Abstract
Consciousness, a cornerstone of human cognition, is believed to arise from complex neural interactions. Traditional views have focused on localized fronto-parietal networks or broader inter-regional dynamics. In our study, we leverage advanced fMRI techniques, including the novel Functionnectome framework, to unravel the intricate relationship between brain circuits and functional activity shaping visual consciousness. Our findings underscore the importance of the superior longitudinal fasciculus within the fronto-parietal fibers, linking conscious perception with spatial neglect. Additionally, our data reveal the critical contribution of the temporo-parietal fibers and the splenium of the corpus callosum in connecting visual information with conscious representation and their verbalization. Central to these networks is the thalamus, posited as a conductor in synchronizing these interactive processes. Contrasting traditional fMRI analyses with the Functionnectome approach, our results emphasize the important explanatory power of interactive mechanisms over localized activations for visual consciousness. This research paves the way for a comprehensive understanding of consciousness, highlighting the complex network of neural connections that lead to awareness.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
Consciousness comprises the fundamental aspect of sentient beings, including their perception, thoughts, and emotions1,2. In neuroscience, two prevailing theories attempt to elucidate its mechanisms. The Global Workspace Theory suggests that consciousness arises from the broadcasting of information within a global workspace, where sensory inputs compete for dominance after initial unconscious processing within primary modular cerebral networks3. In contrast, the Integrated Information Theory posits that consciousness emerges from a system’s capacity to integrate information via the interactions between various regions of the brain, highlighting the crucial importance of neural networks4,5,6. Subjective conscious experiences in healthy individuals, whether conveyed through visual7,8, somatosensory9, or auditory10 inputs, elicit distinctive local increases in functional activity within the frontal and parietal cortices11. However, the comprehensive understanding of the integrative mechanisms supporting the conscious experience remains elusive, partly due to the limitation of the traditional topological approach in functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies12. These methods often overlook the dynamic interplay between brain regions, a critical component in the Integrated Information Theory framework6,13. To overcome these limitations, our study explores the role of anatomical connections in visual conscious experience using recent technical advancements like the Functionnectome14,15,16.
The introduction of the Functionnectome represents a new methodology that integrates structural connectivity data within functional analysis. The Functionnectome takes the activity signals (blood oxygenation level-dependent [BOLD] time series) from the grey matter and combines them based on how these grey areas are connected to white matter areas. The strength of each connection influences the final signal. As a result, it creates a new set of 4D brain imaging data. This new data projects the brain activity from the grey matter onto the white matter, with the connections’ strength influencing the outcome. By performing a weighted average of the BOLD signal based on the probability of connection in the white matter and then conducting a standard general linear model, we are assessing whether the involvement of specific white matter tracts is significant. This approach allows for a more nuanced examination of the interplay among various brain regions, moving beyond the traditional focus on their isolated functions in brain processes. Compared with earlier methods that initiated tractography directly from functional activation sites17, the Functionnectome facilitates a data driven statistical analysis of the implicated white matter pathways.
The present paper focuses on the mechanisms of visual consciousness, acknowledging their association with attentional processes. We employed the Functionnectome in combination with three distinct fMRI paradigms that disentangle attention from conscious perception18,19,20. Specifically, phasic alerting, spatial orienting, or executive attention were manipulated in each paradigm by presenting an alerting tone, a peripheral cue, and a Stroop task, respectively for each process. The target was a near-threshold Gabor stimulus individually titrated to be perceived ~50% of the time. Participants had to discriminate the orientation of the Gabor’s lines (except in the executive attention task), and to detect and report whether or not they saw the target (see Methods section for a detailed description of the tasks). Through a conjunction analysis of the contrast seen >unseen, we aimed to reveal specific networks that underlie visual consciousness, expanding our understanding of the neural correlates of visual consciousness independently from attention.
We hypothesized that consciousness emerges from the interaction within and between circuits involving key anatomical pathways, including frontoparietal fibers (i.e., the superior longitudinal fasciculus), vertical temporoparietal fibers (i.e., the posterior segment of the arcuate fasciculus), and anterior thalamic radiations. The superior longitudinal fasciculus projections connect the areas showing an increase of blood flow during explicit conscious access and a reduction in metabolism during the loss of consciousness11, as well as areas usually reported as damaged in patients showing lack of awareness for one side of space (i.e., spatial neglect)21. Initial evidence in a stroke patient suggests the importance of temporoparietal fibers in connecting preconscious processing to the frontoparietal systems linked by the superior longitudinal fasciculus22. Furthermore, thalamic contributions play a critical role in interregional coupling, facilitating interactions through interregional synchronization23 (for a review, see ref. 24).
Results
fMRI task activations
Figure 1a presents the voxelwise analysis of the fMRI paradigms manipulating visual consciousness, following a classical approach. The conjunction of results revealed the standard cortical network of the global neuronal workspace, encompassing frontal (i.e., left precentral and superior frontal gyrus), parietal (mainly the intraparietal sulcus) and temporal lobes (activation centered onto the posterior portion of the inferior temporal sulcus and the fusiform gyrus).
In contrast, Fig. 1b showcases the same data’s Functionnectome analysis, confirming the expected involvement of distinct anatomical pathways. Specifically, the second branch of the superior longitudinal fasciculus, the posterior segment of the arcuate fasciculus, and the anterior thalamic radiations exhibited significant involvement. Furthermore, the splenium of the corpus callosum demonstrated a significant involvement.
Comparison between classical and Functionnectome analysis approach
Classical activation analysis and Functionnectome analysis represent different models of the brain functioning, one focusing statistically on independent activation of single voxels (i.e., functional segregation) and the other considering the weighted average of activation along white matter connections (i.e., functional interaction). Currently, limited knowledge exists regarding the differences between these two methods for depicting brain functioning.
To explore this question, we created histograms depicting the distribution of z values for each brain voxel using both methods (Fig. 2a). An ANOVA (detailed in the methods section) revealed a strong interaction between the employed techniques and the level of activations in the number of activated voxels (F(1, 154) = 14.329, P = 0.000219). Interestingly, activations were more prominently represented in the Functionnectome fMRI analyses than in the classical approach, as revealed by a post-hoc independent sample t test (t(77) = -2.557, P = 0.013). Conversely, de-activations were more frequent in the classical analysis than in the Functionnectome (t(77) = 2.847, P = 0.006).
Discussion
Employing state-of-the-art fMRI techniques, we harnessed the power of advanced analyses to elucidate the neural underpinnings of conscious visual perception. By directly comparing the traditional topological approach (classical fMRI) with the cutting-edge hodological perspective (study of pathways via Functionnectome), we suggested a pivotal role of interactions in the conscious access to visual information. The set of results found with the classical fMRI approach was highly congruent with neuroimaging literature showing brain regions involved in eliciting a conscious visual percept (for a recent meta-analysis see ref. 25). However, the Functionnectome approach led us to identify four distinct anatomical circuitry components (see Fig. 2b), each with the potential to give rise to distinct disorders of conscious visual perception when disconnected.
The extensive body of literature consistently underscores the relevance of the frontoparietal cortical network in facilitating conscious perception (for a review see ref. 11. In individuals with intact cognitive function, variations in the strength of frontoparietal connections—the superior longitudinal fasciculus—have been associated with visual perception, leading to either distortions26 or perceptual enhancements18,27,28. When these critical frontoparietal fibers, and especially the second branch of the superior longitudinal fasciculus, become disconnected, as observed in cases of visual neglect, patients demonstrate an absence of awareness regarding events occurring on the contralesional side29. Furthermore, temporary neglect induced by electrical perturbation of this white matter pathway during awake brain surgery disrupts the symmetrical processing of visual scenes21. Converging evidence on the role of these frontoparietal networks comes from similar studies using magneto-encephalography30 or intracerebral recordings in drug-resistant epilepsy31. Given the existing body of evidence, identifying activation within the networks linked by the second branch of the superior longitudinal fasciculus during conscious visual access aligns seamlessly with our predictions. Moreover, it is essential to note that this network’s functionality extends beyond conscious visual perception, as other crucial cognitive processes, such as working memory and representation, colocalize within it32. These additional functions play an indispensable role in mentally manipulating concepts as a form of stream of consciousness3, allowing individuals to consciously maintain and report information in a meaningful manner.
Fibers connecting occipital-temporal-parietal regions would transfer visual information from the early visual areas to the visual ventral stream33. At that level, the processing of information may be preconscious. Supporting this idea, Dalla Barba and collaborators22 reported a single patient with left visuospatial neglect symptoms that presented a right temporoparietal lesion that caused a disconnection between the occipitotemporal visual processing stream and the frontoparietal attentional networks. Therefore, a crucial part of the visual-conscious access circuit may be supported by those temporoparietal connections.
In addition to intra-hemispheric fibers, connections between left and right hemispheres may be necessary for transferring the information and building a unified conscious representation34, particularly through its verbalization35. Our analysis revealed a significant involvement of the splenium of the corpus callosum, which are projections from the occipital-parietal and temporal homologous cortices. The splenium of the corpus callosum has been implicated in visual neglect when compared with patients without neglect and in the chronic persistence of the symptoms36, and in association with symptom severity37.
The involvement of the medio-dorsal thalamus and its cortical projections through the internal capsule in the visual conscious access circuit is worth considering. The thalamus has been extensively linked to disorders of consciousness, as demonstrated by studies such as Liyana Arachige et al.38, which highlight its connections with both the voluntary movement motor circuit and the anterior forebrain mesocircuit39. In addition to its role in state-based precursors of consciousness, such as states of consciousness and arousal, the thalamus may also play a part in conscious access. Evidence suggests that lesions affecting the laterodorsal portion of the thalamus can lead to spatial neglect in patients with preserved parietal-frontal connections29. Moreover, Kronemer et al.40 demonstrated the existence of a thalamic awareness potential and an increase in BOLD signal within the thalamus following the perception of a visual threshold stimulus, irrespective of whether it involved a report or no-report paradigm. The thalamus’s potential critical role in overseeing interregional coupling at the cortical level23,24 and, accordingly, integrative mechanisms might explain its contribution to visual conscious access.
Our comparative analysis delineates the distinctions between traditional fMRI analyses and the Functionnectome approach in evaluating task-related fMRI signals. This examination reveals that, in contrast to classical analyses that primarily identify isolated activations, the incorporation of brain interactions along white matter pathways provides a significant alternative perspective. This perspective focuses on the exchanges between brain regions during visual-conscious access. Importantly, our analysis detected much less deactivation than activation spreading along white matter pathways. This suggests that deactivation processes may not involve structural connections. Instead, they may be attributed to decreased activation or reduced oxygen consumption41,42 in specific regions without direct structural links. Therefore, our findings propose that consciousness involves more than localized brain activation or deactivation, highlighting the importance of interactions between brain areas.
In conclusion, our study elucidates that consciousness emerges from a network of interactions predominantly between the brain regions implicated in the global neuronal workspace11. Our results suggest that the access to visual consciousness is mediated by a tripartite circuit of connections (Fig. 2b) encompassing the visual ventral stream, which converts visual input into a coherent percept, the frontoparietal networks, which contribute to the continuity of conscious experience, and the interhemispheric connections facilitating left and right hemifields integration and access to verbalization. Within this complex neural network, the thalamus plays a role as a crucial conductor, supervising and monitoring the coordination of these interconnected processes. The integration of these components sheds light on the fundamental mechanisms that give rise to conscious experience, furthering our understanding of the nature of consciousness.
Methods
Neuroimaging data
Three datasets were employed in this work. For the alerting task, we used data from the study conducted by Chica et al.20, which included 18 participants. Similarly, we employed data from Chica et al.’s study19 for the orienting task, which also involved 18 participants. Lastly, we utilized data from Martín-Signes et al.’s study18, which had 20 participants for the executive attention task. Data used in this study was collected with the informed consent of the participants and was approved by the Ethics Committee of the INSERM (France) and the University of Granada (Spain). All ethical regulations relevant to human research participants were followed. The authors of the original studies provided us with the datasets, and we analyzed them with their consent.
Experimental tasks
To learn more about the experiment, please refer to the original research articles18,19,20. To disentangle the attentional mechanism from conscious perception, we combined data from three visual tasks with different types of attentional manipulations (phasic alerting, spatial orienting, and executive attention). The three tasks involved identifying a near-threshold stimulus that could appear in one of two lateral boxes on the screen. In some trials, the stimulus was not presented. The contrast of the stimulus was adjusted for each participant before the task to ensure that they perceived it about half of the time. For a schematic representation of the sequence of events in a given trial of each task, please see Fig. 3.
In the alerting task, a white noise cue was played on half of the trials. Participants had to make two consecutive responses: first, they identified the orientation of the lines in the stimulus, and second, they reported whether or not they consciously detected it by indicating its location (right or left box) or indicating that the target was not seen. The experiment consisted of two sessions with five functional scans each, lasting 12 min each, for 920 trials.
During the orienting task, a square would appear around one of the boxes on the side of the screen, indicating where the target would likely be located on 67% of the trials. This cue was displayed for 300 ms. Similar to the alerting task, participants were required to give two consecutive responses: identifying the orientation of the Gabor’s lines and reporting if they consciously detected the target’s appearance. The experiment had one session of 5 functional scans, each 7 min long, with 280 trials.
During the executive attention task, participants were presented with a Stroop task. This task involved Spanish words for blue, green, and yellow colors displayed in blue, green, or yellow. If the word meaning and the color matched, it was called a congruent trial. If they were different, it was called an incongruent trial (which happened 20% of the time). Participants were asked to do two consecutive tasks: first, they had to discriminate the word’s color, and second, they had to report if they consciously detected the target’s appearance and its location. The experiment consisted of 2 sessions with five functional scans of 8 min each, resulting in 600 trials.
In the three experiments, participants underwent a titration procedure that ensured that ∼50% would be “seen trials” (i.e., participants reported that they consciously perceived the target and correctly located it), while the other ∼50% were “unseen” (i.e., participants indicated that they did not perceive the target). Indeed, when the target was consciously detected, the discrimination response (i.e., identifying the orientation of the Gabor’s lines) was above chance, while it was at chance when it was not detected. The awareness main effect was significant for both the alerting and orienting tasks (alerting task: F(1, 18) = 281.46, P < 0.001, discrimination accuracy for seen reports M = 76%, and unseen reports M = 46%; orienting task: F(1,17) = 195.69, P < 0.001, discrimination accuracy for seen reports M = 93%, and unseen reports M = 47%). Note that the executive attention task did not include a discrimination response. In addition, the percentage of false alarms (i.e., reporting to detect the target when it was not presented) was kept very low (alerting task: 13% of the target-absent trials, SD = 26.20; orienting task: 6.91% of the target-absent trials, SD = 9.19; executive task: 4.9% of the target-absent trials, SD = 6.86).
During the study, participants were shown the task on a screen at the back of the scanner via a mirror mounted on the head coil. They responded by pressing buttons on an MRI-compatible fiber optic box. The order of the trial types and the jitter fixation were determined by an optimal sequencing program called Optseq2. This program is designed to maximize the efficiency of recovering the blood-oxygen-level dependent (BOLD) response, which is a reliable indicator of brain activity43.
Acquisition parameters
The acquisition parameters used for the whole-brain fMRI study can be found in the original publications by Chica et al.19,20 and Martín-Signes et al.18. The studies were conducted on two different Tesla Siemens TRIO MRI scanners using a whole-head coil. Functional images were acquired using a gradient-echo echo-planar pulse sequence. For the alerting task, they used 372 volumes acquired per run with the time-to-repetition (TR) set at 2000 ms, the time-to-echo (TE) at 25 ms, using 39 axial 3-mm cubic slices with no inter-slice gap, a flip angle set at 75°, and the field of view (FoV) of 220 mm. For the orienting task, they used 220 volumes per run with a TR set at 2000 ms, a TE at 25 ms, 34 axial 2.5 × 2.5 × 3-mm slices with no inter-slice gap, a flip angle set at 75°, and a FoV of 220 mm. For the executive attention task, they used 245 volumes per run with a TR set at 2000 ms, a TE at 25 ms, 35 axial 3.4-mm cubic slices with no inter-slice gap, a flip angle set at 75°, and a FOV of 220 mm. In addition, high-resolution T1-weighted anatomical images were systematically collected.
fMRI preprocessing
We employed FEAT (FSL, FMRIB’s Software Library, Woolrich et al.44) to perform preprocessing routines and analyses. In short, those included brain extraction45, slice timing correction, and realignment through rigid-body transformation for motion correction using MCFLIRT46. Motion plots were visually inspected to discard those runs with excessive motion (i.e., relative motion > half of voxel size and/or absolute motion > voxel size). Structural and functional volumes of each participant were coregistered using the Boundary-Based Registration function. Next, we registered the structural volume to a standard image, and a similar transformation was applied to the functional volume using a non-linear registration with 12 degrees of freedom. During normalization to the MNI152 stereotaxic space, volumes were upsampled to 2 mm isotropic voxels. A 128 s high-pass filter was used to eliminate contamination from slow signals drift. Outlier scans corrupted by large motion were detected using the tool fsl_motion_outliers and regressed out. The number of outlier scans never exceeded 20% of the total scans in a run. For the classical approach, the signal from neighboring voxels was combined applying a 5-mm full width at half maximum (FWHM) Gaussian smoothing47. In contrast, the Functionnectome approach required no spatial smoothing as it already combines the signal from distant yet structurally linked voxels15.
Functionnectome approach
We used the Functionnectome (openly available at http://www.bcblab.com) to process preexisting data18,19,20. The Functionnectome projects the signal from each voxel of the fMRI volume (with four dimensions; 4D) to the white matter according to their structural relationships. These structural relationships were based on a probability map that depicts the structural connectivity between a particular voxel and the rest of the brain. This map is derived from a normative high-resolution tractography resource acquired at 7 T in 100 subjects48 derived from the processed version49 of the human connectome project (raw data available at www.humanconnectome.org and processed tractographies at https://osf.io/5zqwg/). The Functionnectome utilizes the structural connectivity data provided by white matter priors to calculate a weighted average of the BOLD time series originating from the grey matter voxels connected to a specific white matter voxel. As a result, the Functionnectome generates a new 4D volume that projects the fMRI signal from grey matter voxels to white matter, weighted by the connections’ probability. This 4D functional volume, with functional time series on the white matter voxels, can be statistically analyzed similarly to a classical fMRI volume15.
Statistics and reproducibility
For both the classical and the Functionnectome approaches, we used the general linear model and convolved task regressors with the FSL double-gamma function to analyze each run. For the three tasks, fMRI trials were categorized as either “seen” or “unseen” based on participants’ responses.
We included six head motion parameters and outlier scans as regressors of no interest to avoid motion artifacts in all first-level analyses for the three tasks. We calculated intra-subject brain activations for the contrast seen > unseen using fixed effects and conducted higher-level mixed-effects using FLAME 150. In order to minimize the contamination of the results by the attentional mechanism, a conjunction analysis using easythresh_conj tool from FSL51 was applied to the three seen > unseen statistical maps derived from the three experimental paradigms (phasic alerting, spatial orienting, and executive attention). Note that these conjunction maps were calculated independently using the contrast maps derived from each analysis approach. Correction for multiple comparisons was conducted at a cluster-defining threshold of Z > 2.3 and a cluster threshold of P < 0.05 to render Z-statistic BOLD images. Results were interpreted and labeled by an expert neuroanatomist (M.T.d.S.).
In order to compare fMRI classical and Functionnectome outcomes, we constructed a histogram showcasing the count of activated voxels for each z value ranging from z = -4 to z = +4, at increments of 0.1. Following this, to evaluate any statistical difference between the methods, we conducted an ANOVA test with voxel count as the independent variable and the discretized z value as the dependent variable.
Reporting summary
Further information on research design is available in the Nature Portfolio Reporting Summary linked to this article.
Data availability
All fMRI results maps and dataset of the Z-values histogram are publicly available via OSF (https://osf.io/gb2nh/52). Z maps of the conjunction results are additionally available via Neurovault for a convenient visualization (https://neurovault.org/collections/15553/). The conditions of the ethics approvals of the datasets do not permit public archiving or sharing of anonymized raw study data.
Code availability
Analyses were carried out using open software and toolboxes (see methods section). The Functionnectome is an open-source software available at http://www.bcblab.com. All fMRI preprocessing routines and analyses were performed with FEAT, part of FSL (FMRIB’s Software Library, http://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl). Conjunction analysis was performed with the easythresh_conj FSL’s tool (https://warwick.ac.uk/fac/sci/statistics/staff/academic-research/nichols/scripts/fsl/easythresh_conj.sh).
References
Sutherland, S. The Macmillan Dictionary of Psychology (Bloomsbury Publishing, 1995).
Seth, A. K. & Bayne, T. Theories of consciousness. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 23, 439–452 (2022).
Baars, B. A Cognitive Theory of Consciousness (Cambridge University Press, 1988).
Tononi, G., Boly, M., Massimini, M. & Koch, C. Integrated information theory: from consciousness to its physical substrate. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 17, 450–461 (2016).
Oizumi, M., Albantakis, L. & Tononi, G. From the phenomenology to the mechanisms of consciousness: integrated information theory 3.0. PLoS Comput. Biol. 10, e1003588 (2014).
Edelman, G. M., Gally, J. A. & Baars, B. J. Biology of consciousness. Front. Psychol. 2, 4 (2011).
Dehaene, S. et al. Cerebral mechanisms of word masking and unconscious repetition priming. Nat. Neurosci. 4, 752–758 (2001).
Van Gaal, S., Ridderinkhof, K. R., Scholte, H. S. & Lamme, V. A. F. Unconscious activation of the prefrontal no-go network. J. Neurosci. 30, 4143–4150 (2010).
Boly, M. et al. Baseline brain activity fluctuations predict somatosensory perception in humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 104, 12187–12192 (2007).
Sadaghiani, S., Hesselmann, G. & Kleinschmidt, A. Distributed and antagonistic contributions of ongoing activity fluctuations to auditory stimulus detection. J. Neurosci. 29, 13410–13417 (2009).
Dehaene, S. & Changeux, J. P. Experimental and theoretical approaches to conscious processing. Neuron 70, 200–227 (2011).
Friston, K. J. Modalities, modes, and models in functional neuroimaging. Science 326, 399–403 (2009).
de Schotten, M. T. & Forkel, S. J. The emergent properties of the connected brain. Science 378, 505–510 (2022).
Nozais, V., Theaud, G., Descoteaux, M., Thiebaut de Schotten, M. & Petit, L. Improved Functionnectome by dissociating the contributions of white matter fiber classes to functional activation. Brain Struct. Funct. 228, 2165–2177 (2023).
Nozais, V., Forkel, S. J., Foulon, C., Petit, L. & Thiebaut de Schotten, M. Functionnectome: a framework to analyse the contribution of brain circuits to fMRI. Commun. Biol. 4, 1035 (2021).
Nozais, V. et al. Atlasing white matter and grey matter joint contributions to resting-state networks in the human brain. Commun. Biol. 6, 1–11 (2023).
Javad, F. et al. Auditory tracts identified with combined fMRI and diffusion tractography. Neuroimage 84, 562–574 (2014).
Martín-Signes, M., Paz-Alonso, P. M. & Chica, A. B. Connectivity of frontoparietal regions reveals executive attention and consciousness interactions. Cereb. Cortex 29, 4539–4550 (2019).
Chica, A. B., Paz-Alonso, P. M., Valero-Cabre, A. & Bartolomeo, P. Neural bases of the interactions between spatial attention and conscious perception. Cereb. Cortex 23, 1269–1279 (2013).
Chica, A. B., Bayle, D. J., Botta, F., Bartolomeo, P. & Paz-Alonso, P. M. Interactions between phasic alerting and consciousness in the fronto-striatal network. Sci. Rep. 6, 31868 (2016).
Thiebaut de Schotten, M. et al. Direct evidence for a parietal-frontal pathway subserving spatial awareness in humans. Science 309, 2226–2228 (2005).
Dalla Barba, G. et al. Different patterns of confabulation in left visuo-spatial neglect. Exp. Brain Res 236, 2037–2046 (2018).
Theyel, B. B., Llano, D. A. & Sherman, S. M. The corticothalamocortical circuit drives higher-order cortex in the mouse. Nat. Neurosci. 13, 84–88 (2009).
Shine, J. M., Lewis, L. D., Garrett, D. D. & Hwang, K. The impact of the human thalamus on brain-wide information processing. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 24, 416–430 (2023).
MacLean, M. W., Hadid, V., Spreng, R. N. & Lepore, F. Revealing robust neural correlates of conscious and unconscious visual processing: activation likelihood estimation meta-analyses. Neuroimage 273, 120088 (2023).
Thiebaut de Schotten, M. et al. A lateralized brain network for visuospatial attention. Nat. Neurosci. 14, 1245–1247 (2011).
Quentin, R., Chanes, L., Vernet, M. & Valero-Cabré, A. Fronto-parietal anatomical connections influence the modulation of conscious visual perception by high-beta frontal oscillatory activity. Cereb. Cortex 25, 2095–2101 (2015).
Martín-Signes, M., Pérez-Serrano, C. & Chica, A. B. Causal contributions of the SMA to alertness and consciousness interactions. Cereb. Cortex 29, 648–656 (2019).
Thiebaut De Schotten, M. et al. Damage to white matter pathways in subacute and chronic spatial neglect: a group study and 2 single-case studies with complete virtual ‘in vivo’ tractography dissection. Cereb. Cortex 24, 691–706 (2014).
Spagna, A. et al. The cost of attentional reorienting on conscious visual perception: an MEG study. Cereb. Cortex 33, 2048–2060 (2023).
Liu, J. et al. Fronto-parietal networks shape human conscious report through attention gain and reorienting. Commun. Biol. 6, 1–14 (2023).
Parlatini, V. et al. Functional segregation and integration within fronto-parietal networks. Neuroimage 146, 367–375 (2017).
Ungerleider, L. G. & Mishkin, M. Two cortical visual systems. In Analysis of Visual Behavior (eds Ingle, D. J. et al.) 549–586 (The MIT Press, 1982).
Gazzaniga, M. S. Cerebral specialization and interhemispheric communication: does the corpus callosum enable the human condition? Brain 123, 1293–1326 (2000).
Geschwind, N. Disconnexion syndromes in animals and man. Part II. Brain 88, 585 (1965).
Lunven, M. et al. White matter lesional predictors of chronic visual neglect: a longitudinal study. Brain 138, 746–760 (2015).
Bozzali, M. et al. Microstructural damage of the posterior corpus callosum contributes to the clinical severity of neglect. PLoS ONE 7, e48079 (2012).
Liyana Arachige, M., Seneviratne, U., John, N., Ma, H. & Phan, T. G. Mapping topography and network of brain injury in patients with disorders of consciousness. Front. Neurol. 14, 1027160 (2023).
Pozeg, P. et al. Structural connectivity in recovery after coma: connectome atlas approach. Neuroimage Clin. 37, 103358 (2023).
Kronemer, S. I. et al. Human visual consciousness involves large scale cortical and subcortical networks independent of task report and eye movement activity. Nat. Commun. 13, 7342 (2022).
Logothetis, N. K., Pauls, J., Augath, M., Trinath, T. & Oeltermann, A. Neurophysiological investigation of the basis of the fMRI signal. Nature 412, 150–157 (2001).
Logothetis, N. K. What we can do and what we cannot do with fMRI. Nature 453, 869–878 (2008).
Dale, A. M. Optimal experimental design for event-related fMRI. Hum. Brain Mapp. 8, 109–114 (1999).
Woolrich, M. W., Ripley, B. D., Brady, M., & Smith, S. M. Temporal autocorrelation in univariate linear modeling of FMRI data. NeuroImage 14, 1370–1386 (2001).
Smith, S. M. Fast robust automated brain extraction. Hum. Brain Mapp. 17, 143–155 (2002).
Jenkinson, M., Bannister, P., Brady, M. & Smith, S. Improved optimization for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain images. Neuroimage 17, 825–841 (2002).
Smith, S. M. & Brady, J. M. SUSAN—a new approach to low level image processing. Int. J. Comput. Vis. 23, 45–78 (1997).
Vu, A. T. et al. High resolution whole brain diffusion imaging at 7T for the Human Connectome Project. Neuroimage 122, 318–331 (2015).
Thiebaut de Schotten, M., Foulon, C. & Nachev, P. Brain disconnections link structural connectivity with function and behaviour. Nat. Commun. 11, 5094 (2020).
Woolrich, M. W., Behrens, T. E. J., Beckmann, C. F., Jenkinson, M. & Smith, S. M. Multilevel linear modelling for FMRI group analysis using Bayesian inference. Neuroimage 21, 1732–1747 (2004).
Nichols, T., Brett, M., Andersson, J., Wager, T. & Poline, J. B. Valid conjunction inference with the minimum statistic. Neuroimage 25, 653–660 (2005).
Martín-Signes, M., Chica, A. B., Bartolomeo, P. & Thiebaut de Schotten, M. Streams of conscious visual experience. Available at: osf.io/gb2nh (2024).
Acknowledgements
M.M.S. is supported by a Margarita Salas fellowship by the Spanish Ministry of Universities and the European Next Generation funding, and by a contract for Young Researchers (PAIDI 2020) by the Ministry of Economy, Knowledge, Enterprise, and Universities of Andalusia. A.B.C. is supported by research project PID2020-119033 GB-I00 funded by MCIN/AEI/10.13039/501100011033, and by “ERDF A way of making Europe”, by the European Union; and by FEDER/Junta de Andalucía-Consejería de Economía y Conocimiento/ project A.SEJ.090. UGR18. P.B. is supported by the Agence Nationale de la Recherche through ANR-16-CE37-0005 and ANR-10-IAIHU-06, and by the Fondation pour la Recherche sur les AVC through FR-AVC-017. M.T.d.S is supported by HORIZON- INFRA-2022 SERV (Grant No. 101147319) “EBRAINS 2.0: A Research Infrastructure to Advance Neuroscience and Brain Health”, by the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme under the European Research Council (ERC) Consolidator grant agreement No. 818521 (DISCONNECTOME), the University of Bordeaux’s IdEx ‘Investments for the Future’ program RRI ‘IMPACT’, and the IHU ‘Precision & Global Vascular Brain Health Institute – VBHI’ funded by the France 2030 initiative (ANR-23-IAHU-0001).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
M.M.S. and M.T.d.S.: conceptualization, formal analysis, writing—original draft, writing—review and editing. A.B.C. and P.B.: writing—review and editing.
Corresponding authors
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests. M.T.d.S. is an Editorial Board Member for Communications Biology but was not involved in the editorial review of, nor the decision to publish this article.
Peer review
Peer review information
Communications Biology thanks Luiz Pessoa, Pablo Barttfeld and the other, anonymous, reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work. Primary Handling Editors: Benjamin Bessieres. A peer review file is available.
Additional information
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Martín-Signes, M., Chica, A.B., Bartolomeo, P. et al. Streams of conscious visual experience. Commun Biol 7, 908 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1038/s42003-024-06593-9
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s42003-024-06593-9
Comments
By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.