W. & Friesen, W. V. Autonomic nervous system activity distinguishes among emotions. Science 221, 1208–1210 (1983).
Ekman, P. & Friesen, W. V. Facial action coding system. https://doi.org/10.1037/t27734-000 (1978).
Amos, B., Ludwiczuk, B. & Satyanarayanan, M. OpenFace: a general-purpose face recognition library with mobile applications (2016).
R Core Team (2013).
van Rossum, G. Python Reference Manual (1995).
Cronbach, L. J. Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of tests. Psychometrika 16, 297–334 (1951).
Tavakol, M. & Dennick, R. Making sense of Cronbach’s alpha. Int. J. Med. Educ. 2, 53–55 (2011).
Taber, K. S. The use of Cronbach’s alpha when developing and reporting research instruments in science education. Res. Sci. Educ. 48, 1273–1296 (2018).
Bakdash, J. Z. & Marusich, L. R. Repeated measures correlation. Front. Psychol. 8, 456 (2017).
Jeffreys, H. An invariant form for the prior probability in estimation problems. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. Math. Phys. Sci. 186, 453–461 (1997).
Barnett, V. D. Order statistics estimators of the location of the Cauchy distribution. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 61, 1205–1218 (1966).
Leonard, K. E., Quigley, B. M. & Collins, R. L. Physical aggression in the lives of young adults: prevalence, location, and severity among college and community samples. J. Interpers. Violence 17, 533–550 (2002).
Bertsch, K. et al. Abnormal processing of interpersonal cues during an aggressive encounter in women with borderline personality disorder: neural and behavioral findings. 493–506 https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000756 (2022).
Krahé, B. et al. Desensitization to media violence: links with habitual media violence exposure, aggressive cognitions, and aggressive behavior. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 100, 630–646 (2011).
McCarthy, R. J. & Elson, M. A conceptual review of lab-based aggression paradigms. Collabra Psychol. 4, 4 (2018).
Fan, L., Molho, C., Kupfer, T. R., Sauter, D. A. & Tybur, J. M. Beyond outrage: observers anticipate different behaviors from expressors of anger versus disgust. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 19485506231176954 https://doi.org/10.1177/19485506231176954. (2023).
Izard, C. E. Anger, disgust, and contempt and their relationship to hostility and aggression. In Human Emotions (ed. Izard, C. E.) 329–354 https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4899-2209-0_13 (Springer, 1977).
Kenny, D. A. Reflections on the actor–partner interdependence model. Pers. Relatsh. 25, 160–170 (2018).
Cook, W. L. & Kenny, D. A. The Actor–Partner Interdependence Model: a model of bidirectional effects in developmental studies. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 29, 101–109 (2005).
Chi, W. E. et al. A Practical guide to causal mediation analysis: illustration with a comprehensive college transition program and nonprogram peer and faculty interactions. Front. Educ. 7, 886722 (2022).
Gunzler, D., Chen, T., Wu, P. & Zhang, H. Introduction to mediation analysis with structural equation modeling. Shanghai Arch. Psychiatry 25, 390–394 (2013).
Wagenmakers, E.-J., Wetzels, R., Borsboom, D. & Van Der Maas, H. L. J. Why psychologists must change the way they analyze their data: the case of psi: Comment on Bem (2011). J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 100, 426–432 (2011).
Jeffreys, H. Theory of Probability (Clarendon Press, 1961).
Braun, V. & Clarke, V. Using thematic analysis in psychology. Qual. Res. Psychol. 3, 77–101 (2006).
Braun, V. Successful Qualitative Research: A Practical Guide for Beginners (SAGE, 2013).
Giancola, P. R. & Zeichner, A. Construct validity of a competitive reaction-time aggression paradigm. Aggress. Behav. 21, 199–204 (1995).
Caldwell, W., da Estrela, C., MacNeil, S. & Gouin, J.-P. Association between romantic partners’ rumination and couples’ conflict is moderated by respiratory sinus arrhythmia. J. Fam. Psychol. 33, 640–648 (2019).
Halmos, M. B., Leone, R. M., Parrott, D. J. & Eckhardt, C. I. Relationship dissatisfaction, emotion regulation, and physical intimate partner aggression in heavy-drinking, conflict-prone couples: a dyadic analysis. J. Interpers. Violence 36, NP5385–NP5406 (2021).
Tiberio, S. S. & Capaldi, D. M. Couples’ affect dynamics: associations with trait hostility and physical intimate partner violence. Dev. Psychopathol. 31, 1715–1727 (2019).
Eckhardt, C. I. et al. Cognitive and affective mediators of alcohol-facilitated intimate-partner aggression. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 9, 385–402 (2021).
Finkel, E. J., DeWall, C. N., Slotter, E. B., Oaten, M. & Foshee, V. A. Self-regulatory failure and intimate partner violence perpetration. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 97, 483–499 (2009).
Eslea, M. & Ritter, D. Hot sauce, toy guns, and graffiti: A critical account of current laboratory aggression paradigms. Aggress. Behav. 31, 407–419 (2005).
Allen, J. J., Anderson, C. A. & Bushman, B. J. The general aggression model. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 19, 75–80 (2018).
Anderson, C. A. & Carnagey, N. L. Violent evil and the General Aggression Model. In The Social Psychology of Good and Evil 168–192 (The Guilford Press, 2004).
Straus, M. A. Dominance and symmetry in partner violence by male and female university students in 32 nations. Child. Youth Serv. Rev. 30, 252–275 (2008).
Bresin, K., Parrott, D. J., Maner, C. & Eckhardt, C. I. Impulsivity and perpetration of intimate partner aggression: the moderating effects of negative affective state and alcohol. Subst. Use Misuse 57, 1618–1625 (2022).
Pollard, D. L. & Cantos, A. L. Attachment, emotion dysregulation, and physical IPV in predominantly Hispanic, young adult couples. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18, 7241 (2021).
Reicher, S. Collective protest, rioting, and aggression. In Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Communication. https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190228613.013.416 (Oxford University Press, 2017).
Russell, G. W. Personalities in the crowd: those who would escalate a sports riot. Aggress. Behav. 21, 91–100 (1995).
Russell, G. W. Sport riots: a social–psychological review. Aggress. Violent Behav. 9, 353–378 (2004).
Veljanova, P. B. & Irena Colakova. Raising awareness, improving victim safety: exploring the efficacy of proactive domestic and family violence prevention measures. In The Routledge International Handbook of Human Aggression (Routledge, 2018).
Watkins, L. E., DiLillo, D., Hoffman, L. & Templin, J. Do self-control depletion and negative emotion contribute to intimate partner aggression? A lab-based study. Psychol. Violence 5, 35–45 (2015).
Acknowledgements
The project leading to these results has received doctoral student funding from the University of St Andrews. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish or preparation of the manuscript. This project was made possible through help from staff in the IT workshop in the School of Psychology and Neuroscience at the University of St Andrews. Further, data was collected with help from two senior honours undergraduate students: Becky Osmond and Maria Bitar.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
Annah G. McCurry: Conceptualization, Methodology, Data collection, Formal analysis, Writing—original draft, Writing—review & editing, Visualization. Robert C. May: Conceptualization, Methodology, Writing—review & editing, Supervision, Project administration, Funding acquisition. David I. Donaldson: Conceptualization, Methodology, Writing—review & editing, Visualisation, Supervision, Project administration, Funding acquisition.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Peer review
Peer review information
Communications psychology thanks Konrad Bresin and the other, anonymous, reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work. Primary Handling Editors: Antonia Eisenkoeck and Marike Schiffer. A peer review file is available.
Additional information
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
McCurry, A.G., May, R.C. & Donaldson, D.I. Both partners’ negative emotion drives aggression during couples’ conflict. Commun Psychol 2, 73 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1038/s44271-024-00122-4
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s44271-024-00122-4