Abstract
The question of why people show individual differences in their behaviours and capacities has intrigued researchers for centuries. Behaviour genetics offers us various methods to address this question. The answers are interesting for a range of research fields, varying from medicine to psychology, economics and neuroscience. Starting with twin and family studies in the late 1970s, the field of behaviour genetics has rapidly developed by applying molecular genetic techniques next to, and sometimes combined with, family data. The overarching conclusion at this point in time is that all measured human traits are to some extent heritable, and that many genetic variants, with each exerting a small effect, explain this heritability. Against this backdrop, we offer readers who might be less familiar with behaviour genetics a brief Primer on the topic. Sitting atop our list of goals is to be a resource for scholars interested in applying the widely useful techniques of the field to their particular specialty, regardless of what that might be.
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution
Access options
Access Nature and 54 other Nature Portfolio journals
Get Nature+, our best-value online-access subscription
$29.99 / 30 days
cancel any time
Subscribe to this journal
Receive 1 digital issues and online access to articles
$119.00 per year
only $119.00 per issue
Buy this article
- Purchase on SpringerLink
- Instant access to full article PDF
Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout
Similar content being viewed by others
References
Turkheimer, E. Three laws of behavior genetics and what they mean. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 9, 160–164 (2000).
Harden, K. P. “Reports of my death were greatly exaggerated”: behavior genetics in the postgenomic era. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 72, 37–60 (2021).
Barnes, J. C. et al. Demonstrating the validity of twin research in criminology. Criminology 52, 588–626 (2014).
Friedman, N. P., Banich, M. T. & Keller, M. C. Twin studies to GWAS: there and back again. Trends Cognit. Sci. 25, 855–869 (2021).
Polderman, T. J. et al. Meta-analysis of the heritability of human traits based on fifty years of twin studies. Nat. Genet. 47, 702–709 (2015).
Chabris, C. F., Lee, J. J., Cesarini, D., Benjamin, D. J. & Laibson, D. I. The fourth law of behavior genetics. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 24, 304–312 (2015).
Visscher, PeterM. et al. Assumption-free estimation of heritability from genome-wide identity-by-descent sharing between full siblings. PLoS Genet. 2, e41 (2006).
Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C. & Loehlin, J. C. Genotype–environment interaction and correlation in the analysis of human behavior. Psychol. Bull. 84, 309–322 (1997).
Scarr, S. & McCartney, K. How people make their own environments: a theory of genotype–environment effects. Child. Dev. 54, 424 (1983).
Fowler-Finn, K. D. & Boutwell, B. B. Using variation in heritability estimates as a test of G × E in behavioral research: a brief research note. Behav. Genet. 49, 340–346 (2019).
Tucker-Drob, E. M. & Bates, T. C. Large cross-national differences in gene × socioeconomic status interaction on intelligence. Psychol. Sci. 27, 138–149 (2016).
Loehlin, J., Corley, R., Reynolds, C. & Wadsworth, S. Heritability × SES interaction for IQ: is it present in US adoption studies? Behav. Genet. 52, 1–8 (2022).
Heath, A. C., Kendler, K. S., Eaves, L. J. & Markell, D. The resolution of cultural and biological inheritance: informativeness of different relationships. Behav. Genet. 15, 439–465 (1985).
Truett, K. R. et al. A model system for analysis of family resemblance in extended kinships of twins. Behav. Genet. 24, 35–49 (1994).
Keller, M. C. et al. Modeling extended twin family data I: description of the Cascade model. Twin Res. Hum. Genet. 12, 8–18 (2009).
Pettersson, E. et al. Genetic influences on eight psychiatric disorders based on family data of 4 408 646 full and half-siblings, and genetic data of 333 748 cases and controls. Psychol. Med. 49, 1166–1173 (2019).
Lawlor, D. A., Tilling, K. & Davey Smith, G. Triangulation in aetiological epidemiology. Int. J. Epidemiol. 45, 1866–1886 (2016).
Yang, J. et al. Common SNPs explain a large proportion of the heritability for human height. Nat. Genet. 42, 565–569 (2010).
Young, A. I. Solving the missing heritability problem. PLoS Genet. 15, e1008222 (2019).
Young, A. I. Discovering missing heritability in whole-genome sequencing data. Nat. Genet. 54, 224–226 (2022).
Bulik-Sullivan, B. et al. An atlas of genetic correlations across human diseases and traits’. Nat. Genet. 47, 1236–1241 (2015).
Burgess, S., Butterworth, A. & Thompson, S. G. Mendelian randomization analysis with multiple genetic variants using summarized data. Genet. Epidemiol. 37, 658–665 (2013).
Falconer, D. S. Introduction to quantitative genetics (Longman Group, 1960).
Falconer, D. S. & Mackay, T. F. C. Quantitative Genetics (Longman Group, 1996).
Martin, N. & Eaves, L. The genetical analysis of covariance structure. Heredity 38, 79–95 (1977).
Neale, M. C. et al. OpenMx 2.0: extended structural equation and statistical modeling. Psychometrika 81, 535–549 (2016).
Burt, S. A., McGue, M. & Iacono, W. G. Nonshared environmental mediation of the association between deviant peer affiliation and adolescent externalizing behaviors over time: results from a cross-lagged monozygotic twin differences design. Dev. Psychol. 45, 1752–1760 (2009).
Bartels, M., Boomsma, D. I., Hudziak, J. J., van Beijsterveldt, T. C. E. M. & van den Oord, E. J. C. G. Twins and the study of rater (dis)agreement. Psychol. Meth. 12, 451–466 (2007); erratum 13, 170 (2008).
Nivard, M. G., Middeldorp, C. M., Dolan, C. V. & Boomsma, D. I. Genetic and environmental stability of neuroticism from adolescence to adulthood. Twin Res. Hum. Genet. 18, 746–754 (2015).
Visscher, P. M. et al. 10 years of GWAS discovery: biology, function, and translation. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 101, 5–22 (2017).
Willoughby, E. & Lee, J. in The Cambridge Handbook of Intelligence and Cognitive Neuroscience (eds Barbey, A., Karama, S., & Haier, R.) 349–364 (Cambridge Univ. Press, 2021).
Dudbridge, F. Power and predictive accuracy of polygenic risk scores. PLoS Genet. 9, e1003348 (2013).
Plomin, R. & von Stumm, S. Polygenic scores: prediction versus explanation. Mol. Psychiatry 27, 49–52 (2022).
Li, M. X. et al. A major gene model of adult height is suggested in Chinese. J. Hum. Genet. 49, 148–153 (2004).
Roberts, D. F., Billewicz, W. Z. & McGregor, I. A. Heritability of stature in a West African population. Ann. Hum. Genet. 42, 15–24 (1978).
Tarnoki, A. D., Tarnoki, D. L. & Molnar, A. A. Past, present and future of cardiovascular twin studies. Cor Vasa 56, e486–e493 (2014).
Pechlivanis, S. et al. Risk prediction for coronary heart disease by a genetic risk score — results from the Heinz Nixdorf Recall study. BMC Med. Genet. 21, 178 (2020).
Yang, R. et al. A healthy lifestyle mitigates the risk of heart disease related to type 2 diabetes: a prospective nested case–control study in a nationwide Swedish twin cohort. Diabetologia 64, 530–539 (2021).
McGue, M., Osler, M. & Christensen, K. Causal inference and observational research: the utility of twins. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 5, 546–556 (2010).
Lemvigh, C. et al. The relative and interactive impact of multiple risk factors in schizophrenia spectrum disorders: a combined register-based and clinical twin study. Psychol. Med. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291721002749 (2021).
Squarcina, L., Fagnani, C., Bellani, M., Altamura, C. A. & Brambilla, P. Twin studies for the investigation of the relationships between genetic factors and brain abnormalities in bipolar disorder. Epidemiol. Psychiatr. Sci. 25, 515–520 (2016).
Bouchard, T. J. & McGue, M. Genetic and environmental influences on human psychological differences. J. Neurobiol. 54, 4–45 (2003).
Wilson, R. S. The Louisville twin study: developmental synchronies in behavior. Child. Dev. 54, 298–316 (1983).
Bouchard, T. J., Lykken, D. T., McGue, M., Segal, N. L. & Tellegen, A. Sources of human psychological differences: the minnesota study of twins reared apart. Science. 250, 223–228 (1990).
Bouchard, T. The Wilson effect: the increase in heritability of IQ with age. Twin Res. Hum. Genet. 16, 923–930 (2013).
Deary, I. J., Johnson, W. & Houlihan, L. M. Genetic foundations of human intelligence. Hum. Genetics. 126, 215–232 (2009).
Jang, K. L., Livesley, W. J. & Vernon, P. A. Heritability of the big five personality dimensions and their facets: a twin study. J. Pers. 64, 577–591 (1996).
Tellegen, A. & Niels, G. W. Exploring personality through test construction: development of the multidimensional personality questionnaire. SAGE Handb. Pers. Theory Assess. 2, 261–292 (2008).
Vukasović, T. & Bratko, D. Heritability of personality: a meta-analysis of behavior genetic studies. Psychol. Bull. 141, 769–785 (2015).
Krueger, R. F., South, S., Johnson, W. & Iacono, W. The heritability of personality is not always 50%: gene–environment interactions and correlations between personality and parenting. J. Pers. 76, 1485–1522 (2008).
Matteson, L. K., McGue, M. & Iacono, W. G. Shared environmental influences on personality: a combined twin and adoption approach. Behav. Genet. 43, 491–504 (2013).
Howe, L. J. et al. Within-sibship genome-wide association analyses decrease bias in estimates of direct genetic effects. Nat. Genet. 54, 581–592 (2022).
Lee, J. J. et al. Gene discovery and polygenic prediction from a genome-wide association study of educational attainment in 1.1 million individuals. Nat. Genet. 50, 1112–1121 (2018).
Kong, A. et al. The nature of nurture: effects of parental genotypes. Science 359, 424–428 (2018).
Belsky, D. W. et al. Genetic analysis of social-class mobility in five longitudinal studies. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 115, E7275–E7284 (2018).
Bates, T. C. et al. Social competence in parents increases children’s educational attainment: replicable genetically-mediated effects of parenting revealed by non-transmitted DNA. Twin Res. Hum. Genet. 22, 1–3 (2019).
Willoughby, E. A., McGue, M., Iacono, W. G., Rustichini, A. & Lee, J. J. The role of parental genotype in predicting offspring years of education: evidence for genetic nurture. Mol. Psychiatry 26, 3896–3904 (2021).
Loehlin, J. C., Horn, J. M. & Willerman, L. in Intelligence, Heredity, and Environment (eds Sternberg, R. J., & Grigorenko, E. L.) 105–125 (Cambridge Univ. Press, 1997).
Cadoret, R. J. Adoption Studies. Alcohol Health Res. World 19, 195–200 (1995).
Rhea, S. A., Bricker, J. B., Corley, R. P., DeFries, J. C. & Wadsworth, S. J. Design utility and history of the Colorado Adoption Project: examples involving adjustment interactions. Adopt. Q. 16, 17–39 (2013).
Willoughby, E. A., McGue, M., Iacono, W. G. & Lee, J. J. Genetic and environmental contributions to IQ in adoptive and biological families with 30-year-old offspring. Intelligence 88, 101579 (2021).
Baker, M. Reproducibility crisis? Nature 533, 26 (2016).
Fanelli, D. Opinion: is science really facing a reproducibility crisis, and do we need it to? Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 115, 2628–2631 (2018).
Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., Knopik, V. S. & Neiderhiser, J. M. Top 10 replicated findings from behavioral genetics. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 11, 3–23 (2016).
Chabris, C. F. et al. Most reported genetic associations with general intelligence are probably false positives. Psychol. Sci. 23, 1314–1323 (2012).
Johnson, E. C. et al. No evidence that schizophrenia candidate genes are more associated with schizophrenia than noncandidate genes. Biol. Psychiatry 82, 702–708 (2017).
Border, R. et al. No support for historical candidate gene or candidate gene-by-interaction hypotheses for major depression across multiple large samples. Am. J. Psychiatry 176, 376–387 (2019).
Lin, X. Learning lessons on reproducibility and replicability in large scale genome-wide association studies. Harvard Data Sci. Rev. https://doi.org/10.1162/99608f92.33703976 (2020).
Røysamb, E., & Tambs, K. The beauty, logic and limitations of twin studies. Nor. Epidemiol. 26, 35–46 (2016).
van Dongen, J., Slagboom, P. E., Draisma, H. H., Martin, N. G. & Boomsma, D. I. The continuing value of twin studies in the omics era. Nat. Rev. Genet. 13, 640–653 (2012).
Wilson, S. et al. Minnesota Center for Twin and Family Research. Twin Res. Hum. Genet. 22, 746–752 (2019).
Hindorff, L. A. et al. Prioritizing diversity in human genomics research. Nat. Rev. Genet. 19, 175–185 (2017).
Mills, M. C. & Rahal, C. A scientometric review of genome-wide association studies. Commun. Biol. 2, 1–11 (2019).
George, S., Duran, N. & Norris, K. A systematic review of barriers and facilitators to minority research participation among African Americans, Latinos, Asian Americans, and Pacific Islanders. Am. J. Public Health https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2013.301706 (2014).
Wojcik, G. L. et al. Genetic analyses of diverse populations improves discovery for complex traits. Nature 570, 514–518 (2019).
Martin, A. R. et al. Human demographic history impacts genetic risk prediction across diverse populations. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 100, 635–649 (2017).
Giannakopoulou, O. et al. The genetic architecture of depression in individuals of East Asian ancestry: a genome-wide association study. JAMA Psychiatry 78, 1258–1269 (2021).
Meng, X. et al. Multi-ancestry GWAS of major depression aids locus discovery, fine-mapping, gene prioritisation, and causal inference. Preprint at bioRxiv https://doi.org/10.1101/2022.07.20.500802 (2022).
Martin, A. R. et al. Clinical use of current polygenic risk scores may exacerbate health disparities. Nat. Genet. 51, 584–591 (2019).
Bigdeli, T. B. et al. Genetic effects influencing risk for major depressive disorder in China and Europe. Transl. Psychiatry 7, e1074 (2017).
Bigdeli, T. B. et al. Contributions of common genetic variants to risk of schizophrenia among individuals of African and Latino ancestry. Mol. Psychiatry 25, 2455–2467 (2020).
Hyttinen, V., Kaprio, J., Kinnunen, L., Koskenvuo, M. & Tuomilehto, J. Genetic liability of type 1 diabetes and the onset age among 22,650 young Finnish twin pairs: a nationwide follow-up study. Diabetes 52, 1052–1055 (2003).
Snieder, H. et al. HbA1c levels are genetically determined even in type 1 diabetes: evidence from healthy and diabetic twins. Diabetes 50, 2858–2863 (2001).
McAdams, T. A., Rijsdijk, F. V., Zavos, H. M. & Pingault, J. B. Twins and causal inference: leveraging nature’s experiment. Cold Spring Harb. Perspect. Med. 11, a039552 (2021).
Alberg, A. J., Brock, M. V. & Samet, J. M. in Murray & Nadel’s Textbook of Respiratory Medicine 6th edn, Ch. 52, 927–939 (Saunders Elsevier, 2016).
Poulton, R., Moffitt, T. E. & Silva, P. A. The Dunedin multidisciplinary health and development study: overview of the first 40 years, with an eye to the future. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol., 50, 679–693 (2015).
Krüger, O., Korsten, P. & Hoffman, J. I. in J. Call, G. M. Burghardt, I. M. Pepperberg, C. T. Snowdon, & T. Zentall (Eds.), APA handbook of comparative psychology: Basic concepts, methods, neural substrate, and behavior (eds Call, J., Burghardt, G. M., Pepperberg, I. M., Snowdon, C. T. & Zentall, T.) 365–379 (American Psychological Association, 2017).
Turkheimer, E. & Harden, K. P. in Handbook of Research Methods in Social and Personality Psychology (eds Reis, H. T. & Judd, C. M.) 159–187 (Cambridge Univ. Press, 2014).
Okbay, A. et al. Polygenic prediction of educational attainment within and between families from genome-wide association analyses in 3 million individuals. Nat. Genet. 54, 437–449 (2022).
Marouli, E. et al. Rare and low-frequency coding variants alter human adult height. Nature 542, 186190 (2017).
Wainschtein, P. Assessing the contribution of rare variants to complex trait heritability from whole-genome sequence data. Nat. Genet. 54, 263–273 (2022).
Wang, Z., Gerstein, M. & Snyder, M. RNA-seq: a revolutionary tool for transcriptomics. Nat. Rev. Genet. 10, 57–63 (2009).
Gupta, S. et al. Transcriptome analysis reveals dysregulation of innate immune response genes and neuronal activity-dependent genes in autism. Nat. Commun. 5, 5748 (2014).
Pers, T.H. et al. Biological interpretation of genome-wide association studies using predicted gene functions. Nat. Commun. 6, 5890 (2015).
de Leeuw, C. A., Mooij, J. M., Heskes, T. & Posthuma, D. MAGMA: generalized gene-set analysis of GWAS data. PLoS Comput. Biol. 11, 1–19 (2015).
Neale, M. C. & Cardon, L. R. Methodology for Genetic Studies of Twins and Families. (Kluwer Academic/Plenum, 1992).
Vitaro, F., Brendgen, M. & Arseneault, L. Methods and measures: the discordant MZ-twin method: one step closer to the holy grail of causality. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 33, 376–382 (2009).
Keller, M. C., Medland, S. E. & Duncan, L. E. Are extended twin family designs worth the trouble? A comparison of the bias, precision, and accuracy of parameters estimated in four twin family models. Behav. Genet. 40, 377–393 (2010).
McAdams, T. A. et al. Revisiting the children-of-twins design: improving existing models for the exploration of intergenerational associations. Behav. Genet. 48, 397–412 (2018).
Scarr, S. & Weinberg, R. A. The Minnesota Adoption Studies: genetic differences and malleability. Child. Dev. 54, 260–267 (1983).
Murphy, K. et al. Twins Research Australia: a new paradigm for driving twin research. Twin Res. Hum. Genet. 22, 438–445 (2019).
Otta, E. et al. The University of São Paulo Twin Panel: current status and prospects for Brazilian twin studies in behavioral research. Twin Res. Hum. Genet. 22, 467–474 (2019).
Huang et al. The Chinese National Twin Registry: a unique data source for systems epidemiology of complex disease. Twin Res. Hum. Genet. 22, 482–485 (2019).
Pedersen et al. The Danish Twin Registry: an updated overview. Twin Res. Hum. Genet. 22, 499–507 (2020).
Bjerregaard-Andersen et al. The Guinea-Bissau Twin Registry update: a platform for studying twin mortality and metabolic disease. Twin Res. Hum. Genet. 22, 554–560 (2019).
Gharipour et al. Isfahan Twins Registry (ITR): an invaluable platform for epidemiological and epigenetic studies: design and methodology of ITR. Twin Res. Hum. Genet. 22, 579–582 (2019).
Ligthart et al. The Netherlands Twin Register: longitudinal research based on twin and twin-family designs. Twin Res. Hum. Genet. 22, 623–636 (2019).
Rimfeld et al. Twins Early Development Study: a genetically sensitive investigation into behavioral and cognitive development from infancy to emerging adulthood. Twin Res. Hum. Genet. 22, 508–513 (2019).
Pingault, J. B. et al. Using genetic data to strengthen causal inference in observational research. Nat. Rev. Genet. 19, 566–580 (2018).
Lee, J. J. Correlation and causation in the study of personality. Eur. J. Personal. 26, 372–412 (2012).
Vilhjámsson, B. J. et al. Modeling linkage disequilibrium increases accuracy of polygenic risk scores. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 97, 576–592 (2015).
de Vlaming, R. & Groenen, P. J. F. The current and future use of ridge regression for prediction in quantitative genetics. BioMed. Res. Int. 2015, 1–18 (2015).
Vattikuti, S., Lee, J. J., Chang, C. C., Hsu, S. D. H. & Chow, C. C. Applying compressed sensing to genome-wide association studies. GigaScience 3, 10 (2014).
Lello, L. et al. Accurate genomic prediction of human height. Genetics 210, 477–497 (2018).
Allegrini, A. G. et al. Genomic prediction of cognitive traits in childhood and adolescence. Mol. Psychiatry 24, 819–827 (2019).
Acknowledgements
T.J.C.P. is supported by ZonMw grant 60-63600-98-834. Additionally, all of the authors thank the individuals who together provided incredibly constructive and useful feedback. All of these individuals contributed to the improvement of this Primer, and have their sincere gratitude for this.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
Introduction (B.B.B. and T.J.C.P.); Experimentation (E.A.W.); Results (E.A.W.); Applications (E.A.W.); Reproducibility and data deposition (T.J.C.P.); Limitations and optimizations (B.B.B. and T.J.C.P.); Outlook (E.A.W. and B.B.B.); Overview of the Primer (T.J.C.P. and B.B.B.).
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Peer review
Peer review information
Nature Reviews Methods Primers thanks Thalia Eley, Elliott Rees, Lawrence Wilkinson and the other, anonymous, reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work.
Additional information
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
Glossary
- Akaike’s information criteria
-
A statistical method for evaluating how well a model fits the given data in which fit is penalized for the number of parameters estimated. In twin family studies, this term is often used to compare different possible models to determine which has the best fit.
- Bayesian information criteria
-
A statistical method for evaluating model fit in which a penalty term is introduced for the number of parameters. This is a more explanatory tool that assesses the underlying data.
- Cascade model
-
An extension of the extended twin family design that models covariances of twin pairs in addition to their siblings, spouses, parents and children. The Cascade model relaxes the assumptions of assortative mating and vertical transmission by allowing for the inclusion of latent phenotypes.
- Classical twin design
-
The simplest twin design in which pairs of monozygotic and dizygotic twins are compared for similarity on some phenotype, and the observed covariances are used to calculate the relative magnitudes of genetic and environmental sources of variance.
- Dizygotic
-
(DZ). Describes a pair of twins derived from two different sperm and two different ova who develop together in utero. DZ twins share approximately 50% of their DNA, similar to regular siblings.
- Equal environment assumption
-
The assumption that monozygotic and dizygotic twin pairs experience the same environmental factors.
- Extended twin family design
-
A type of twin family design that models observed covariances using the relatives of twins in addition to the focal twin covariances for some variable.
- Gene–environment correlations
-
(rGE). A phenomenon in which genes may influence individual variations in exposure to certain types of environment.
- Gene–environment interaction
-
An interplay between genes and environments in which different genomes can cause individuals to respond differently to the same environmental exposure.
- Monozygotic
-
(MZ). Describes a pair of twins derived from a single sperm and ovum, who are therefore genetically identical.
- Missing heritability
-
The observation that the sum total of genetic variants from a genome-wide association study cannot completely explain the heritabilities of complex traits derived from twin family studies.
- Nuclear twin family design
-
A type of twin family design that models observed covariances of the parents of twins in addition to the twins themselves.
- Placement effects
-
The non-random placement of children based on traits that are similar between biological and adoptive families.
- Polygenic score
-
(PGS). A number generated from genome-wide association data that summarizes the estimated effect of a large number of summed genetic variants on a phenotype of interest.
- Stealth model
-
An extension of the extended twin family design that models covariances of twin pairs in addition to their siblings, spouses, parents and children. The Stealth model relies on primary phenotypic assortment to model assortative mating, and on direct parent to offspring phenotypic transmission to model vertical transmission.
Rights and permissions
Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.
About this article
Cite this article
Willoughby, E.A., Polderman, T.J.C. & Boutwell, B.B. Behavioural genetics methods. Nat Rev Methods Primers 3, 10 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1038/s43586-022-00191-x
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s43586-022-00191-x
This article is cited by
-
Educational Attainment, Crime, and Causality: a Population-wide Sibling-based Design
Journal of Developmental and Life-Course Criminology (2024)
-
Same-Sex Relationships and Criminal Behavior: A Total Population Study in The Netherlands
Archives of Sexual Behavior (2024)
-
The Challenges and Opportunities for Mental Health Twin Research in Nigeria
Behavior Genetics (2024)
-
Genetic and environmental contributions to the subjective burden of social isolation during the COVID-19 pandemic
BMC Psychology (2023)
-
On the Usefulness of Behavior Genetics: Using Family Studies in Evolutionary Psychological Science to Improve Causal Inference and Sharpen Theory
Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology (2023)