Abstract
The mechanisms of romantic bonding in humans are largely unknown. Recent research suggests that physiological synchrony between partners is associated with bonding. This study combines an experimental approach with a naturalistic dating setup to test whether the individual differences in social and nonsocial synchrony are interdependent, and linked to romantic attractiveness. In a preregistered online experiment with 144 participants, we discover that inducing physiological synchrony between an actor and an actress determines their attractiveness ratings by participants, indicating that synchrony can increase perceived attraction. In a lab-based naturalistic speed-dating experiment, we quantify in 48 participants the individual tendency for social physiological synchrony, nonsocial sensorimotor synchrony, and romantic attractiveness. We discover that the individual propensity to synchronize in social and nonsocial tasks is correlated. Some individuals synchronize better regardless of partners or tasks, and such Super Synchronizers are rated as more attractive. Altogether, this demonstrates that humans prefer romantic partners who can synchronize.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
Romantic bonding is a central feature of human life. It is associated with well-being1,2,3, physical health4,5,6,7,8,9,10, mental health11,12,13, happiness14, life satisfaction15, development16, and sexual desire17. Yet, the neural and behavioral mechanisms that determine why we are attracted to selected individuals, and not others, are still unknown. From an evolutionary perspective, attractiveness reflects a preference for adaptations that increase survival and reproduction18,19. Accordingly, attractiveness is affected by physical features20,21,22, resources, social position, and strength23,24,25,26.
In addition to such static features of fitness, recent research points out that dynamic interactive features, such as synchrony, may also determine attraction in humans. We and others recently demonstrated that physiological synchrony predicts romantic and sexual attraction between partners during a speed-date27,28. In psychobiology, dyadic synchrony refers to the temporal matching of rhythms in physiology, behavior, or affective states between two partners29. Synchrony between humans has been widely documented across different measures and time scales, including neural function30,31,32,33,34,35, arousal36, respiration37,38,39,40,41, heart rate40,42,43,44,45,46, hormones47,48,49,50, motion34,51,52,53, and behavior46,54,55,56,57. Across domains, physiological synchrony between adults is consistently associated with social cooperation58, romantic satisfaction39, and sexual satisfaction59,60. In addition to adult-adult bonds, synchrony was studied in parents and infants as a mechanism for bonding, attachment, and co-regulation54,61,62,63,64,65,66,67, predicting developmental outcomes and social skills68. Altogether, the ability to synchronize with a partner is a central feature of social behavior.
Despite its key role in social behavior, synchrony is not exclusively a social phenomenon, as synchronization is evident not only between individuals but also within individuals, such as in sensorimotor control69,70,71,72. While social and nonsocial synchrony have been widely studied, it is still unclear whether they share common mechanisms73,74,75,76. Interestingly, recent research discovered that individuals with autism show impaired performance in a sensorimotor synchronization task, where they are requested to adjust a finger tap to synchronize with an external metronome beat71,72, compared to neurotypicals77. Moreover, sensorimotor synchronization was associated with the self-reported score of social function77. Based on this, we hypothesize that the ability for social interactions is rooted in domain-general features of sensorimotor integration, which is key for both social and nonsocial synchrony.
This research aims to answer two key open questions about the role of synchrony in romantic attraction. First, does synchrony have a role in eliciting attraction? Alternatively, synchrony can result from an increased attraction that raises the motivation to synchronize. Such interactions can also be bi-directional. Second, are there individual differences in the ability to synchronize that determine attractiveness? To address these questions, we ran two experiments. The first is an online experiment that tested whether experimentally manipulating the level of synchrony between a man and a woman determines their attractiveness ratings by 144 participants. The second is a speed-dating experiment (Fig. 1), in which participants meet for four speed-dates with different partners in a round-robin setup (48 participants, 85 dates). We quantified in each participant the individual levels of social synchrony, or tendency to synchronize regardless of the partner, by calculating their average electrodermal synchrony with different partners during speed-dates. In addition to social synchrony, we quantified the individual performance of nonsocial synchrony in a finger tapping sensorimotor task. Last, we calculated the individual scores of romantic attractiveness by averaging the attractiveness scores each participant received from their different partners. We tested whether there are Super Synchronizers who can better synchronize regardless of the task or the partner and whether Super Synchronizers are rated as more attractive.
Methods
Online experiment
Sample size and power analysis
Before the experiment, we estimated the target sample size using power analysis (G*Power 3.1.9.4) for repeated measures MANOVA between two different groups, with one independent variable (synchrony) and two dependent variables (attractiveness of the actors, attraction between the actors). The parameters of the power analysis are a power of 0.95 to detect an effect size of Cohen’s f = 0.3 at a standard α = 0.05 error probability rate, with an estimated correlation between repeated measures of r = 0.8. The target sample size was 132 participants (66 participants per condition), and we recruited 160 participants. This experiment was preregistered on August 28, 2022 – https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/H9WYR.
Participants
One hundred sixty participants were recruited via iPanel to participate in the online experiment. Out of these, 16 participants were excluded for not meeting the inclusion criteria (15 reported a diagnosis of a psychiatric disorder, and one exceeded the predefined age range). The final dataset comprises 144 participants (76 women; gender was determined based on participant identification), aged 18–30 (Mean = 24.89, SD = 3.61 years). Participants were randomly assigned to one of the two conditions: watch a high synchrony interaction (73 participants); or watch a low synchrony interaction (71 participants). All participants were Hebrew speakers. Participants were remunerated for their participation. The ethical committee of the Faculty of Social Sciences of the Hebrew University of Jerusalem approved the experiment per relevant guidelines and regulations. Each participant signed an informed consent form before participation.
Procedure
In the first stage of the experiment, participants completed a socio-demographic questionnaire. Then, participants watched a 92-second video of a low or high synchrony interaction between an actor and an actress. After the video, participants provided four attractiveness ratings78,79,80,81: (1) how romantically attractive is the man in the video, (2) how romantically attractive is the woman in the video, (3) the attraction of the man in the video to the woman in the video, (4) the attraction of the woman in the video to the man in the video. At the end of the experiment, participants were asked to rate the perceived behavioral synchrony on the date as a manipulation validation. This question was introduced at the end of the experiment to avoid priming the participants towards the phenomenon of synchrony while rating the attractiveness of the actors. The t-test between the perceived synchrony ratings for the synchronous condition (Mean = 5.863, SD = 2.411) and the non-synchronous condition (Mean = 4.338, SD = 2.443) shows a significant difference between the two conditions (t = −3.769, df = 141.76, p-value < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.628, 95% Confidence Interval = [0.966, 0.291]). Attractiveness and synchrony ratings were rated on a Likert scale (0 is the lowest score, 10 is the highest score) using Qualtrics.
Stimuli
The stimuli included videos of a man and a woman on a date performed by actors. The same actor and actress participated in both conditions (high synchrony interaction; low synchrony interaction), and the manipulated variable in the videos was the level of social synchrony between the actors. This was achieved by directing instructions given to the actors during filming. In the synchronous condition, the actors were directed to increase their physiological synchrony by being more attuned and sensitive to the partner and trying to adapt to them. In the non-synchronous condition, the actors were directed to lower their physiological synchrony by acting independently of their partner, being less affected by them. The videos were controlled for the content of the actors’ conversation, the physical appearance of the actors, the settings (a dedicated room at our lab with a homey arrangement), the actors’ positions, and the length of the videos (92 seconds). The level of synchronization in the videos was confirmed with physiological assessments of electrodermal activity during the interactions. In the high synchrony interaction, the electrodermal synchrony was r = 0.614, compared to r = −0.097 in the low synchrony interaction. To further address potential differences between the two videos, we conducted a post hoc behavioral analysis to characterize the behavioral display of the actors in the two videos (Supplementary Note S1, Supplementary Table S1, and Supplementary Fig. S1).
Physiology
During each interaction, we quantified the electrodermal synchrony between the partners by measuring the electrodermal activity of each person using Empatica E4 wristbands placed on the wrist of the left hand. Electrodermal activity refers to the continuous variation in the electrical characteristics of the skin. Varying numbers of eccrine sweat glands secrete varying amounts of sweat, depending on the degree of sympathetic activation, and as more sweat is being secreted, electrodermal activity increases82,83,84. Typically, electrodermal activity is measured as skin conductance by applying a small, constant voltage to the skin. Skin conductance can be calculated by measuring the current flow through the electrodes, as the voltage is kept constant83,85,86. Electrodermal activity is controlled by the sympathetic nervous system and reflects the level of physiological arousal82,87. Electrodermal activity is a validated and standard measure in psychology82,88. Furthermore, electrodermal activity has been reported to be sensitive to social stimuli and reactive during social interactions89,90, specifically between romantic partners27,28,36, making it a valid autonomic measure for social synchrony in a romantic context.
The wristbands contain an electrodermal activity sensor with a sampling frequency of 4 Hz, resolution of one digit –900 pSiemens, range of 0.01–100 μSiemens, and alternating current (8 Hz frequency) with a maximum peak to peak value of 100 μAmps (at 100 μSiemens) (https://empatica.app.box.com/v/E4-User-Manual). The obtained electrodermal signal was uploaded to the E4 application for Windows after each run, then downloaded from the Empatica website for processing and analysis. Previous studies reported using Empatica E4 wristbands in behavioral experiments that measured electrodermal activity91,92,93. Notably, the E4 wristband is quick to connect, wearable, and wireless, hence it does not interfere with natural behavior in experimental settings, enhancing the ecological validity of the obtained results (https://empatica.app.box.com/v/E4-User-Manual, https://empatica.app.box.com/v/E4-getting-started). The E4 device was validated in a previous dating experiment conducted in our lab28, which was consistent with additional validating studies94,95,96. The preprocessing of the raw data included the temporal alignment of the data from both partners based on a global timestamp (https://empatica.app.box.com/v/E4-User-Manual, https://empatica.app.box.com/v/E4-getting-started). Then, to calculate the electrodermal synchrony between the partners, we applied Pearson Correlation using R (2022.12.0 + 353)28.
Speed-dating experiment
Sample size and power analysis
Power was calculated according to the association between synchrony and romantic attractiveness. Given the multiple sources of random variability in a design, simulations that capitalize on random effects revealed in actual data are the most accurate method of determining the power97. Hence, to estimate our target sample size, we used bootstrapped power calculation on a separate dataset of a previous speed-dating experiment from our lab with 30 participants28. Using bootstrap sampling (n = 10,000) to assess the power of different sample sizes, we found that to reach a statistical power of 95%, a sample size of 48 participants is needed. Forty-eight participants were recruited, out of which physiological data were available for 32 participants (64 dates), with a statistical power of 81% in the analyses that include electrodermal synchrony. This experiment was not preregistered.
Participants
Forty-eight students (24 men and 24 women; gender was determined based on participant identification), aged 19–28 (Mean = 24.72, SD = 1.93 years) participated in the speed-dating experiment. The experiment was conducted over seven runs, where in each run, four men and four women were invited to the lab to meet in a speed-dating round-robin rotation. Of the forty-eight participants, thirty-two completed dates with all four partners. Due to last-minute participant cancellations, ten participants completed dates with three partners and six participants with two partners. Due to equipment malfunction, the physiological data was not successfully collected for runs 5–7 (16 participants; 8 women), and thus, these runs are not included in the physiological analyses. Participants were recruited via social media and the university’s online experiment system. All participants are native Hebrew speakers, not diagnosed with any psychiatric disorder, heterosexual, cis-gender, single, and interested in a romantic relationship. The ethical committee of the Faculty of Social Sciences of the Hebrew University of Jerusalem approved the experiment per relevant guidelines and regulations. Each participant signed an informed consent before participating and was remunerated for their participation.
Procedure
Men and women met in a speed-dating round-robin rotation. Each date lasted five minutes and took place in a dedicated room with a homey atmosphere. At the beginning of each date, participants reported their initial interest by rating their interest in succeeding on this specific date on a scale of 1–5, where one is not interested and five is highly interested. Immediately after each date, participants rated their post-date attraction to the partner. We recorded the participants’ electrodermal activity during the dates.
After the dates, participants completed a paced finger tapping task. The participants were asked to synchronize to the beat of an external metronome by tapping with their finger on a wooden box71,72,77 (Fig. 2). The task was composed of six blocks, using three protocols (two blocks of each): fixed metronome tempo (2 Hz, beat every 500 ms); tempo alternated (every random 7–13 intervals) between two beats: in one, they differed by 120 ms; in the other, by 180 ms. Each block lasted sixty seconds, with a five-second break before each block (six minutes and thirty seconds total).
Electrodermal synchrony Scores
Synchrony reflects partners’ simultaneous alignment of electrodermal activity, and was calculated for each date as the Pearson Correlation between the two partners’ electrodermal activity during the date using R (2022.12.0 + 353). Given that every participant partook in multiple dates, the electrodermal synchrony scores were averaged per participant across all of their dates to compute an Individual Electrodermal Synchrony Score. Our previous research demonstrated that synchrony during the first two minutes of the date is primarily predictive of the romantic attraction between the participants28. Thus, synchrony was calculated during the first two minutes and across the entire date; see Supplementary Notes S2.1, S2.2, and S2.3 for all electrodermal synchrony measures. Bonferroni correction is applied to control for multiple hypothesis testing.
Romantic attractiveness scores
Attractiveness reflects the attraction ratings that each participant received from their partners. After each date, participants rated their level of attraction to the partner on a scale of 1–5. The romantic attraction ratings of each participant were averaged across all of their dates, computing an individual measure of Individual Romantic Attractiveness Scores. Out of 85 dates, romantic attraction scores were successfully collected on 83 dates. On one date, neither partner completed the attraction questionnaire, and on another date, the man did not complete it.
Sensorimotor synchrony scores
The finger tapping task quantifies how accurately and reliably participants synchronize their finger tap to an auditory metronome beat. Perfectly synchronous behavior means perfect alignment between the auditory beats and the participant’s taps, whereas more significant gaps represent lower synchrony. Each participant is given an Individual Sensorimotor Synchrony Score based on their mean performance score on the task. The score is calculated by averaging the size of the off-beat temporal errors in milliseconds between the auditory beats and participant’s taps (referred to asynchrony) across the 6-minute duration of the task. A perfect performance is scored 0, representing complete sensorimotor synchrony with no off-beat temporal error. In contrast, increased negative values (where the finger taps typically precede the metronome beats) represent poorer sensorimotor synchrony between the auditory stimuli and motor finger taps. Sensorimotor data collection was completed for 44 participants.
Testing the association between the initial interest and electrodermal synchrony during the date
We tested whether initial interest in the partner predicts the rate synchrony during the date. Since this analysis is performed on separate dates, we applied a multilevel model to account for the statistical non-independence of the data points as similar individuals participated on four dates, nested in four different runs28,98,99. The initial interests of both the man and the woman were applied as fixed effects, while accounting for the random effects for the intercept of recurring data from individuals that repeated on different dates and for their nesting in specific runs. To augment the classical statistical inference, we also included Bayesian analyses and computed Jeffreys-Zellener-Siow (JZS) Bayes Factors (BFs). The default prior settings (used by R) were left unchanged. BF01 values around 3 provide weak to moderate support in favor of the null hypothesis100. The analyses include 64 dates between thirty-two participants (16 men, 16 women), aged 19–28 (Mean = 24.8, SD = 2.02 years).
Testing the association between electrodermal synchrony scores and sensorimotor synchrony scores
We used Pearson Correlation to test the association between Electrodermal Synchrony Scores and Sensorimotor Synchrony Scores (both are continuous variables, N = 28 participants (15 men, 13 women), with an age range of 19–28 (Mean = 24.88, SD = 2.1 years)).
Testing the association between romantic attractiveness scores and electrodermal synchrony scores
We tested whether the individual ability of electrodermal synchrony during the date predicts attraction by calculating the correlation between Electrodermal Synchrony Scores and Romantic Attractiveness Scores. We used the Spearman Correlation since the attraction was measured as a rank variable on a scale of one to five. (N = 32 participants (16 men, 16 women), with an age range of 19–28 (Mean = 24.8, SD = 2.02 years)).
Testing the association between romantic attractiveness scores and sensorimotor synchrony scores
We used the Spearman Correlation to test the association between the Romantic Attractiveness Scores and the Sensorimotor Synchrony Scores. The Spearman Correlation is applied since attraction was measured as a categorical variable on a scale of one to five. Forty-four participants (23 men, 21 women), aged 19–28 (Mean = 24.76, SD = 1.97 years), completed both the speed-date and metronome tasks.
Reporting summary
Further information on research design is available in the Nature Portfolio Reporting Summary linked to this article.
Results
Does increasing physiological synchrony increase attraction, and does increased attraction increase synchrony?
While the correlation between physiological synchrony and romantic attraction has been reported by two studies27,28, the direction of the relationship between synchrony and attraction is still unknown. We assessed the extent to which increased physiological synchrony leads to increased romantic attraction, and vice versa – the extent to which increased initial attraction leads to elevated synchronization.
A preregistered online experiment: testing the extent to which manipulating physiological synchrony between an actor and an actress determines their attractiveness ratings by participants
In an online experiment with an experimental design, 144 participants observed a video of a man and a woman actors interacting in either high or low synchrony levels. Based on previous research demonstrating an association between synchrony and attraction27,28, we hypothesized that increasing synchrony between the actors would make participants rate them as more attractive and more attracted to each other.
We applied a repeated measures MANOVA with one independent variable (level of synchrony, high v. low) and two dependent variables (the total attractiveness of the actors and perceived attraction between the actors). As predicted, inducing synchrony significantly increases attraction scores (F (1,142) = 6.073; p = 0.003). Compared to the low-synchrony interaction, the high-synchrony interaction increased both the attractiveness scores of the actors (F (1,142) = 5.034; p = 0.026) and the perceived attraction between the actors (F (1,142) = 11.92; p < 0.001) (Fig. 3). Assumptions on homogeneity of variances, multivariate normality, and normality of residuals were confirmed by Levene’s tests (Actors’ attractiveness: F (1,142) = 2.425, p = 0.122; Actors’ attraction to each other: F (1,142) = 0.443, p = 0.507), the Mardia’s test (Skewness statistic = 7.1, p = 0.131; Kurtosis statistic = 0.841, p = 0.4), and the Shapiro–Wilk test (W = 0.993, p = 0.248), respectively.
Testing the extent to which attraction impacts physiological synchrony
To examine the extent to which initial interest in the partner at the beginning of the date determines the electrodermal synchrony between partners during the date, we tested the association between the initial interest in the partner measured before the partners started to interact, and the electrodermal synchrony during the date.
Multilevel model analysis shows that initial interest in the partner at the beginning of the date is not significantly associated with electrodermal synchrony during the date in both men (β = 0.061; p = 0.44; 95% Confidence Interval = [−0.091, 0.213]; N = 64 dates) and women (β = 0.039; p = 0.513; 95% Confidence Interval = [−0.076, 0.153]; N = 64 dates). Given the null results, we also computed Spearman Correlations and the Jeffreys-Zellener-Siow (JZS) Bayes Factors (BFs), for men (Spearman r = 0.105; p = 0.41; 95% Confidence Interval = [−0.126, 0.335]; BF01 = 2.44; N = 64 dates), for women (Spearman r = 0.059; p = 0.646; 95% Confidence Interval = [−0.207, 0.324]; BF01 = 3.162; N = 64 dates), and for both (Spearman r = 0.128; p = 0.312; 95% Confidence Interval = [−0.129, 0.386]; BF01 = 2.247; N = 64 dates). The results provide anecdotal to moderate support in favor of the null hypothesis, indicating little credible evidence for the effect of initial interest on electrodermal synchrony100.
Summarizing 1a and 1b demonstrates that the level of synchrony determines the level of attractiveness and that there is no credible evidence that initial interest determines the level of synchrony.
Is synchrony interrelated across tasks and a determinant of attractiveness?
Synchrony is correlated across social and nonsocial tasks
To examine the association between social and nonsocial synchrony, we calculated the correlation between the Individual Electrodermal Synchrony Scores (social synchrony during speed-dates) and the Individual Sensorimotor Synchrony Scores (nonsocial synchrony in the finger tapping task). Results indicate that individual scores of social and nonsocial synchrony are significantly correlated (Pearson r = 0.494; p = 0.008; 95% Confidence Interval = [0.148, 0.732]; N = 28) (Fig. 4a), as some individuals tend to have increased synchrony scores regardless of the task or the partner, to which we refer as Super Synchronizers (see Supplementary Note S3, Supplementary Figs. S2 and S3 for statistical characterization of Super Synchronizers). Pearson Correlation is calculated here since both measurements are continuous.
Individuals with increased electrodermal synchrony are rated as more attractive
To examine the extent to which individuals with increased social synchrony are rated as more romantically attractive, we calculated the correlation between the Individual Electrodermal Synchrony Scores and the Individual Romantic Attractiveness Scores. The results show that the individual propensity to physiologically synchronize with partners while dating is correlated with romantic attractiveness (Spearman r = 0.415; p = 0.018; 95% Confidence Interval = [0.08, 0.746]; N = 32) (Fig. 4b). This effect is optimal when computing the electrodermal synchrony for the first two minutes of the date, based on our previous research showing that this time frame is most predictive of romantic attraction28. When computing the electrodermal synchrony across the entire date, the results are Spearman r = 0.342; p = 0.056; 95% Confidence Interval = [−0.015, 0.695]; N = 32. See Supplementary Note S4 for analyses on the date level.
Individuals with increased sensorimotor synchrony are rated as more attractive
To examine the extent to which individuals with better performance in the tapping task are rated as more romantically attractive, we calculated the correlation between the Individual Sensorimotor Synchrony Scores and the Individual Romantic Attractiveness Scores. The results demonstrate that individuals with an improved ability of sensorimotor synchrony are rated as more attractive (Spearman r = 0.413; p = 0.005; 95% Confidence Interval = [0.176, 0.648]; N = 44) (Fig. 4c).
Discussion
Our findings demonstrate that experimentally manipulating the physiological synchrony between two partners as they interact affects their perceived attractiveness: the same actors are rated more attractive by participants when in sync than when not in sync. This suggests that attractiveness can be experimentally affected by synchrony. Moreover, the capacity to synchronize is individually associated in social and nonsocial tasks and predicts the participants’ attractiveness. Some individuals are Super Synchronizers, i.e., demonstrate increased synchrony regardless of the task or the partner, and those individuals are considered more romantically attractive.
The association between electrodermal synchrony and sensorimotor synchrony suggests that social synchrony is rooted in domain-general sensorimotor skills, potentially because both require behavioral adjustment to dynamic sensory input, whether social or nonsocial. Sensorimotor synchrony measured by the finger tapping task requires adjustment of finger movements to an auditory stimulus, and social synchrony measured by electrodermal activity requires adjustment of physiological processes. Notably, both processes are regulated in an anticipatory manner101,102,103. This supports the hypothesis that, like sensorimotor synchrony, social interactions also rely on prediction104,105, perception-action106, and learning106,107,108,109,110,111,112, possibly based on general sensorimotor predictive mechanisms needed to interact73. Interestingly, improved performance in those features is associated with romantic attraction.
There are several possible explanations for why synchrony is attractive. First, physiological synchrony can benefit physiological regulation in close partners28, or- social regulation113,114,115. This hypothesis is supported by evidence showing that physiological synchrony is correlated with improved physiological regulation of different homeostatic systems, including temperature116, immune function117, heart rate64, and affect113,118. Through social regulation, humans can learn to prefer synchronizing partners and to synchronize with others to optimize homeostasis28,30,114,115,119. Second, interacting with Super Synchronizers, who can better detect and adjust to external cues, can result in a more adapted interaction, with immediate physiological and social gains, making synchronous interactions more rewarding and enjoyable114,115,120. Lastly, being a computationally heavy task, the improved ability to synchronize can indicate greater cognitive aptitudes and fitness, which is in line with the ‘handicap’ principle by Amotz and Avishag Zahavi, suggesting that costly signals reflect the high qualification of their owners26, which are considered more physiologically fit, and thus more attractive26. Importantly, such an evolutionary explanation is a speculation and hard to evaluate empirically. Yet, the hypotheses that synchrony is attractive because it is rewarding and improves physiological regulation are empirical questions that can be addressed in future research.
In addition to physiological synchrony27,28,36,37,40, behavioral synchrony is another focus of study in romantic attraction121,122, demonstrating that specific non-verbal behaviors are related to romantic and sexual interest123,124,125,126. Specifically, increased romantic interest is related to faster and more frequent movements, more patterns initiated by the male, longer patterns of behavior, more repetitions of the same patterns124,126, and coupling of body sway127. In addition to romantic interest and attraction28,128, coordinated motion was found to be associated with rapport between teacher and student129, attentiveness in physician-patient interactions51, and intimacy between same-sex strangers53. Such behavioral transactions could serve as a behavioral mechanism that supports physiological synchrony by providing consistent behavioral cues that signal the physiological fluctuations to the partner and enabling an attuned response. Future research is needed to investigate how people regulate their behavior to physiologically synchronize during social interactions, and the extent to which the association between different behavioral patterns and romantic attraction is mediated through social physiological regulation.
Limitations
Our findings show that the individual capacity for synchrony is associated across social and sensorimotor tasks and predicts attractiveness. This raises the hypothesis that synchrony is an individual trait. While synchrony in this study was associated across social and nonsocial tasks, it was not tested longitudinally. Thus, we cannot yet determine its stability across time. Previous longitudinal research on mothers and children shows that dyadic synchrony is stable across time130. Yet, future research is needed to determine whether synchrony is an individual trait, discover its underlying cognitive, behavioral, and neural mechanisms, and assess its social consequences.
Another limitation is the causal inference of synchrony and attraction. This research provides initial evidence that experimentally manipulating synchrony in actors increases their attractiveness ratings by participants. Moreover, increased synchrony during the date precedes increased attraction27,28, while there is no credible evidence that initial interest at the beginning of the date predicts improved synchrony during the date. This suggests that synchrony potentially elicits attraction, and not the consequence of attraction. However, it is important to note that we can still not infer the causal effect of synchrony on actual romantic feelings of partners on dates. This must be addressed in future research that manipulates the level of synchrony during actual dates, examining its short-term effects on the mutual romantic interest of the participants, and on the long-term relationship outcomes. Lastly, future research is needed to investigate the role of synchrony in mate selection in homosexuals.
In summary, this research demonstrates that some individuals are Super Synchronizers regardless of the task or the partner, and Super Synchronizers are more attractive. This suggests that mate selection in humans depends on dynamic interactive features that enable humans to attune their physiological regulation.
Data availability
All data used in the analyses is available at https://osf.io/nzhv6/.
Code availability
The R code (2022.12.0+353) used for the analyses is available at https://osf.io/nzhv6/.
References
Collins, W. A. Relationships and development during adolescence: Interpersonal adaptation to individual change. Personal. Relatsh. 4, 1–14 (1997).
Collins, W. A. More than Myth: The Developmental Significance of Romantic Relationships During Adolescence. J. Res. Adolescence 13, 1–24 (2003).
Johnson, H. D., Kent, A. & Yale, E. Examination of Identity and Romantic Relationship Intimacy Associations with Well-Being in Emerging Adulthood. Identity 12, 296–319 (2012).
Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., Gouin, J.-P. & Hantsoo, L. Close relationships, inflammation, and health. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 33–38 (2010).
Weiss, R. L. & Aved, B. M. Marital satisfaction and depression as predictors of physical health status. J. Consulting Clin. Psychol. 46, 1379–1384 (1978).
Pietromonaco, P. R., Uchino, B. & Dunkel Schetter, C. Close relationship processes and health: Implications of attachment theory for health and disease. Health Psychol. 32, 499–513 (2013).
Powers, S. I., Pietromonaco, P. R., Gunlicks, M. & Sayer, A. Dating couples’ attachment styles and patterns of cortisol reactivity and recovery in response to a relationship conflict. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 90, 613–628 (2006).
Pietromonaco, P. R. & Collins, N. L. Interpersonal mechanisms linking close relationships to health. Am. Psychologist 72, 531–542 (2017).
Pietromonaco, P. R. & Beck, L. A. Adult attachment and physical health. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 25, 115–120 (2019).
Ge, F., Lembke, J. & Pietromonaco, P. R. Intimate Relationships and Physical Health. In The Wiley Encyclopedia of Health Psychology (eds. Paul, R. H., Salminen, L. E., Heaps, J. & Cohen, L. M.) 337–345 (Wiley, 2020). https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119057840.ch83.
Braithwaite, S. & Holt-Lunstad, J. Romantic relationships and mental health. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 13, 120–125 (2017).
Braithwaite, S. R., Delevi, R. & Fincham, F. D. Romantic relationships and the physical and mental health of college students. Personal. Relatsh. 17, 1–12 (2010).
Gove, W. R., Hughes, M. & Style, C. B. Does marriage have positive effects on the psychological well-being of the individual? J. Health Soc. Behav. 24, 122–131 (1983).
Demir, M. Sweetheart, you really make me happy: romantic relationship quality and personality as predictors of happiness among emerging adults. J. Happiness Stud. 9, 257–277 (2008).
Beckmeyer, J. J. & Cromwell, S. Romantic Relationship Status and Emerging Adult Well-Being: Accounting for Romantic Relationship Interest. Emerg. Adulthood 7, 304–308 (2019).
Collins, W. A., Welsh, D. P. & Furman, W. Adolescent Romantic Relationships. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 60, 631–652 (2009).
Van Lankveld, J., Jacobs, N., Thewissen, V., Dewitte, M. & Verboon, P. The associations of intimacy and sexuality in daily life: Temporal dynamics and gender effects within romantic relationships. J. Soc. Personal. Relatsh. 35, 557–576 (2018).
Darwin, C. 1981. The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex. 2 vols. (Facsimile, 1871).
Rhodes, G., Simmons, L. W. & Peters, M. Attractiveness and sexual behavior: Does attractiveness enhance mating success? Evol. Hum. Behav. 26, 186–201 (2005).
Borelli, C. & Berneburg, M. “Beauty lies in the eye of the beholder”? Aspects of beauty and attractiveness. JDDG J. Dtsch. Dermatologischen Ges. 8, 326–330 (2009).
Fisher, A. N. & Stinson, D. A. Ambivalent attraction: Beauty determines whether men romantically desire or dismiss high status women. Personal. Individ. Differences 154, 109681 (2020).
Mueser, K. T., Grau, B. W., Sussman, S. & Rosen, A. J. You’re only as pretty as you feel: Facial expression as a determinant of physical attractiveness. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 46, 469–478 (1984).
Buss, D. M. Sex differences in human mate preferences: Evolutionary hypotheses tested in 37 cultures. Behav. Brain Sci. 12, 1–14 (1989).
Buunk, B. P., Dijkstra, P., Fetchenhauer, D. & Kenrick, D. T. Age and Gender Differences in Mate Selection Criteria for Various Involvement Levels. Personal. Relatsh. 9, 271–278 (2002).
Sprecher, S., Sullivan, Q. & Hatfield, E. Mate selection preferences: Gender differences examined in a national sample. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 66, 1074–1080 (1994).
Zahavi, A. & Zahavi, A. The Handicap Principle: A Missing Piece of Darwin’s Puzzle (Oxford University Press, 1999).
Prochazkova, E., Sjak-Shie, E., Behrens, F., Lindh, D. & Kret, M. E. Physiological synchrony is associated with attraction in a blind date setting. Nat. Hum. Behav. 6, 269–278 (2021).
Zeevi, L. et al. Bio-behavioral synchrony is a potential mechanism for mate selection in humans. Sci. Rep. 12, 4786 (2022).
Feldman, R. Parent?infant synchrony and the construction of shared timing; physiological precursors, developmental outcomes, and risk conditions. J. Child Psychol. Psychiat 48, 329–354 (2007).
Goldstein, P., Weissman-Fogel, I., Dumas, G. & Shamay-Tsoory, S. G. Brain-to-brain coupling during handholding is associated with pain reduction. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 115, E2528–E2537 (2018).
Azhari, A. et al. Physical presence of spouse enhances brain-to-brain synchrony in co-parenting couples. Sci. Rep. 10, 7569 (2020).
Kinreich, S., Djalovski, A., Kraus, L., Louzoun, Y. & Feldman, R. Brain-to-Brain Synchrony during Naturalistic Social Interactions. Sci. Rep. 7, 17060 (2017).
Liu, N. et al. NIRS-based hyperscanning reveals inter-brain neural synchronization during cooperative Jenga game with face-to-face communication. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10, 82 (2016).
McNaughton, K. A. & Redcay, E. Interpersonal synchrony in autism. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 22, 1–11 (2020).
Mu, Y., Guo, C. & Han, S. Oxytocin enhances inter-brain synchrony during social coordination in male adults. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 11, 1882–1893 (2016).
Coutinho, J. et al. Psychophysiological Synchrony During Verbal Interaction in Romantic Relationships. Fam. Proc. 58, 716–733 (2019).
Chatel-Goldman, J., Congedo, M., Jutten, C. & Schwartz, J.-L. Touch increases autonomic coupling between romantic partners. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 8, 95 (2014).
Goldstein, P., Weissman-Fogel, I. & Shamay-Tsoory, S. G. The role of touch in regulating inter-partner physiological coupling during empathy for pain. Sci. Rep. 7, 3252 (2017).
Helm, J. L., Sbarra, D. A. & Ferrer, E. Coregulation of respiratory sinus arrhythmia in adult romantic partners. Emotion 14, 522–531 (2014).
Danyluck, C. & Page-Gould, E. Social and Physiological Context can Affect the Meaning of Physiological Synchrony. Sci. Rep. 9, 8222 (2019).
McFarland, D. H., Fortin, A. J. & Polka, L. Physiological measures of mother–infant interactional synchrony. Dev. Psychobiol. 62, 50–61 (2020).
Bizzego, A. et al. Strangers, Friends, and Lovers Show Different Physiological Synchrony in Different Emotional States. Behav. Sci. 10, 11 (2019).
Mitkidis, P., McGraw, J. J., Roepstorff, A. & Wallot, S. Building trust: Heart rate synchrony and arousal during joint action increased by public goods game. Physiol. Behav. 149, 101–106 (2015).
Tschacher, W. & Meier, D. Physiological synchrony in psychotherapy sessions. Psychother. Res. 30, 558–573 (2020).
Vanutelli, M. E., Gatti, L., Angioletti, L. & Balconi, M. Affective Synchrony and Autonomic Coupling during Cooperation: A Hyperscanning Study. BioMed. Res. Int. 2017, 1–9 (2017).
Suveg, C., Shaffer, A. & Davis, M. Family stress moderates relations between physiological and behavioral synchrony and child self-regulation in mother-preschooler dyads: Physiological Synchrony in Mother-Child Dyads. Dev. Psychobiol. 58, 83–97 (2016).
Pauly, T., Gerstorf, D., Ashe, M. C., Madden, K. M. & Hoppmann, C. A. You’re under my skin: Long-term relationship and health correlates of cortisol synchrony in older couples. J. Fam. Psychol. 35, 69–79 (2021).
Pauly, T. et al. Cortisol Synchrony in Older Couples: Daily Socioemotional Correlates and Interpersonal Differences. Psychosom. Med. 82, 669–677 (2020).
Saxbe, D. E. et al. Fathers’ decline in testosterone and synchrony with partner testosterone during pregnancy predicts greater postpartum relationship investment. Hormones Behav. 90, 39–47 (2017).
Zilcha-Mano, S., Goldstein, P., Dolev-Amit, T. & Shamay-Tsoory, S. Oxytocin synchrony between patients and therapists as a mechanism underlying effective psychotherapy for depression. J. Consulting Clin. Psychol. 89, 49–57 (2021).
Hart, Y. et al. Automated video analysis of non-verbal communication in a medical setting. Front. Psychol. 7, 1130 (2016).
Latif, N., Barbosa, A. V., Vatiokiotis-Bateson, E., Castelhano, M. S. & Munhall, K. G. Movement Coordination during Conversation. PLoS One 9, e105036 (2014).
Sharon-David, H., Mizrahi, M., Rinott, M., Golland, Y. & Birnbaum, G. E. Being on the same wavelength: Behavioral synchrony between partners and its influence on the experience of intimacy. J. Soc. Personal. Relatsh. 36, 2983–3008 (2019).
Atzil, S., Hendler, T. & Feldman, R. Specifying the Neurobiological Basis of Human Attachment: Brain, Hormones, and Behavior in Synchronous and Intrusive Mothers. Neuropsychopharmacology 36, 2603–2615 (2011).
Abu Salih, M. et al. Evidence for cultural differences in affect during mother–infant interactions. Sci. Rep. 13, 4831 (2023).
Cote, L. R. & Bornstein, M. H. Synchrony in mother-infant vocal interactions revealed through timed event sequences. Infant Behav. Dev. 64, 101599 (2021).
Waters, S. F. & Mendes, W. B. Physiological and Relational Predictors of Mother-Infant Behavioral Coordination. Adapt. Hum. Behav. Physiol. 2, 298–310 (2016).
Behrens, F. et al. Physiological synchrony is associated with cooperative success in real-life interactions. Sci. Rep. 10, 19609 (2020).
Freihart, B. K. & Meston, C. M. Preliminary Evidence for a Relationship Between Physiological Synchrony and Sexual Satisfaction in Opposite-Sex Couples. J. Sex. Med. 16, 2000–2010 (2019).
Freihart, B., Browman, K. & Meston, C. (077) Physiological Synchrony and Sexual Satisfaction: A Replication and Extension. J. Sex. Med. 20, qdad061.073 (2023).
Atzil, S. et al. Dopamine in the medial amygdala network mediates human bonding. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 114, 2361–2366 (2017).
Feldman, R. Parent-infant synchrony: A biobehavioral model of mutual influences in the formation of affiliative bonds. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 77, 42–51 (2012).
Bernieri, F. J., Reznick, J. S. & Rosenthal, R. Synchrony, pseudosynchrony, and dissynchrony: Measuring the entrainment process in mother-infant interactions. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 54, 243–253 (1988).
Feldman, R., Magori-Cohen, R., Galili, G., Singer, M. & Louzoun, Y. Mother and infant coordinate heart rhythms through episodes of interaction synchrony. Infant Behav. Dev. 34, 569–577 (2011).
Field, T., Healy, B. T., Goldstein, S. & Guthertz, M. Behavior-state matching and synchrony in mother-infant interactions of nondepressed versus depressed dyads. Dev. Psychol. 26, 7–14 (1990).
Condon, W. S. & Sander, L. W. Neonate Movement Is Synchronized with Adult Speech: Interactional Participation and Language Acquisition. Science 183, 99–101 (1974).
Condon, W. S. The relation of interactional synchrony to cognitive and emotional processes. In The relationship of verbal and nonverbal communication 49–65 (De Gruyter Mouton, 1980).
Feldman, R., Rosenthal, Z. & Eidelman, A. I. Maternal-Preterm Skin-to-Skin Contact Enhances Child Physiologic Organization and Cognitive Control Across the First 10 Years of Life. Biol. Psychiatry 75, 56–64 (2014).
Aschersleben, G. Temporal Control of Movements in Sensorimotor Synchronization. Brain Cognition 48, 66–79 (2002).
Monier, F. & Droit-Volet, S. Development of sensorimotor synchronization abilities: Motor and cognitive components. Child Neuropsychol. 25, 1043–1062 (2019).
Repp, B. H. Sensorimotor synchronization: A review of the tapping literature. Psychonomic Bull. Rev. 12, 969–992 (2005).
Repp, B. H. & Su, Y.-H. Sensorimotor synchronization: A review of recent research (2006–2012). Psychon. Bull. Rev. 20, 403–452 (2013).
Novembre, G., Mitsopoulos, Z. & Keller, P. E. Empathic perspective taking promotes interpersonal coordination through music. Sci. Rep. 9, 12255 (2019).
Tunçgenç, B., Cohen, E. & Fawcett, C. Rock With Me: The Role of Movement Synchrony in Infants’ Social and Nonsocial Choices. Child Dev. 86, 976–984 (2015).
Curtindale, L. M., Bahrick, L. E., Lickliter, R. & Colombo, J. Effects of multimodal synchrony on infant attention and heart rate during events with social and nonsocial stimuli. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 178, 283–294 (2019).
Howard, E. M., Ropar, D., Newport, R. & Tunçgenç, B. Social context facilitates visuomotor synchrony and bonding in children and adults. Sci. Rep. 11, 22869 (2021).
Vishne, G. et al. Impaired Online Error-Correction Disrupts Synchronization to External Events in Autism. bioRxiv, https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.09.28.316828 (2020).
Borkenau, P., Mauer, N., Riemann, R., Spinath, F. M. & Angleitner, A. Thin Slices of Behavior as Cues of Personality and Intelligence. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 86, 599–614 (2004).
Kenny, D. A., Horner, C., Kashy, D. A. & Chu, L. Consensus at zero acquaintance: Replication, behavioral cues, and stability. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 62, 88–97 (1992).
Kramer, R. S., Gottwald, V. M., Dixon, T. A. & Ward, R. Different cues of personality and health from the face and gait of women. Evolut. Psychol. 10, 147470491201000208 (2012).
Place, S. S., Todd, P. M., Penke, L. & Asendorpf, J. B. Humans show mate copying after observing real mate choices. Evol. Hum. Behav. 31, 320–325 (2010).
Dawson, M. E., Schell, A. M. & Filion, D. L. The electrodermal system. In Handbook of psychophysiology. (eds Cacioppo, J. T. et al.) (Cambridge University Press, 2017).
Mendes, W. B. Assessing autonomic nervous system activity. Methods Soc. Neurosci. 118, 21 (2009).
Quigley, K. S. Sympathetic Nervous System. In The Corsini Encyclopedia of Psychology (eds. Weiner, I. B. & Craighead, W. E.) 1–2 (Wiley, 2010). https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470479216.corpsy0969.
Fowles, D. C. et al. Publication Recommendations for Electrodermal Measurements. Psychophysiology 18, 232–239 (1981).
Lykken, D. T. & Venables, P. H. Direct measurement of skin conductance: a proposal for standardization. Psychophysiology 8, 656–672 (1971).
Wallin, B. G. Sympathetic Nerve Activity Underlying Electrodermal and Cardiovascular Reactions in Man. Psychophysiology 18, 470–476 (1981).
Bradley, M. M., Codispoti, M., Cuthbert, B. N. & Lang, P. J. Emotion and motivation I: defensive and appetitive reactions in picture processing. Emotion 1, 276 (2001).
Dittes, J. E. Galvanic skin response as a measure of patient’s reaction to therapist’s permissiveness. J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol. 55, 295–303 (1957).
Levenson, R. W. & Gottman, J. M. Marital interaction: Physiological linkage and affective exchange. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 45, 587–597 (1983).
Curran, M. T., Gordon, J. R., Lin, L., Sridhar, P. K. & Chuang, J. Understanding Digitally-Mediated Empathy: An Exploration of Visual, Narrative, and Biosensory Informational Cues. in Proceedings of the 2019 CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems 1–13 (ACM, Glasgow Scotland Uk, 2019). https://doi.org/10.1145/3290605.3300844.
De Cecco, M. et al. Augmented reality to enhance the clinician’s observation during assessment of daily living activities. in Augmented Reality, Virtual Reality, and Computer Graphics: 4th International Conference, AVR 2017, Ugento, Italy, June 12-15, 2017, Proceedings, Part II 4 3–21 (Springer, 2017).
Jacob, S., Ishimaru, S. & Dengel, A. Interest Detection While Reading Newspaper Articles by Utilizing a Physiological Sensing Wristband. In Proceedings of the 2018 ACM International Joint Conference and 2018 International Symposium on Pervasive and Ubiquitous Computing and Wearable Computers 78–81 (ACM, Singapore Singapore, 2018). https://doi.org/10.1145/3267305.3267666.
Menghini, L. et al. Stressing the accuracy: Wrist‐worn wearable sensor validation over different conditions. Psychophysiology 56, e13441 (2019).
Ollander, S., Godin, C., Campagne, A. & Charbonnier, S. A comparison of wearable and stationary sensors for stress detection. In 2016 IEEE International Conference on systems, man, and Cybernetics (SMC) 004362–004366 (IEEE, 2016).
Van Lier, H. G. et al. A standardized validity assessment protocol for physiological signals from wearable technology: Methodological underpinnings and an application to the E4 biosensor. Behav. Res 52, 607–629 (2020).
Finkel, E. J., Eastwick, P. W. & Reis, H. T. Best research practices in psychology: Illustrating epistemological and pragmatic considerations with the case of relationship science. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 108, 275–297 (2015).
Ackerman, R. A., Kashy, D. A. & Corretti, C. A. A tutorial on analyzing data from speed-dating studies with heterosexual dyads: Analyzing speed-dating data. Pers. Relatsh. 22, 92–110 (2015).
Kenny, D. A. & Kashy, D. A. Dyadic data analysis using multilevel modeling. in Handbook of advanced multilevel analysis 343–378 (Routledge, 2011).
Jeffreys, H. The Theory of Probability (OuP Oxford, 1998).
Pietromonaco, P. R. & Barrett, L. F. Working models of attachment and daily social interactions. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 73, 1409–1423 (1997).
Ariani, G. & Diedrichsen, J. Sequence learning is driven by improvements in motor planning. J. Neurophysiol. 121, 2088–2100 (2019).
Ariani, G., Kordjazi, N., Pruszynski, J. A. & Diedrichsen, J. The Planning Horizon for Movement Sequences. eNeuro 8, ENEURO.0085–21.2021 (2021).
Frith, C. D. & Frith, U. How we predict what other people are going to do. Brain Res. 36, 46 (2006).
Bar, M. The proactive brain: using analogies and associations to generate predictions. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 280–289 (2007).
Knoblich, G. & Sebanz, N. The Social Nature of Perception and Action. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 15, 99–104 (2006).
Swanson, H. L. & Malone, S. Social Skills and Learning Disabilities: A Meta-Analysis of the Literature. Sch. Psychol. Rev. 21, 427–443 (1992).
Broadhead, P. Early Years Play and Learning: Developing Social Skills and Cooperation (Psychology Press, 2004).
Molapour, T. et al. Seven computations of the social brain. Soc. Cognit. Affect. Neurosci. nsab024, https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsab024 (2021).
Olsson, A., Knapska, E. & Lindström, B. The neural and computational systems of social learning. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 21, 197–212 (2020).
Rolf, M., Hanheide, M. & Rohlfing, K. J. Attention via Synchrony: Making Use of Multimodal Cues in Social Learning. IEEE Trans. Auton. Ment. Dev. 1, 55–67 (2009).
Hart, Y., Noy, L., Feniger-Schaal, R., Mayo, A. E. & Alon, U. Individuality and Togetherness in Joint Improvised Motion. PLoS One 9, e87213 (2014).
Feldman, R. Infant-mother and infant-father synchrony: The coregulation of positive arousal. Infant Ment. Health J. 24, 1–23 (2003).
Atzil, S., Gao, W., Fradkin, I. & Barrett, L. F. Growing a social brain. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2, 624–636 (2018).
Atzil, S. & Gendron, M. Bio-behavioral synchrony promotes the development of conceptualized emotions. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 17, 162–169 (2017).
Levin, B. E. Metabolic imprinting: critical impact of the perinatal environment on the regulation of energy homeostasis. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 361, 1107–1121 (2006).
Arrieta, M.-C., Stiemsma, L. T., Amenyogbe, N., Brown, E. M. & Finlay, B. The Intestinal Microbiome in Early Life: Health and Disease. Front. Immunol. 5, 427 (2014).
Feldman, R., Greenbaum, C. W. & Yirmiya, N. Mother–infant affect synchrony as an antecedent of the emergence of self-control. Dev. Psychol. 35, 223–231 (1999).
Pietromonaco, P. R., DeBuse, C. J. & Powers, S. I. Does Attachment Get Under the Skin? Adult Romantic Attachment and Cortisol Responses to Stress. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 22, 63–68 (2013).
Djerassi, M., Ophir, S. & Atzil, S. What Is Social about Autism? The Role of Allostasis-Driven Learning. Brain Sci. 11, 1269 (2021).
Ramseyer, F. T. Motion energy analysis (MEA): A primer on the assessment of motion from video. J. Counseling Psychol. 67, 536–549 (2020).
Cao, Z., Hidalgo, G., Simon, T., Wei, S.-E. & Sheikh, Y. OpenPose: Realtime Multi-Person 2D Pose Estimation Using Part Affinity Fields. IEEE Trans. Pattern Anal. Mach. Intell. 43, 172–186 (2021).
Guerrero, L. K. & Wiedmaier, B. 19 Nonverbal intimacy: affectionate communication, positive involvement behavior, and flirtation. In Nonverbal Communication (eds. Hall, J. A. & Knapp, M. L.) 577–612 (DE GRUYTER, 2013). https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110238150.577.
Grammer, K., Kruck, K. B. & Magnusson, M. S. The courtship dance: Patterns of nonverbal synchronization in opposite-sex encounters. J. Nonverbal Behav. 22, 3–29 (1998).
Grammer, K., Kruck, K., Juette, A. & Fink, B. Non-verbal behavior as courtship signals: the role of control and choice in selecting partners. Evol. Hum. Behav. 21, 371–390 (2000).
Grammer, K., Honda, M., Juette, A. & Schmitt, A. Fuzziness of nonverbal courtship communication unblurred by motion energy detection. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 77, 487–508 (1999).
Chang, A. et al. Body sway predicts romantic interest in speed dating. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 16, 185–192 (2021).
Cappella, J. N. Behavioral and judged coordination in adult informal social interactions: Vocal and kinesic indicators. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 72, 119–131 (1997).
Bernieri, F. J. Coordinated movement and rapport in teacher-student interactions. J. Nonverbal Behav. 12, 120–138 (1988).
Feldman, R. The relational basis of adolescent adjustment: trajectories of mother–child interactive behaviors from infancy to adolescence shape adolescents’ adaptation. Attachment Hum. Dev. 12, 173–192 (2010).
Acknowledgements
The authors thank Yitzchak Yadegari for the graphic illustrations in Figs. 1 and 4, and Keren Kasten for the graphic illustration in Fig. 2. This study was funded by internal funding from the Hebrew University of Jerusalem granted to Dr. Shir Atzil. The funders had no role in planning the study design, collecting the data, performing the data analyses, publishing, or preparing the manuscript.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
Matan Cohen- conceptualization, methodology, analysis, investigation, data curation, validation, visualization, writing. Maayan Abargil- methodology, validation. Merav Ahissar- conceptualization, methodology, writing. Shir Atzil- conceptualization, methodology, analysis, writing, funding, supervision.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Peer review
Peer review information
Communications Psychology thanks Yuhang Long and the other, anonymous, reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work. Primary Handling Editors: Yafeng Pan and Jennifer Bellingtier. A peer review file is available.
Additional information
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Cohen, M., Abargil, M., Ahissar, M. et al. Social and nonsocial synchrony are interrelated and romantically attractive. Commun Psychol 2, 57 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1038/s44271-024-00109-1
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s44271-024-00109-1