Main

Technologies employed along the supply chain have enhanced the safety, nutritional value and sustainability of food, and yet agri-food technologies are not always perceived in a positive way by consumers1,2,3. As we look to ‘disruptive technologies’ for transforming food systems, important questions arise: Why do consumers reject food technologies that experts perceive safe? What factors influence consumers’ perceptions and acceptance of novel food technologies? Consumers will have a fundamental influence on what technologies in agriculture and food production are implemented and successful on the market. Many consumers perceive the use of food technologies as contradictory to healthy, nutritious, tasty food, which may be a challenge for the food industry. It is crucial to take consumers’ views into account during the early stages of product development.

Here, we review the literature on consumer perceptions of novel food technologies related to the production, preparation and storage of food (that is, gene technology (GT), nanotechnology, cultured meat and food irradiation). The term ‘novel’ does not necessarily refer to the invention of a technology, but rather to its introduction into the market. For example, even though food irradiation was invented in the previous century, we consider it as a novel technology because in some countries it has only been introduced recently and irradiated foods are accepted in some countries but not in others. We do not review how experts evaluate agricultural and food technologies, but solely on the factors that shape consumers’ attitudes toward and acceptance of such technologies. Consumers often rely on simple cues or heuristics, such as their perceived naturalness of food technologies or feelings of disgust evoked by the unfamiliar, as well as trust in the food industry, because they lack the technological knowledge. Several personality factors, including food technology neophobia or food disgust sensitivity, explain individual differences in people’s attitudes toward food technologies. For selected food technologies, we explain the most important factors that influence consumer acceptance.

Perception of food technologies

The food domain has witnessed technological progress; Fig. 1 overviews the timeline for invention or introduction of selected significant innovations in the agri-food domain4,5. Unlike in other domains, technological development in food has little obsolescence over time—new technologies do not replace older ones so much, but build upon and add to. As a consequence, there is much less pressure for consumers to accept innovations compared with other domains.

Fig. 1: Historical timeline of food technologies.
figure 1

www.flaticon.com: Freepik (salt, refrigerator, Pasteurization, fast food, microwave, sous vide, molecular gastronomy); Smashicon (deep frying, canned food, molecular gastronomy); Darius Dan (pressure cooker); Photo3ideastudio (GMO food).

Some food technologies (for example, GT and food irradiation) encounter strong resistance, whereas others (for example, refrigerators and freeze drying) have been widely accepted. Unlike in other domains, new food technologies do not replace older ones so much. As a result, there is much less pressure to accept innovations.

In many domains, technological progress is perceived positively6. The food domain has been shown to be slightly different in this respect. Some novel food technologies (for example, GT) encounter strong resistance by a considerable number of consumers7. Regarding food, technological applications are most often viewed as negative attributes, while food naturalness—produced with a minimum of obvious human intervention—is viewed as an inherently positive attribute8. The negative image of highly processed food is strongly influenced by a perceived lack of naturalness9. Moreover, humans by nature tend to be conservative concerning unfamiliar, novel foods10 and thus novel food technologies11. How conservative humans can be regarding culturally defined dietary behaviour is emphasized by the fact that even if non-traditional foods become accepted (for example, sushi, plant-based meat substitutes in the US or Europe), this does not fundamentally change people’s established diets and might only increase food variety12,13.

Role of heuristics

Consumers tend to have limited nutrition knowledge14, incorrect perceptions of the environmental impact of food products15 and scarce knowledge about food production16. Consequently, laypeople’s evaluation of food technologies is often based on heuristic processes, not on elaborate information processing17. Undoubtedly, heuristics play an important role in many decision situations18,19. When people rely on a heuristic to assess a food hazard, a target attribute (for example, number of fatalities caused by a food hazard) that is not readily accessible is substituted with a heuristic attribute that comes to mind more easily (for example, number of specific events someone suffered from a food hazard)20. However, the literature is divided on whether the use of heuristics results in smart or poor decisions. The so-called ‘heuristic and bias programme’ focuses on the negative side of heuristics19. This research tradition has identified a large number of heuristics that may cause both laypeople and experts to make biased decisions21. By contrast, research on the positive aspects demonstrates that heuristics can be efficient tools that, if used in the right environment, allow human beings to make sound decisions18,22. However, relying on heuristics when evaluating food hazards can result in biased decisions23. The same benefit was perceived more positively when it was the result of traditionally bred crops compared with genetically modified (GM) crops, for example23.

People utilize a large set of heuristics when making decisions19,21,24. In the following subsections, we focus on affect, trust and natural-is-better heuristics, which are most often used to explain consumers’ risk perceptions or acceptance of food technologies and food hazards.

Affect heuristic

The affect heuristic proposes that people rely on the affective meaning that they associate with an image or the associations elicited by an object when asked to evaluate its risks or benefits25. In the case of GT, some consumers may spontaneously think of ‘Frankenstein food’, and this perception may evoke strong negative feelings, whereas other consumers may associate GT with ‘golden rice’; as a result, their association is positively tagged.

In line with the affect heuristic, emotions have been found to influence people’s risk perceptions or risk judgments26,27. Hazards associated with stronger feelings of dread are judged to be riskier compared with hazards that do not evoke such feelings27. The affect heuristic may therefore explain why laypeople are concerned about some food hazards but not others, as well as why laypeople and experts differ in their risk perceptions. This heuristic has also been used to explain why laypeople differ in their acceptance of the same technology28. For example, consumers for whom GT is associated with negative feelings are less willing to purchase GM foods compared with people who associate positive images with this technology29. The affect heuristic has also been used to explain the acceptance of food irradiation3 or of nanotechnology in foods and food packaging30. In contrast with laypeople’s risk judgments, those of experts are not influenced by the affect associated with a hazard31. Due to their knowledge, experts can rely more on the analytical system for the evaluation of a food technology, but due to laypeople’s lack of technical knowledge, they need to rely on their experiential system, which is more driven by affect, concrete images, metaphors or narratives17, when assessing risks and benefits of novel food technologies. Most of the studies that have examined the impact of the affect heuristic on the acceptance of a technology have focused on general affect, ranging from negative to positive25. A more specific emotion is disgust, which can be viewed as a particular case of the affect heuristic. Evoked disgust has been found to influence the acceptance of GT32 and other novel food technologies33.

Trust heuristic

Trust has been described as34, and fulfils the criteria of, a heuristic20,30. If people rely on trust to evaluate a food technology, they substitute a target attribute (for example, improved yields) with cues that indicate trust in the source of this information (for example, value similarity)35. Consumers who buy organic foods cannot tell how these items are produced and whether their premium price is justified36. Regarding other credence attributes of products, consumers need to trust that the agents in the food chain honestly label the products and do not take advantage of the existing information asymmetry36. Trust is also important in situations where people lack knowledge for assessing a technology’s benefits and risks and therefore have to rely on others37. Society can be characterized by high degrees of division of labour and complexity. The trust heuristic, which implies that people depend on others’ performance or assessments, helps them reduce complexity and remain capable of acting in a complex environment38,39. Therefore, trust plays an important role in food acceptance, and it has been shown to influence risk and benefit perceptions of novel food technologies40,41,42. Trust also seems to be important in consumers’ reacceptance of a product after a crisis situation43.

How do people decide whom to trust? Reviews of the trust and risk perception literature suggest that it might be helpful to distinguish between two types of trust, namely social trust and confidence34. Social trust is based on perceived value similarities, and people tend to trust institutions with similar values as theirs and to distrust institutions whose values differ from theirs44, whereas confidence is based on past experience44 or perceived competencies45. Consumers may have confidence in the food industry’s competencies to produce safe foods or in a food technology, but they may lack social trust in the food industry because they believe that it values profit more than consumers’ health. Social trust is therefore more relevant to consumer acceptance than confidence, in that consumers believe that the promised added value in food products (for example, fair trade, organic production, healthiness) is actually delivered. It is not mainly a question of the food industry’s ability and competence but whether it is honest in disclosing benefits that cannot be directly judged by consumers46.

Natural-is-better heuristic

Natural evokes almost exclusively positive emotions in Western countries47. As a consequence, for the majority of consumers, naturalness in foods is of high importance, and natural foods are automatically perceived as healthier and tastier, as well as better for the environment9. The absence of human processing is a key feature of perceived naturalness8,47. However, the type of processing determines how strongly it influences perceived naturalness. Chemical changes reduce perceived naturalness more than physical transformations8,48. Furthermore, the process (for example, minerals added to spring water versus minerals naturally in the spring water) seems to be more important than the content (for example, percentage of natural minerals)8,49. Domesticated animals or plants are perceived as more natural compared with GM animals or GM plants8. The absence of additives is another important aspect of perceived naturalness50,51. People perceive orange juice with added vitamin C as less natural compared with orange juice with removed vitamin C, for example51. These findings suggest that the idea of some sort of contagion or harm to purity negatively influences perceived naturalness47.

The preference for natural foods could be driven by instrumental (for example, health) or ideational reasons (for example, moral reasons)47. Should the latter be the dominant motivation, it would be very difficult or even impossible to change consumers’ preferences47,52. However, some empirical evidence shows that instrumental beliefs strongly contribute to the natural-is-better heuristic53. It could therefore be possible to influence consumers’ preferences toward foods they perceive as less natural, if they provide tangible benefits.

Reliance on the natural-is-better heuristic for risk evaluation may introduce bias in decisions. For example, compared with experts, laypeople tend to be less concerned about hazards of natural origin, such as Listeria or Campylobacter54. The same benefits, such as a rich harvest, are perceived as less valuable for a farmer when they are the results of GM crops compared with traditionally bred crops, with the lack of perceived naturalness of GT seemingly the reason23. Perceived naturalness has also been shown to be important for the acceptance of functional foods55 or cultured meat56. Both are the results of human processing and interference with otherwise familiar products.

Framing effects

The framing of information influences people’s decisions, and how a food technology is described influences consumers57. For example, the US media has labelled lean, finely textured beef as ‘pink slime’ and as a consequence, consumers have been influenced by the negative label when assessing the health risks associated with the consumption of ground beef58. Similarly, it has been shown that the use of E numbers for food additives reduces their perceived naturalness compared with the situation in which the food additives are presented without E numbers48,59. How cultured meat is labelled (for example, laboratory grown meat, clean meat, animal-free meat) influences the valence of the evoked associations, which in turn help shape attitudes and behavioural intentions60. The labels used for new food technologies can, therefore, have a strong impact on consumer acceptance.

Individual differences among people

People differ in their preferences and values, which may explain some of the observed differences in consumer acceptance of agri-food technologies. Prior research has mostly examined the impacts of food technology neophobia, disgust sensitivity and cultural factors on individual differences in risk perception and acceptance. However, a recent study suggests that people’s general personality traits, such as openness and conscientiousness, also influence their preference for GM foods61.

Food technology neophobia

Food technology neophobia is a personality trait that influences consumers’ willingness to accept new food technologies11,62. This personality trait is only weakly correlated with food neophobia11,63, which is the tendency to reject unfamiliar foods10. People with more food knowledge seem to have lower food technology neophobia scores compared with people with less food knowledge64. However, consumers concerned about sustainability aspects of their food consumption tend to have higher food technology neophobia scores compared with consumers who are less concerned about sustainability64. The impact of food technology neophobia on the acceptance of novel food technologies seems to be not restricted to high-income countries, but has also been an important factor for the lack of food technology acceptance in Uganda, a low-income country65. Thus, there is some evidence that food technology neophobia is a universal factor that influences acceptance of innovations related to food.

Food neophobia has a somewhat mixed impact on attitudes toward new food technologies: it has no association with attitudes toward GT66 but does have with 3D-printed food67. Consumers may perceive 3D-printed food not only as produced by a new technology but also as a novel food, such as insect-based products15. In the case of GT, consumers may perceive the food as familiar and may be willing to taste it68 but may have negative feelings toward the technology.

Disgust sensitivity

Disgust is one of the evolved mechanisms that motivate people to avoid pathogens and to show disease-avoidance behaviour69. In an environment with limited food supply, people have to make trade-offs between avoiding disease and not forgoing calories. Depending on the environment to which people have adapted, whether food is scarce or more abundant, different strategies may have evolved. People living in the latter environment may react more often with disgust compared with people living in the former70.

Various instruments have been proposed to measure individual differences in disgust sensitivity; some scales focus on the pathogen, the sexual or the moral domain71, whereas other measures are applied to the food domain only72. A recently published food disgust scale predicts people’s tendency to experience feelings of disgust when the food has some cues that might be considered indications of pathogen presence (for example, mould) or contamination (for example, touched by another person)72. Undoubtedly, disgust sensitivity can have an impact, not only on people’s hygienic behaviour (for example, hand washing, kitchen hygiene)73,74, but also on their food selection behaviours and routines75. Concerning the acceptance of food technologies, researchers have just started to understand the influence of disgust. There are some controversies about whether disgust plays a role and effect sizes vary by the technological application33. For instance, state (that is, experienced in a situation) and trait (that is, personality factor) disgust are found to be linked to the rejection of artificial meat and milk, as well as GM foods33. In the latter case, associations with food contamination and perceived unnaturalness may make GM foods not only disgusting but also morally repugnant because of the perceived violation of nature76. Furthermore, people with higher disgust sensitivity may be less willing to accept GM food compared with people with lower disgust sensitivity. Such reasoning is supported by the finding that GM opponents have a higher disgust sensitivity than GM proponents32. Similar results have been found for other food technologies33. The explanatory power of disgust sensitivity regarding the acceptance of novel food technologies might be biased by language inconsistencies, however77. It has been argued that in the English language, people also use the term ‘disgust’ to describe non-specific feelings, such as ‘bothered’ and ‘upset’77. Therefore, it is not fully clear whether the disgust scales in the English language measure specific disgust or a general negative feeling. Despite the plausibility of the impact of disgust on the acceptance of novel food technologies, additional empirical support for such a claim would be desirable.

Cultural factors

The cultural theory of risk perception postulates that worldviews—beliefs about the functioning and values regarding society—are important determinants of the perspectives that people form about risks78. This paradigm maintains that people are either group or individual oriented, and they either prefer adhering to many rules to control human behaviour or believe that few rules are necessary79,80. On the basis of these dichotomies, four basic worldviews can be distinguished: hierarchical, fatalistic, individualistic and egalitarian. People in the US with more egalitarian worldviews seemed to be more concerned about food irradiation, GT or pesticides in foods compared with people with less egalitarian worldviews80. In a study conducted in the UK, hierarchical, individualistic, egalitarian and fatalistic worldviews were non-significantly correlated with risk perceptions of GT and only weakly associated with risk perceptions as regards food coloring81. Worldviews that are measured using a two-dimensional scale that distinguishes hierarchical–egalitarian worldviews from individualistic–communitarianistic principles were also found unrelated to risk/benefit perceptions with respect to nanotechnology82. Nevertheless, such worldviews exert an important effect on the interpretation of new information about nanotechnology. Hierarchical individualists lean more toward perceiving benefits than risks, whereas egalitarian communitarians tend to more readily discern risks than benefits after being exposed to new information about nanotechnology82.

Another approach for measuring cultural differences is based on the value concept83. This method is underlain by the assumption that basic values (for example, security, power) influence people’s attitudes and behaviours and that the importance of various values may differ across cultures. The explanatory power of this approach has been questioned84 because Schwartz’s well-known value scale83, as well as other value measures, is only weakly correlated with risk perception84.

The persuasive power of the cultural theory of risk perception stands in contrast to its explanatory power84. The type of hazard, how cultural values are measured, and whether information about a technology is provided seem to strongly influence the observed association. The cultural theory could be more strongly related to the acceptance of various risk regulation measures than with risk perception—an issue that would be an interesting avenue for future research.

Acceptance of selected food technologies

The voluntariness of being exposed to a risk is an important factor for its acceptance85. If consumers have the impression of being in control of their exposure to a food hazard, a higher risk is accepted compared with the situation where consumers lack this sense of voluntary exposure to a hazard54. The consumption of alcohol and an unbalanced diet, for example, are associated with certain benefits, whereas novel food technologies such as GT are not linked to any tangible benefits for many consumers.

The psychometric paradigm has been used to further explain why people are concerned about some hazards but not about others27. The results of these cited studies suggest that for laypeople, the qualitative characteristics of a hazard are more important than fatalities. Perceived dread and familiarity have been shown as important characteristics for the perception of food hazards86,87,88. GT, food irradiation and pesticide residues are perceived as unknown and dreadful risks; hazards, such as excessive intake of alcohol and calories, are perceived as familiar and less severe risks86,87,88. The likelihood and the possible negative consequences of a food hazard have a stronger impact on experts’ risk perceptions compared with laypeople’s assessments27,54.

Many food technologies have been introduced and, of course, not all have been rejected by consumers89. Unless there is a public discourse about a novel food technology, consumers may be unaware that it exists or is frequently used. If a food technology can be identified by its specific characteristics (for example, perceived as dreadful or unnatural), then nongovernmental organizations, scientists or the media may help a public discourse to emerge, and people may start to have negative perceptions of a food technology or even reject it. In the following sections, we focus on some selected food technologies for which social science research has examined the factors influencing their acceptance.

Gene technology

There has been comprehensive research on public acceptance of agricultural and food technologies, and that conducted in Europe shows large differences in how GT is perceived across countries7,90. The 2010 Eurobarometer, including representative samples with participants from 32 countries, also shows that opponents of GM foods outnumber supporters by three to one7. The main reasons for this low acceptance are perceived risks, lack of perceived benefits and perceived unnaturalness7.

People generally lack knowledge about GT91; therefore, they may rely on the affect, trust or naturalness heuristics to evaluate GM foods. The findings of empirical studies indicate the effects of all three heuristics on laypeople’s perceptions. Studies conducted in different countries across global regions find that social trust influences laypeople’s benefit and risk perceptions of GM foods41,92,93. These perceptions further influence the acceptance of GM foods. However, the importance of perceived risks and benefits for the acceptance of GM foods has been questioned76 because information about risks and benefits has little impact on the acceptance of GM foods94. This finding does not necessarily imply that perceived benefits are not important for the acceptance of GM foods. Rather, the results suggest that if there are no tangible benefits (that is, cheaper, better tasting food) for consumers, the perceived unnaturalness of GM foods may play an important role in the lack of acceptance95.

People’s acceptance of GT differs across applications7. The public seems to be more sceptical of GM foods and GM crops compared with genetic testing or medical applications96. Furthermore, in the food domain, not all genetic modifications are perceived in the same way. When genes are exchanged between different species, laypeople are more worried97. Similarly, cisgenic apples have received higher support than transgenic apples in all European Union countries7. It has also been shown that inserting a gene in an organism decreases its perceived naturalness more than deleting a gene51. Even though GM foods are negatively perceived overall, there are differences across applications and among consumers in how GT is evaluated.

Nanotechnology

In food products, engineered nanoparticles are already used as additives, and future applications of the technology in the food and the agricultural domains are under development98. There was substantial concern when the first nanotechnology applications in the food domain were discussed, as well as apprehension that nanotechnology could suffer a similar fate as that of GT99,100. Surveys found that for laypeople, the benefits of nanotechnology outweighed the risks, yet a relatively large percentage of the respondents did not answer the risk-versus-benefit question101. In other words, the participants were unable to assess the risks and the benefits of nanotechnology. This finding suggests that the perception of nanotechnology may still be rather malleable.

Several fundamental differences between GT and nanotechnology are relevant for people’s perceptions. The latter is related to a physical transformation of materials, whereas the former involves the manipulation of biological entities that may be perceived as tampering with nature8. Furthermore, physical transformation has a much lower impact on perceived naturalness compared with chemical or even biological transformation8.

In some studies, the majority of the participants had no prior knowledge about nanotechnology82; therefore, they had to form an opinion when filling in the questionnaire. Given the participants’ low level of technological knowledge, it is not surprising that several studies found that affect associated with nanotechnology was a strong predictor of perceived risks and that trust in the government regulations of this sector also influenced laypeople’s perceptions101. People’s cultural values had an effect on how they interpreted the information about nanotechnology82.

Even though nanotechnology will most likely never be perceived as unnatural or as tampering with nature (as is the case with GT), a possible increase in risk perception in the future cannot be completely ruled out. The media has the power to change public perception of nanotechnology100. Furthermore, government regulations may have an impact on how laypeople perceive food products containing nanomaterials102. For example, mandatory labelling of nanotechnology in food products could be misinterpreted as a warning signal by consumers102. Why would there be a label on a product if nanotechnology was not associated with some risks? Labelling does not only inform consumers, but it may also influence the perception of a food technology.

Cultured meat

Based on stem cell technology, in vitro or cultured meat is grown in the laboratory103. It has been proposed as a more sustainable and animal-friendly alternative to the conventional production of meat through livestock. In studies examining the stated acceptance of cultured meat, the use of different descriptions and labels may have been an important factor in explaining why the measured acceptance substantially differs across studies104. Importantly, how cultured meat is described influences the perceived naturalness of the product105. Consumers’ spontaneous reactions to the idea of laboratory-produced meat are the perception of unnaturalness and the feeling of disgust106,107. Perceived naturalness has also been shown to be an important factor influencing the acceptance of cultured meat59,105,108. Consumers who perceive cultured meat as unnatural find the risks less acceptable compared with conventional meat59 and are more disgusted105 than consumers who perceive cultured meat as more natural. For consumers’ acceptance of cultured meat, it will be crucial how the product will be described and what label will be used.

Cultured meat has animal welfare benefits, of course. How information about this aspect influences acceptance has not been examined, however56. Some research suggests that the importance of this aspect may be limited because consumers use various cognitive strategies for the justification of raising and killing animals for meat consumption109. Therefore, for many consumers, the animal welfare argument may be less important for the acceptance of cultured meat compared with other aspects, such as perceived naturalness, taste or price.

Food irradiation

The label used for a food technology can have a large impact on how the technology and its acceptability are perceived. The term ‘food irradiation’ evokes not only negative associations, such as a nuclear plant or cell destruction110, but the affect associated with nuclear power seems to influence the perception of this food technology3. Consumers for whom nuclear power is very negatively tagged, perceive food irradiation as riskier and less beneficial compared with consumers for whom nuclear power is neutral or positive3. The affect heuristic produces biased perceptions that may even result in the perception that irradiated food poses a health risk111. Experimental studies show how important the name of a food technology is for its perception. Products labelled as ‘treated with irradiation’ are perceived to be of lower quality compared with products labelled as ‘treated with ionization’3. The latter term does not seem to elicit associations as negative as food irradiation does. Therefore, it is not surprising that irradiated food has been evaluated as unacceptable by consumers112 and as a technology that substantially decreases the perceived utility of foods.

Conclusions

A broad set of agri-food technologies is needed to produce safe, healthy, sustainable foods, and technologies labelled ‘disruptive’ are seen as part of the solution to food security in the era of climate change and population growth. Most disruptive innovations that fundamentally changed an industry are from outside the food domain113. Figure 1 further shows the limited number of disruptive innovations in the food system. In order to meet the challenges posed by climate change, population growth and disruptions in the food chain, there is clear need for disruptive innovations. For the successful introduction of such innovations, societal acceptance needs to be examined at an early stage of development.

For consumers in general, the production of foods is increasingly a black box. The prevalence of highly processed convenience foods and declining cooking skills114 further alienate many consumers from how food is produced and how meals are prepared. Future studies need to examine whether a crisis such as COVID-19 influences consumers’ perceptions of food technologies, because canned and frozen foods, for example, are essential for stockpiling. Due to their lack of knowledge and familiarity with most of these food technologies, they often rely on simple heuristics when evaluating them. The experiential system is more important than the analytical system in how novel food technologies are perceived17. Generally, consumers’ reliance on the natural-is-better and the affect heuristics is one reason for their lack of acceptance of some novel food technologies. As Fig. 2 illustrates, both the aspects of the technology and the characteristics of the person further influence their perception. If a technology is viewed as unnatural, dreadful, and uncontrollable, and if people are not voluntarily exposed to it, its acceptance tends to be low. Person-related factors, such as food technology neophobia, disgust sensitivity and cultural values, further influence the perception of a technology. The factors that are most crucial for the lack of acceptance differ across food technologies, however.

Fig. 2: Factors influencing the perception of food technologies.
figure 2

How a food technology is perceived by consumers depends both on the perceived aspects of a food technology and on the individual characteristics of the consumers. Furthermore, both of these factors influence the heuristics that consumers rely on when evaluating the benefits, risks and acceptance of food technologies.

The natural-is-better heuristic is especially relevant for people’s acceptance of novel food technologies and for their evaluation of food. The food industry players are aware of how important perceived naturalness is for consumers; therefore, the naturalness of its products is emphasized in marketing campaigns. It may seem strange that on one hand, the food industry struggles with consumers’ acceptance of novel food technologies due to the lack of perceived naturalness; on the other hand, the food industry emphasizes the naturalness of its products, which may further strengthen consumers’ belief that natural products are superior. Similarly, the promotion of organic foods may foster the idea that natural foods are better despite the lack of scientific support for a lower environmental impact of organically produced foods115. Technological innovations and scientific progress have resulted in high levels of food safety and food security in many countries. The somewhat ironic consequence is that, at least in many developed countries, the naturalness of food is highly valued because food safety and security are considered guaranteed.

Progress toward a more sustainable, more secure, safer food system is difficult to envisage without novel food technologies. Therefore, general scepticism regarding technologies in the food domain will remain a challenge. In this Review, we have presented some factors that help explain why people often evaluate novel food technologies in a rather negative way. For various reasons, condemnation of technologies is problematic, in our view. The deep freezer can be used to store ice cream or vegetables that are most likely richer in vitamins than fresh ones bought in the supermarket that are not frozen but stored for days between harvest and consumption. Even an extruder that produces highly processed foods can be used for foods (for example, whole-grain pasta) that contribute to a balanced diet. The question is for what purpose, not whether a technology is used. Society should move towards a healthier diet, but it should be recognized that food technologies need to be part of such a trend instead of being perceived as barriers.

We have also identified some knowledge gaps that future research should address. Most studies in this field have been conducted in developed countries, mainly in selected European countries or North America. For many of the topics covered in this Review, there is a lack of studies from Asian or African countries. For example, it needs to be known whether the concept of naturalness is equally important in developing countries. Another fruitful avenue for future research is the impact of cultural values on the acceptance of novel food technologies. The few studies that have examined the impact of cultural values have focused on the differences among the people in a certain country but have failed to address the question of whether cultural values also explain differences in perceptions across countries.

Lastly, genome editing of crops has gained much progress in recent years, and countries differ in how CRISPR-engineered crops are regulated and whether or not they are perceived similarly to genetically modified organisms116. There is some indication that consumers generally perceive CRISPR-engineered crops more positively than GT7. Perceptions of new food technologies are malleable, however. Future studies need to examine how different genome editing technologies are perceived by consumers and which aspects of these technologies most strongly influence their acceptance.