Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Review Article
  • Published:

Foundations of theory of mind and its development in early childhood

Abstract

Theory of mind is the human conceptual capacity to understand other people as agents who have subjective mental states such as beliefs, desires, and intentions. It is the basis of distinctively human forms of social understanding and interaction that are essential for communication, cooperation, and culture. In this Review, I summarize the current state of research about the emergence and development of theory of mind in early childhood. I describe the typical developmental trajectory and review findings about the cognitive, linguistic, social and neural foundations of theory of mind development. Finally, I review an ongoing debate regarding whether there are different — implicit versus explicit — forms of theory of mind that develop independently, and conclude by providing an outlook on future challenges and perspectives for research in this area.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Fig. 1: Milestones in the development of theory of mind.
Fig. 2: Level I and level II perspective-taking tasks.
Fig. 3: False-belief and related theory of mind tasks.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Perner, J. Understanding the Representational Mind (MIT Press, 1991).

  2. Wellman, H. M. Making Minds: How Theory of Mind Develops (Oxford Univ. Press, 2014).

  3. Rakoczy, H. In defense of a developmental dogma: children acquire propositional attitude folk psychology around age 4. Synthese 194, 689–707 (2017).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Schilbach, L. et al. Toward a second-person neuroscience. Behav. Brain Sci. 36, 393–414 (2013).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Tomasello, M. & Rakoczy, H. What makes human cognition unique? From individual to shared to collective intentionality. Mind Lang. 18, 121–147 (2003).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T. & Moll, H. Understanding and sharing intentions: the ontogeny and phylogeny of cultural cognition. Behav. Brain Sci. 28, 675–735 (2005).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Perner, J. & Lang, B. Development of theory of mind and executive control. Trends Cogn. Sci. 3, 337–344 (1999).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Frankfurt, H. Freedom of the will and the concept of a person. J. Phil. 68, 5–20 (1971).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Peterson, C., Slaughter, V., Moore, C. & Wellman, H. M. Peer social skills and theory of mind in children with autism, deafness, or typical development. Dev. Psychol. 52, 46–57 (2016).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Peterson, C. C., Slaughter, V. & Wellman, H. M. Nimble negotiators: how theory of mind (ToM) interconnects with persuasion skills in children with and without ToM delay. Dev. Psychol. 54, 494–509 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Slaughter, V., Peterson, C. C. & Moore, C. I can talk you into it: theory of mind and persuasion behavior in young children. Dev. Psychol. 49, 227–231 (2013).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Slaughter, V., Imuta, K., Peterson, C. C. & Henry, J. D. Meta-analysis of theory of mind and peer popularity in the preschool and early school years. Child Dev. 86, 1159–1174 (2015).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Imuta, K., Henry, J. D., Slaughter, V., Selcuk, B. & Ruffman, T. Theory of mind and prosocial behavior in childhood: a meta-analytic review. Dev. Psychol. 52, 1192–1205 (2016).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Lecce, S., Caputi, M., Pagnin, A. & Banerjee, R. Theory of mind and school achievement: the mediating role of social competence. Cogn. Dev. 44, 85–97 (2017).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Ding, X. P., Wellman, H. M., Wang, Y., Fu, G. & Lee, K. Theory-of-mind training causes honest young children to lie. Psychol. Sci. 26, 1812–1821 (2015).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Talwar, V. & Lee, K. Social and cognitive correlates of children’s lying behavior. Child Dev. 79, 866–881 (2008).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Gasser, L. & Keller, M. Are the competent the morally good? Perspective taking and moral motivation of children involved in bullying. Soc. Dev. 18, 798–816 (2009).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Johnson, M. in Oxford Handbook of Face Perception (eds Rhodes, G., Calder, A., Johnson, M. & Haxby, J. V.) 3–14 (Oxford Univ. Press, 2012).

  19. Simion, F., Regolin, L. & Bulf, H. A predisposition for biological motion in the newborn baby. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 105, 809–813 (2008).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Rochat, P., Querido, J. G. & Striano, T. Emerging sensitivity to the timing and structure of protoconversation in early infancy. Dev. Psychol. 35, 950–957 (1999).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Meltzoff, A. N. & Moore, M. K. Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science 198, 75–78 (1977).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Oostenbroek, J. et al. Re-evaluating the neonatal imitation hypothesis. Dev.Sci. 22, e12720 (2019).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Oostenbroek, J. et al. Comprehensive longitudinal study challenges the existence of neonatal imitation in humans. Curr. Biol. 26, 1334–1338 (2016).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Davis, J. et al. Does neonatal imitation exist? Insights from a meta-analysis of 336 effect sizes. Persp. Psychol. Sci. 16, 1373–1397 (2021).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Tomasello, M. The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition (Harvard Univ. Press, 1999).

  26. Brooks, R. & Meltzoff, A. The importance of eyes: how infants interpret adult looking behavior. Dev. Psychol. 38, 958–966 (2002).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Woodward, A. L. Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor’s reach. Cognition 69, 1–34 (1998).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Gergely, G. & Csibra, G. Teleological reasoning in infancy: the naive theory of rational action. Trends Cogn. Sci. 7, 287–292 (2003).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Wellman, H. M. in The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Cognitive Development 2nd edn (ed. Goswami, U.) 258–284 (Wiley-Blackwell, 2011).

  30. Sodian, B., Thoermer, C. & Metz, U. Now I see it but you don’t: 14-month-olds can represent another person’s visual perspective. Dev. Sci. 10, 199–204 (2007).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Heider, F. & Simmel, M. An experimental study of apparent behavior. Am. J. Psychol. 57, 243–259 (1944).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Gergely, G. & Csibra, G. Teleological reasoning in infance: the naive theory of rational action. Trends Cogn. Sci. 7, 287–292 (2003).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Warneken, F. & Tomasello, M. Altruistic helping in human infants and young chimpanzees. Science 311, 1301–1303 (2006).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Gergely, G., Bekkering, H. & Kiraly, I. Rational imitation in preverbal infants. Nature 415, 755 (2002).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Carpenter, M., Nagell, K. & Tomasello, M. Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 63, 176 (1998).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T. & Moll, H. Understanding and sharing intentions: the origins of cultural cognition. Behav. Brain Sci. 28, 675–735 (2005).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Flavell, J. H., Everett, B. A., Croft, K. & Flavell, E. R. Young children’s knowledge about visual perception: further evidence for the level 1–level 2 distinction. Dev. Psychol. 17, 99–103 (1981).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Wellman, H. M. & Liu, D. Scaling of theory-of-mind tasks. Child Dev. 75, 523–541 (2004).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Wimmer, H. & Perner, J. Beliefs about beliefs — representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition 13, 103–128 (1983).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Gopnik, A. & Astington, J. W. Children’s understanding of representational change and its relation to the understanding of false belief and the appearance–reality distinction. Child Dev. 59, 26–37 (1988).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Flavell, J. H., Flavell, E. R. & Green, F. L. Development of the appearance–reality distinction. Cogn. Psychol. 15, 95–120 (1983).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Rakoczy, H., Fizke, E., Bergfeld, D. & Schwarz, I. Explicit theory of mind is even more unified than previously assumed: belief ascription and understanding aspectuality emerge together in development. Child Dev. 86, 486–502 (2015).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Perner, J. & Roessler, J. From infants’ to children’s appreciation of belief. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 519–525 (2012).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. Lee, J. Y. S. & Imuta, K. Lying and theory of mind: a meta-analysis. Child Dev. 92, 536–553 (2021).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Perner, J., Mauer, M. C. & Hildenbrand, M. Identity: key to children’s understanding of belief. Science 333, 474–477 (2011).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Perner, J., Huemer, M. & Leahy, B. Mental files and belief: a cognitive theory of how children represent belief and its intentionality. Cognition 145, 77–88 (2015).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  47. Leekam, S. R., Perner, J. L., Healey, L. & Sewell, C. False signs and the non-specificity of theory of mind: evidence that preschoolers have general difficulties in understanding representations. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 26, 485–497 (2008).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Redshaw, J. Does metarepresentation make human mental time travel unique? Wiley Interdisc. Rev. Cogn. Sci. 5, 519–531 (2014).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Redshaw, J. & Suddendorf, T. Temporal junctures in the mind. Trends Cogn. Sci. 24, 52–64 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Perner, J. in The Self in Time: Developmental Perspectives (ed Chris, M. & Karen, L.) 268 (Psychology Press, 2001).

  51. Hadwin, J. & Perner, J. Pleased and surprised: children’s cognitive theory of emotion. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 9, 215–234 (1991).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Apperly, I. in Understanding Other Minds: Perspectives from Developmental Social Neuroscience (eds Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H. & Lombardo, M. V.) 3rd edn, 72–92 (Oxford Univ. Press, 2013).

  53. Blakemore, S.-J. The social brain in adolescence. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 267–277 (2008).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Liddle, B. & Nettle, D. Higher-order theory of mind and social competence in school-age children. J. Cult. Evol. Psychol. 4, 231–244 (2006).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. O’Grady, C., Kliesch, C., Smith, K. & Scott-Phillips, T. C. The ease and extent of recursive mindreading, across implicit and explicit tasks. Evol. Hum. Behav. 36, 313–322 (2015).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Filippova, E. & Astington, J. W. Children’s understanding of social-cognitive and social-communicative aspects of discourse irony. Child Dev. 81, 913–928 (2010).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Happé, F. An advanced test of theory of mind: understanding of story characters’ thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 24, 129–154 (1994).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Zelazo, P. D. The development of conscious control in childhood. Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, 12–17 (2004).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Conway, J. R., Catmur, C. & Bird, G. Understanding individual differences in theory of mind via representation of minds, not mental states. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 26, 798–812 (2019).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  60. Henry, J. D., Phillips, L. H., Ruffman, T. & Bailey, P. E. A meta-analytic review of age differences in theory of mind. Psychol. Aging 28, 826–839 (2013).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. de Villiers, J. G. & de Villiers, P. A. The role of language in theory of mind development. Top. Lang. Disord. 34, 313–328 (2014).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Devine, R. T. & Hughes, C. Family correlates of false belief understanding in early childhood: a meta-analysis. Child Dev. 89, 971–987 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Wade, M. et al. On the relation between theory of mind and executive functioning: a developmental cognitive neuroscience perspective. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 25, 2119–2140 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Carlson, S. M., Mandell, D. J. & Williams, L. Executive function and theory of mind: stability and prediction from ages 2 to 3. Dev. Psychol. 40, 1105–1122 (2004).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Marcovitch, S. et al. A longitudinal assessment of the relation between executive function and theory of mind at 3, 4, and 5 years. Cogn. Dev. 33, 40–55 (2015).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Benson, J. E., Sabbagh, M. A., Carlson, S. M. & Zelazo, P. D. Individual differences in executive functioning predict preschoolers’ improvement from theory-of-mind training. Dev. Psychol. 49, 1615–1627 (2013).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Devine, R. T. & Hughes, C. Relations between false belief understanding and executive function in early childhood: a meta-analysis. Child Dev. 85, 1777–1794 (2014).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Carlson, S. M. & Moses, L. J. Individual differences in inhibitory control and children’s theory of mind. Child Dev. 72, 1032–1053 (2001).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Sabbagh, M., Moses, L. & Shiverick, S. Executive functioning and preschoolers’ understanding of false beliefs, false photographs, and false signs. Child Dev. 77, 1034–1049 (2006).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Fizke, E., Barthel, D., Peters, T. & Rakoczy, H. Executive function plays a role in coordinating different perspectives, particularly when one’s own perspective is involved. Cognition 130, 315–334 (2014).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Rakoczy, H. Executive function and the development of belief–desire psychology. Dev. Sci. 13, 648–661 (2010).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Reed, M., Pien, D. L. & Rothbart, M. K. Inhibitory self-control in preschool children. Merrill Palmer Q. 30, 131–147 (1984).

    Google Scholar 

  73. Milligan, K., Astington, J. W. & Dack, L. A. Language and theory of mind: meta-analysis of the relation between language ability and false-belief understanding. Child Dev. 78, 622–646 (2007).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Hofmann, S. G. et al. Training children’s theory-of-mind: a meta-analysis of controlled studies. Cognition 150, 200–212 (2016).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  75. Lohmann, H. & Tomasello, M. The role of language in the development of false belief understanding: a training study. Child Dev. 74, 1130–1144 (2003).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Yu, C.-L., Stanzione, C. M., Wellman, H. M. & Lederberg, A. R. Theory-of-mind development in young deaf children with early hearing provisions. Psychol. Sci. 32, 109–119 (2021).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Peterson, C. C. & Siegal, M. Representing inner worlds: theory of mind in autistic, deaf, and normal hearing children. Psychol. Sci. 10, 126–129 (1999).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  78. Senghas, A., Kita, S. & Özyürek, A. Children creating core properties of language: evidence from an emerging sign language in Nicaragua. Science 305, 1779–1782 (2004).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  79. Pyers, J. E. & Senghas, A. Language promotes false-belief understanding: evidence from learners of a new sign language. Psychol. Sci. 20, 805–812 (2009).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Newton, A. M. & de Villiers, J. G. Thinking while talking. Psychol. Sci. 18, 574–579 (2007).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  81. Dungan, J. & Saxe, R. Matched false-belief performance during verbal and nonverbal interference. Cogn. Sci. 36, 1148–1156 (2012).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  82. deVilliers, J. G. & deVilliers, P. A. in Children’s Reasoning and the Mind (eds Mitchell, P. & Riggs, K. J.) 191–228 (Psychology Press/Taylor & Francis, 2000).

  83. Bermudez, J. Thinking without Words (Oxford Univ. Press, 2003).

  84. Carey, S. The Origin of Concepts (Oxford Univ. Press, 2009).

  85. Leahy, B. P. & Carey, S. E. The acquisition of modal concepts. Trends Cogn. Sci. 24, 65–78 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  86. Spelke, E. in Advances in the Study of Language and Thought (eds Gentner, D. & Goldin-Meadow, S.) 277–311 (MIT Press, 2003).

  87. Schurz, M., Radua, J., Aichhorn, M., Richlan, F. & Perner, J. Fractionating theory of mind: a meta-analysis of functional brain imaging studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 42, 9–34 (2014).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  88. Schurz, M. et al. Toward a hierarchical model of social cognition: a neuroimaging meta-analysis and integrative review of empathy and theory of mind. Psychol. Bull. 147, 293–327 (2021).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  89. Saxe, R. in Encyclopedia of Consciousness (ed. Banks, W. P.) 401–409 (Academic Press, 2009).

  90. Frith, C. D. & Frith, U. The neural basis of mentalizing. Neuron 50, 531–534 (2006).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  91. Van Overwalle, F. Social cognition and the brain: a meta-analysis. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 829–858 (2009).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  92. Liu, D., Sabbagh, M. A., Gehring, W. J. & Wellman, H. M. Neural correlates of children’s theory of mind development. Child Dev. 80, 318–326 (2009).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  93. Jamali, M. et al. Single-neuronal predictions of others’ beliefs in humans. Nature 591, 610–614 (2021).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  94. Xiao, Y., Geng, F., Riggins, T., Chen, G. & Redcay, E. Neural correlates of developing theory of mind competence in early childhood. NeuroImage 184, 707–716 (2019).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  95. Wiesmann, C. G., Friederici, A. D., Singer, T. & Steinbeis, N. Two systems for thinking about others’ thoughts in the developing brain. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 117, 6928–6935 (2020).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  96. Grosse Wiesmann, C., Schreiber, J., Singer, T., Steinbeis, N. & Friederici, A. D. White matter maturation is associated with the emergence of theory of mind in early childhood. Nat. Commun. 8, 14692 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  97. Hughes, C. & Devine, R. T. Individual differences in theory of mind from preschool to adolescence: achievements and directions. Child Dev. Persp. 9, 149–153 (2015).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  98. Hughes, C. et al. Origins of individual differences in theory of mind: from nature to nurture? Child Dev. 76, 356–370 (2005).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  99. Ebert, S., Peterson, C., Slaughter, V. & Weinert, S. Links among parents’ mental state language, family socioeconomic status, and preschoolers’ theory of mind development. Cogn. Dev. 44, 32–48 (2017).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  100. McAlister, A. R. & Peterson, C. C. Siblings, theory of mind, and executive functioning in children aged 3–6 years: new longitudinal evidence. Child Dev. 84, 1442–1458 (2013).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  101. Dunn, J. et al. Young children’s understanding of other people’s feelings and beliefs: individual differences and their antecedents. Child Dev. 62, 1352–1366 (1991).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  102. Slaughter, V. & Peterson, C. C. in Access to Language and Cognitive Development (eds Siegal, M. & Surian, L.) 3–22 (Oxford Univ. Press, 2012).

  103. Aldrich, N. J., Chen, J. & Alfieri, L. Evaluating associations between parental mind-mindedness and children’s developmental capacities through meta-analysis. Dev. Rev. 60, 100946 (2021).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  104. Meins, E. et al. Maternal mind-mindedness and attachment security as predictors of theory of mind understanding. Child Dev. 73, 1715–1726 (2002).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  105. Callaghan, T. et al. Synchrony in the onset of mental-state reasoning: evidence from five cultures. Psychol. Sci. 16, 378–384 (2005).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  106. Bradford, E. E. et al. Cross-cultural differences in adult theory of mind abilities: a comparison of native-English speakers and native-Chinese speakers on the self/other differentiation task. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 71, 2665–2676 (2018).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  107. Shahaeian, A., Peterson, C. C., Slaughter, V. & Wellman, H. M. Culture and the sequence of steps in theory of mind development. Dev. Psychol. 47, 1239–1247 (2011).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  108. Taumoepeau, M., Sadeghi, S. & Nobilo, A. Cross-cultural differences in children’s theory of mind in Iran and New Zealand: the role of caregiver mental state talk. Cogn. Dev. 51, 32–45 (2019).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  109. Bloom, P. & German, T. P. Two reasons to abandon the false belief task as a test of theory of mind. Cognition 77, B25–B31 (2000).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  110. Onishi, K. H. & Baillargeon, R. Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs? Science 308, 255–258 (2005).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  111. Southgate, V., Senju, A. & Csibra, G. Action anticipation through attribution of false belief by 2-year-olds. Psychol. Sci. 18, 587–592 (2007).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  112. Schneider, D., Bayliss, A. P., Becker, S. I. & Dux, P. E. Eye movements reveal sustained implicit processing of others’ mental states. J. Exp. Psychol. 141, 433–438 (2012).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  113. Surian, L. & Geraci, A. Where will the triangle look for it? Attributing false beliefs to a geometric shape at 17 months. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 30, 30–44 (2012).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  114. Buttelmann, D., Carpenter, M. & Tomasello, M. Eighteen-month-old infants show false belief understanding in an active helping paradigm. Cognition 112, 337–342 (2009).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  115. Southgate, V., Chevallier, C. & Csibra, G. Seventeen-month-olds appeal to false beliefs to interpret others’ referential communication. Dev. Sci. 13, 907–912 (2010).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  116. Samson, D., Apperly, I. A., Braithwaite, J. J., Andrews, B. J. & Bodley Scott, S. E. Seeing it their way: evidence for rapid and involuntary computation of what other people see. J. Exp. Psychol. 36, 1255–1266 (2010).

    Google Scholar 

  117. Kovács, Á. M., Téglás, E. & Endress, A. D. The social sense: susceptibility to others’ beliefs in human infants and adults. Science 330, 1830–1834 (2010).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  118. Scott, R. M. & Baillargeon, R. Early false-belief understanding. Trends Cogn. Sci. 21, 237–249 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  119. Baillargeon, R., Scott, R. M. & He, Z. False-belief understanding in infants. Trends Cogn. Sci. 14, 110–118 (2010).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  120. Carruthers, P. Mindreading in infancy. Mind Lang. 28, 141–172 (2013).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  121. Leslie, A. M. Developmental parallels in understanding minds and bodies. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 459–462 (2005).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  122. Apperly, I. A. & Butterfill, S. A. Do humans have two systems to track beliefs and belief-like states? Psychol. Rev. 116, 953–970 (2009).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  123. Frith, C. D. & Frith, U. Implicit and explicit processes in social cognition. Neuron 60, 503–510 (2008).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  124. Low, J., Apperly, I. A., Butterfill, S. A. & Rakoczy, H. Cognitive architecture of belief reasoning in children and adults: a primer on the two-systems account. Child Dev. Persp. 10, 184–189 (2016).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  125. Butterfill, S. A. & Apperly, I. A. How to construct a minimal theory of mind. Mind Lang. 28, 606–637 (2013).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  126. Edwards, K. & Low, J. Reaction time profiles of adults’ action prediction reveal two mindreading systems. Cognition 160, 1–16 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  127. Low, J. & Watts, J. Attributing false beliefs about object identity reveals a signature blind spot in humans’ efficient mind-reading system. Psychol. Sci. 24, 305–311 (2013).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  128. Dörrenberg, S., Rakoczy, H. & Liszkowski, U. How (not) to measure infant theory of mind: testing the replicability and validity of four non-verbal measures. Cogn. Dev. 46, 12–30 (2018).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  129. Powell, L. J., Hobbs, K., Bardis, A., Carey, S. & Saxe, R. Replications of implicit theory of mind tasks with varying representational demands. Cogn. Dev. 46, 40–50 (2018).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  130. Poulin-Dubois, D. & Yott, J. Probing the depth of infants’ theory of mind: disunity in performance across paradigms. Dev. Sci. 21, e12600 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  131. Yott, J. & Poulin-Dubois, D. Are infants’ theory-of-mind abilities well integrated? Implicit understanding of intentions, desires, and beliefs. J. Cogn. Dev. 17, 683–698 (2016).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  132. Burnside, K., Ruel, A., Azar, N. & Poulin-Dubois, D. Implicit false belief across the lifespan: non-replication of an anticipatory looking task. Cogn. Dev. 46, 4–11 (2018).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  133. Grosse Wiesmann, C., Friederici, A. D., Disla, D., Steinbeis, N. & Singer, T. Longitudinal evidence for 4-year-olds’ but not 2- and 3-year-olds’ false belief-related action anticipation. Cogn. Dev. 46, 58–68 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  134. Schuwerk, T., Priewasser, B., Sodian, B. & Perner, J. The robustness and generalizability of findings on spontaneous false belief sensitivity: a replication attempt. R. Soc. Open. Sci. 5, 172273 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  135. Kulke, L. & Rakoczy, H. Testing the role of verbal narration in implicit theory of mind tasks. J. Cogn. Dev. 20, 1–14 (2019).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  136. Kulke, L., Johannsen, J. & Rakoczy, H. Why can some implicit theory of mind tasks be replicated and others cannot? A test of mentalizing versus submentalizing accounts. PLoS ONE 14, e0213772 (2019).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  137. Kulke, L., Wübker, M. & Rakoczy, H. Is implicit theory of mind real but hard to detect? Testing adults with different stimulus materials. R. Soc. Open. Sci. 6, 190068 (2019).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  138. Kulke, L., von Duhn, B., Schneider, D. & Rakoczy, H. Is implicit theory of mind a real and robust phenomenon? Results from a systematic replication study. Psychol. Sci. 29, 888–900 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  139. Kulke, L., Reiß, M., Krist, H. & Rakoczy, H. How robust are anticipatory looking measures of theory of mind? Replication attempts across the life span. Cogn. Dev. 46, 97–111 (2018).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  140. Kampis, D., Kármán, P., Csibra, G., Southgate, V. & Hernik, M. A two-lab direct replication attempt of Southgate, Senju and Csibra (2007). R. Soc. Open. Sci. 8, 210190 (2021).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  141. Crivello, C. & Poulin-Dubois, D. Infants’ false belief understanding: a non-replication of the helping task. Cogn. Dev. 46, 51–57 (2018).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  142. Wenzel, L., Dörrenberg, S., Proft, M., Liszkowski, U. & Rakoczy, H. Actions do not speak louder than words in an interactive false belief task. R. Soc. Open Sci. 7, 191998 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  143. Dörrenberg, S., Wenzel, L., Proft, M., Rakoczy, H. & Liszkowski, U. Reliability and generalizability of an acted-out false belief task in 3-year-olds. Infant. Behav. Dev. 54, 13–21 (2019).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  144. Grosse Wiesmann, C., Friederici, A. D., Singer, T. & Steinbeis, N. Implicit and explicit false belief development in preschool children. Dev. Sci. 20, e12445 (2017).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  145. Conway, J. R., Lee, D., Ojaghi, M., Catmur, C. & Bird, G. Submentalizing or mentalizing in a level 1 perspective-taking task: a cloak and goggles test. J. Exp. Psychol. 43, 454–465 (2017).

    Google Scholar 

  146. Cole, G. G., Atkinson, M. A., D’Souza, A. D. C. & Smith, D. T. Spontaneous perspective taking in humans? Vision 1, 17 (2017).

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  147. Kulke, L. & Rakoczy, H. Implicit theory of mind — an overview of current replications and non-replications. Data Brief. 16, 101–104 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  148. Priewasser, B., Rafetseder, E., Gargitter, C. & Perner, J. Helping as an early indicator of a theory of mind: mentalism or teleology? Cogn. Dev. 46, 69–78 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  149. Phillips, J. et al. A second look at automatic theory of mind. Psychol. Sci. 26, 1353–1367 (2015).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  150. Wellman, H., Cross, D. & Watson, J. Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: the truth about false belief. Child Dev. 72, 655–684 (2001).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  151. Baillargeon, R., Buttelmann, D. & Southgate, V. Invited Commentary: Interpreting failed replications of early false-belief findings: methodological and theoretical considerations. Cogn. Dev. 46, 112–124 (2018).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  152. Poulin-Dubois, D. et al. Do infants understand false beliefs? We don’t know yet — a commentary on Baillargeon, Buttelmann and Southgate’s commentary. Cogn. Dev. 48, 302–315 (2018).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  153. Holland, C. & Phillips, J. S. A theoretically driven meta-analysis of implicit theory of mind studies: the role of factivity. Proc. 42nd Annu. Conf. Cognitive Science Society (2020).

  154. Baker, C. L., Jara-Ettinger, J., Saxe, R. & Tenenbaum, J. B. Rational quantitative attribution of beliefs, desires and percepts in human mentalizing. Nat. Hum. Behav. 1, 0064 (2017).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  155. Jara-Ettinger, J. Theory of mind as inverse reinforcement learning. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 29, 105–110 (2019).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  156. Frank, M. C. et al. A collaborative approach to infant research: promoting reproducibility, best practices, and theory-building. Infancy 22, 421–435 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  157. Schuwerk, T., et al. (2022). Action anticipation based on an agent’s epistemic state in toddlers and adults. Child Development, (In-Principle Acceptance of Registered Report Stage 1: Study Design).

  158. Mellers, B., Hertwig, R. & Kahneman, D. Do frequency representations eliminate conjunction effects? An exercise in adversarial collaboration. Psychol. Sci. 12, 269–275 (2001).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  159. Yott, J. & Poulin-Dubois, D. Breaking the rules: do infants have a true understanding of false belief? Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 30, 156–171 (2012).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  160. Krupenye, C., Kano, F., Hirata, S., Call, J. & Tomasello, M. Great apes anticipate that other individuals will act according to false beliefs. Science 354, 110–114 (2016).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  161. Hayashi, T. et al. Macaques exhibit implicit gaze bias anticipating others’ false-belief-driven actions via medial prefrontal cortex. Cell Rep. 30, 4433–4444.e5 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  162. Király, I., Oláh, K., Csibra, G. & Kovács, Á. M. Retrospective attribution of false beliefs in 3-year-old children. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 115, 11477–11482 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  163. Michael, J. et al. Seeing it both ways: using a double-cuing task to investigate the role of spatial cuing in level-1 visual perspective-taking. J. Exp. Psychol. 44, 693–702 (2018).

    Google Scholar 

  164. Surtees, A. D. R., Butterfill, S. A. & Apperly, I. A. Direct and indirect measures of level-2 perspective-taking in children and adults. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 30, 75–86 (2012).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  165. Call, J. & Tomasello, M. Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 years later. Trends Cogn. Sci. 12, 187–192 (2008).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  166. Martin, A. & Santos, L. R. What cognitive representations support primate theory of mind? Trends Cogn. Sci. 20, 375–382 (2016).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  167. Horschler, D. J., MacLean, E. L. & Santos, L. R. Do non-human primates really represent others’ beliefs? Trends Cogn. Sci. 24, 594–605 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  168. Tomasello, M. How children come to understand false beliefs: a shared intentionality account. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 115, 8491–8498 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The author thanks Natalie Bleijlevens, Isa Garbisch, Feride Nur Haskaraca Kızılay, Lydia Schidelko, Britta Schünemann and Rowan Titchener for helpful comments on a previous version of this paper.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Hannes Rakoczy.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The author declares no competing interests.

Peer review

Peer review information

Nature Reviews Psychology thanks Martin Doherty, Virginia Slaughter, and the other, anonymous, reviewer for their contribution to the peer review of this work.

Additional information

Publisher’s note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Rakoczy, H. Foundations of theory of mind and its development in early childhood. Nat Rev Psychol 1, 223–235 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1038/s44159-022-00037-z

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s44159-022-00037-z

This article is cited by

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing