Abstract
The aging brain undergoes major changes in its topology. The mechanisms by which the brain mitigates age-associated changes in topology to maintain robust control of brain networks are unknown. Here we use diffusion MRI data from cognitively intact participants (nā=ā480, ages 40ā90) to study age-associated differences in the average controllability of structural brain networks, topological features that could mitigate these differences, and the overall effect on cognitive function. We find age-associated declines in average controllability in control hubs and large-scale networks, particularly within the frontoparietal control and default mode networks. Further, we find that redundancy, a hypothesized mechanism of reserve, quantified via the assessment of multi-step paths within networks, mitigates the effects of topological differences on average network controllability. Lastly, we discover that average network controllability, redundancy, and grey matter volume, each uniquely contribute to predictive models of cognitive function. In sum, our results highlight the importance of redundancy for robust control of brain networks and in cognitive function in healthy-aging.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
As populations world-wide are aging1, dementia and other degenerative central nervous system diseases associated with cognitive decline are projected to increase in prevalence2. Cognitive decline is not restricted to pathological aging, but also occurs in healthy older adults. Yet healthy cognitive aging can vary greatly between individuals3. For those that resist cognitive decline, greater life-satisfaction, well-being, and higher levels of happiness are reported4. Several lifestyle factors have been found to contribute to successful cognitive aging, such as exercise5, and education6,7, yet the mechanisms that could support cognitive function late in life remain incompletely understood.
Studying the topological properties of macroscopic brain connectivity with tools from network science8 is one method by which the mechanisms that could promote cognitive function in aging were examined. Studies focused on measures of network topology that change throughout healthy9,10,11,12,13,14,15,16,17, and pathological aging18,19,20,21,22,23, and attempted to relate alterations in topology to cognition. One such central measure is network controllability24. Controllability is a concept that originated in engineering within the domain of control theory25,26,27. In networks, controllability examines the ability of key nodes to enable dynamic state transitions between an initial and target state24. The two most commonly studied forms of network controllability are average and modal controllability. Average controllability reflects a nodeās ability to push the network into an easy to reach state28. In the brain the default mode network, a collection of brain regions more active at rest29,30, and believed to contain general priors for cognitive function31, has been observed to have several hubs of average controllability28. This positions the default mode network to easily direct the brain from a resting state towards activity relevant for behavioral tasks30,32. In contrast, modal controllability quantifies a nodeās ability to push the network into difficult to reach states33,34, which has been shown to be important in brain networks associated with cognitive control28,35.
Network controllability has been postulated as a dual mechanism of brain and cognitive reserve in aging by combining structural connectivity, typically viewed as a mechanism of brain reserve, and brain dynamics believed to be necessary to support general cognitive processes to jointly measure the brainās ability to respond and adapt to changing cognitive demands as a unified form of reserve36. In this framework, age-associated cognitive decline can be viewed as a breakdown of network control, in which individuals have difficulty transitioning to, or maintaining, specific brain states relevant for cognitive function. Controllability metrics enable researchers to estimate the ability of dynamic interactions between brain regions, mediated by the connections between them, to drive the brain towards brain states relevant for cognitive activity, and thus support the cognitive processes associated with cognitive reserve. Recent work has found that longitudinal changes in the modal controllability of cognitive control systems in aging could underlie age-associated declines in executive function37,38. Other work demonstrated that the ability of temporal-parietal regions to control other brain regions decreases with age, and is particularly vulnerable to simulated lesions39. However, it remains unknown how average controllability is influenced by aging, and whether these putative changes affect cognition. The properties of underlying network topology that could mitigate differences in average controllability in aging and thus facilitate reserve are also unknown.
A mechanism we hypothesize that the brain may use to mitigate age-associated alterations in average controllability is through increased redundancy40,41,42,43. Redundancy is a general principle ubiquitous in engineering that protects systems from the failure of individual components44. Redundancy is also evident in biological systems at many scales. Examples include at the level of genes41,45, organs43, and in population coding within neural networks46. In the context of brain networks, redundant paths are hypothesized to provide alternate routes for information transmission that could serve as a form of brain reserve47,48,49 supporting information transmission if some paths fail due to the effects of aging and/or disease. Redundant links have been previously identified as potential mechanisms that support robust control of complex networks during disconnections24,50,51, but this has not been investigated in the context of network control in aging brain networks. Furthermore, the compensatory effects of redundancy have been postulated as a neuroprotective mechanism43, but only recently studied within the context of healthy and pathological aging12,19,23. It was reported that functional hippocampal redundancy supports cognitive resilience in pathological aging19,23, and that network-wide functional redundancy mediates the relationships between age and executive function12. However, redundancy has yet to be investigated in the context of structural brain networks and the alterations in controllability they undergo in aging. We hypothesize that increased redundancy could mitigate the effect of age-associated topological degradations to enable robust average controllability of brain networks.
It was recently hypothesized that network controllability and brain volume, a more traditional measure of brain reserve, should each explain unique variance related to cognitive status36. In particular, because it is unlikely that reserve related to volumetric properties of the brain is entirely explained by the organizational properties of structural networks, like network controllability, they should have additive predictive value as proxies of cognitive function. In the current study we investigated this aforementioned hypothesis, as well as the relevance of redundancy in structural brain networks as a potential mechanism of brain reserve. We chose processing speed as the cognitive function evaluated because it is believed to be heavily dependent on communication along white-matter tracts52,53. Relatedly, processing speed is known to exhibit age-associated declines54, that correspond with changing topological properties of structural brain networks55,56. Processing speed is also associated with commonly used measures of brain reserve, such as hippocampal volume57,58. We hypothesized that processing speed would be related to measures of regional influence on network dynamics, such as average controllability, particularly in functional networks that have been reported as important in age-related differences in processing speed (e.g., default mode and frontoparietal control networks15,59). We expected that redundancy could support rapid communication between task-relevant functional networks52,53,60, and thus be positively associated with processing speed.
To test our hypotheses we used diffusion MRI (dMRI) data from 480 participants (femaleā=ā281, maleā=ā199) between the ages of 40ā90 from the HCP-aging dataset61 (Fig.Ā 1a). We examined how average controllability, a regionās ability to push a network into easy to reach states, changes in aging. We constructed structural networks using the functional Schaefer local-global parcellation62 (Fig.Ā 1b). For results presented in the main text, networks were thresholded by removing edges with streamline counts below 0.001 multiplied by the maximum streamline count per network. To investigate the robustness of our results, we repeated analyses with thresholds of 0.005, 0.010, and 0.015. After constructing structural networks, we computed average controllability for each brain region (Fig.Ā 1c), and then identified age-related differences in average controllability of control hubs and large-scale networks (Fig.Ā 1c). We hypothesized that the average controllability of control hubs and large-scale networks would be negatively related to participantsā age. Next, we investigated how redundancy, a measure of multi-step paths between nodes, supports average controllability in middle- and old-aged participants (Fig.Ā 1d). Due to previous work that has highlighted the role of redundancy for control in complex systems24,50,51,63, we hypothesized that differences in network redundancy could mediate age-associated differences in in network controllability in brain networks (Fig.Ā 1e). Finally, we investigated the extent to which grey matter volume, network controllability, and redundancy, could serve as partial proxies of age-associated variance in processing speed (Fig.Ā 1f). We hypothesized that both redundancy and average controllability would serve as partial proxies of cognitive function that were complementary to the traditional measure of reserve in grey matter volume.
Results
Hubs of average controllability are largely consistent in middle- and old-aged adults
To begin our investigation of the relationship between network control and aging, we evaluated if the average controllability of control hubs in middle-aged participants (nā=ā305, ages 40ā65) were different than in old-aged participants (nā=ā175, ages 65ā90). We classified a node as a control hub if its average controllability was greater than one standard deviation above the mean average controllability for all nodes in middle-aged participants. This yielded 15 hubs, which were predominately within networks associated with cognitive function (Fig.Ā 2a). These hubs were mostly consistent across global network thresholds of 0.001, 0.005, 0.010, 0.015 (Supplementary TablesĀ S1āS4). However, the 15th hub DefaultB ā PFCd_1 was only classified as a hub at lower global network thresholds of 0.001, and 0.005 (Supplementary TablesĀ S1āS2). The distribution of hubs within the different large-scale networks (shown as percentages in Fig.Ā 2a) were corrected by network size by normalizing the number of hubs in each network by their respective size28. Hubs of average controllability were most commonly within the default mode network (~40%), followed by the salience/ventral attention network (~25%), similar to previously reported results28. Next, we examined if average controllability for each of these identified hubs was different between middle- and old-aged participants (Fig.Ā 2b). For the hubs with the greatest average controllability, the mean values were consistent across age groups. However, old-aged participants had less average controllability in two hubs within the default mode network (DefaultA ā PFCm_4: Fā=ā9.78, pbonf.ā=ā0.028, DefaultB ā PFCd_1: Fā=ā28.13, pbonf.ā=ā9.46eā08). The differences in average controllability between middle- and old-aged participants for the hub DefaultA ā PFCm_4, remained significant at all thresholds tested (Supplementary TablesĀ S1āS4), whereas the hub classification of DefaultB ā PFCd_1 was sensitive to higher thresholds of 0.010, 0.015 (Supplementary TablesĀ S3 and S4). To assess the extent to which the hubs were dependent on our choice of hub threshold, we performed an additional analysis identifying hubs as regions within the top 10% of average controllability, which corresponds to 40 nodes in our parcellation, and repeated the analysis with the global network thresholds of 0.001, 0.005, 0.010, 0.015 (Supplementary TablesĀ S5āS8). The previously identified hub DefaultB ā PFCd_1, was the only region to show significant differences between middle- and old-aged participants after correction for multiple comparisons.
Declines in mean average controllability of large-scale networks are implicated in aging
Next, we investigated if average controllability showed age-associated differences at the level of large-scale networks. We calculated the mean average controllability for each of the 17 large-scale networks in our parcellation, and computed the ranked Spearmanās correlation with age. Age was negatively associated with mean average controllability in the default mode (DefaultB: Spearmanās Ļā=āā0.307, pbonf.ā=ā1.15eā10), the frontoparietal control (ContB: Spearmanās Ļā=āā0.279, pbonf.ā=ā8.92eā09), and the limbic (LimbicB: Spearmanās Ļā=āā0.223, pbonf.ā=ā8.92eā09) networks (Fig.Ā 2c and Supplementary TableĀ S9). Significant associations between age and mean network average controllability were found for these networks across all global network thresholds tested (Supplementary TablesĀ S9āS12). For the frontoparietal control network, this decline appeared to occur mostly before the age of 61 (Supplementary Fig.Ā S1a), whereas for the default mode and limbic networks, these declines continued throughout the age range studied (Supplementary Fig.Ā S1b, c, respectively).
Minimal sex-related differences in mean network average controllability and relationships with age
Sex-related differences are crucial to understand when investigating the relationships between brain connectivity and aging. To evaluate the extent to which males and females differed in mean network average controllability, we performed ANCOVAs, with participantās age and years of education included as covariates. In most networks, we did not observe sex-related differences. However, females showed lower mean network average controllability in the default mode (DefaultA: Fā=ā13.68, pbonf.ā=ā0.004) and the salience/ventral attention (SalVentAttnB: Fā=ā10.79, pbonf.ā=ā0.018) (Supplementary TableĀ S13) networks. These results were consistent across thresholds assessed (Supplementary TablesĀ S13āS16). Next, we assessed the degree to which associations between mean average network controllability and age differed between the sexes. We first computed sex-specific rank correlations between mean network average controllability and age for 17 networks. Then we computed the significance of these differences by computing the z-scored differences in correlation values using the Fischerās z-transform. Males showed stronger negative relationships between age and mean network average controllability in the default mode (DefaultB) and frontoparietal control (ContB) networks, but no correlations were significantlyĀ different between the sexes across all thresholds assessed (Supplementary TablesĀ S17āS20). Since we observed minimal sex-related differences in mean network average controllability, and no significant sex-related differences for the relationships between mean network average controllability and age for each of the 17 networks, we did not partition participants by sex in any of the following analyses.
Nodal degree and redundancy show similar relationships to average controllability
To examine if multi-step connectivity was related to average controllability, we calculated network redundancy42, defined as number of non-circular paths between nodes up to a designated length L (Lā=ā4 in our study, see Methods). We summed this measure at the node level, to get the total number of paths to and from each node for each participantās structural brain network. Similarly, we calculated nodal degree for all nodes by summing the number of first-order edges that each node was involved in. Since redundancy is typically calculated on binary networks42, we calculated both degree and redundancy on binarized adjacency matrices where Aijā=ā1 if a streamline existed between nodes i and j, else Aijā=ā0. After computing nodal degree and nodal redundancy for all brain regions in the parcellation, we averaged these values, as well as the previously computed average controllability values for each node, across participants to get group level nodal degree, nodal redundancy, and nodal average controllability values. Next, we performed rank correlations between these network features and average controllability (Supplementary Fig.Ā S2a). Both degree and redundancy showed strong relationships with average controllability (degree: Spearmanās Ļā=ā0.823, pā=ā1eā99, Supplementary Fig.Ā S2a.i, redundancy: Spearmanās Ļā=ā0.804, pā=ā7eā92, Supplementary Fig.Ā S2a.ii). To investigate the relationship between multi-step pathways and average controllability, we then performed a rank-correlation between degree-regressed redundancy and degree-regressed average controllability. Degree regressed redundancy and degree-regressed average controllability still maintained a positive relationship (Spearmanās Ļā=ā0.444, pā=ā1.1eā20, Supplementary Fig.Ā S2b).
Degree mediates differences in average controllability with age
Before assessing the influence of redundancy in age-associated differences in average controllability, we began by examining the importance of edges immediately connecting to nodes via degree. The average degree in 15 of 17 networks showed negative rank-correlations with age across all thresholds tested (pbonfā<ā0.05, see Supplementary TablesĀ S21āS24). For the threshold of 0.001(Fig.Ā 3a), the strongest of these associations were in the dorsal attention network (DorsAttnA: Spearmanāsā=āā0.464, pbonf.ā=ā9.90eā26) and the salience/ventral attention network (SalVentAttnA: Spearmanās Ļā=āā0.446, pbonf.ā=ā1.44eā23) (Supplementary TableĀ S21). With the strong relationships between degree and average controllability, we expected that average network degree should influence the association between age and average controllability. We assessed this putative relationship by testing if degree mediated age-related differences in mean network average controllability. We performed mediation analyses for each of the 17 networks, and found that degree influenced the relationship between age and average controllability for 14 of 17 networks (all pbonf.āsā<ā0.05, see Supplementary TableĀ S17) (Fig.Ā 3b), with the degree of the limbic network having the strongest impact on the relationship between its mean network average controllability and age (LimbicB, Ī²ā=ā0.0149, pbonf.ā<ā1eā20). These 14 networks consistently mediated the relationship between mean network average controllability and age across thresholds (Supplementary TablesĀ S25āS28).
Redundancy mediates differences in average controllability with age over and above the effects of degree
We next turned towards examining the effects of redundancy in the relationship between age and average controllability. First, we calculated the average nodal redundancy for each network within our parcellation, and calculated a Spearmanās rank correlation with age. Similar to degree, average network redundancy shows widespread negative relationships with age (Fig.Ā 3c). Redundancy in the frontoparietal control network showed the strongest negative relationship with age for the global threshold of 0.001 (ContA: Spearmanās Ļā=āā0.414, pbonf.ā=ā5.26eā20, Supplementary TableĀ S29), but all negative associations were significant across all thresholds (all pbonf.āsā<ā0.05; see Supplementary TablesĀ S29ā32). Next, we investigated if the multi-step connectivity indexed by redundancy influenced the relationship between mean network averageĀ controllability and age, over and above the effects of degree. For each large-scale network in our parcellation, we performed a mediation analysis between age and average controllability with redundancy as the mediator, and included average degree of the respective large-scale networks as covariates. Redundancy mediated the relationship between age and mean network average controllability in 3 of 17 networks, over and above the effects of degree (all pbonf.āsā<ā0.05, see Supplementary TableĀ S5) (Fig.Ā 3d). This included the default mode network (DefaultB: Ī²ā=ā0.0005, pbonf.ā<ā0.014), which exhibited an age-associated decline in average controllability (Fig.Ā 2c), the dorsal attention network (DorsAttnB: Ī²ā=ā0.0008, pbonf.ā<ā0.010), and the visual network (VisPeri: Ī²ā=ā0.002, pbonf.ā<ā1eā20). Of these networks, the mediations performed by redundancy were significant across all thresholds for the dorsal attention (DorsAttnB) and default model (DefaultB) networks (Supplementary TablesĀ S33āS36).
Average controllability and redundancy show moderate relationships in brain networks when controlling for participant age
Due to similarities in the computation of redundancy and average controllability, it is possible that the results from our mediation analyses are primarily driven by correlations between redundancy and average controllability, rather than empirical observations specific to age-associated variance in brain networks. To investigate this possibility, we performed partial correlations between mean network average controllability and average network redundancy, while controlling for average network degree, participant age, and education, across global network thresholds of 0.001, 0.005, 0.010, 0.015 (Supplementary TablesĀ S37āS40). For the lowest threshold tested, 0.001 (Supplementary TableĀ S37), we did not see significant relationships for any of the subnetworks that were identified in our mediation analyses (DefaultB, DorsAttnB, Supplementary TablesĀ S33āS36). Moreover, only the default mode network (DefaultA) showed a relationship between mean network average controllability and average network redundancy, and this relationship was negative. For the second threshold, 0.005 (Supplementary TableĀ S38), there were positive associations between redundancy and mean network average controllability in 4 of the 17 networks, and a negative association with the default mode network identified in the previous mediation analyses (DefaultB). For the remaining thresholds we observed positive relationships between mean network average controllability and average network redundancy in the dorsal attention network (DorsAttnB), but not in the aforementioned default mode network (DefaultB) (Supplementary TablesĀ S39 and S40).
Average controllability and redundancy are associated with processing speed
After focusing on age-associated variance in average controllability and network properties that contribute to it, we studied the relationships between average controllability, redundancy, and cognitive function. We first associated the mean network average controllability for each large-scale network with processing speed, assessed by the Pattern Comparison Processing Speed Test64. We hypothesized that processing speed would be related to measures of influence on overall network dynamics, such as average controllability. We found that processing speed was positively associated with average controllability in the frontoparietal control (ContB: Spearmanās Ļā=ā0.144, pbonf.ā=ā0.026) and the default mode (DefaultB: Spearmanās Ļā=ā0.174, pbonf.ā=ā0.002) networks (Fig.Ā 4a). These relationships were consistent across all thresholds examined (Supplementary TablesĀ S41āS44). Next, we associated average network redundancy with processing speed. Redundancy in 6 of 17 large-scale networks was positively related to processing speed (all pbonf.āsā<ā0.05) (Fig.Ā 4b). This also included the frontoparietal control network (ContB: Spearmanās Ļā=ā0.168, pbonf.ā=ā0.004), but redundancy in other networks was also positively associated with processing speed, including the salience/ventral attention (SalVentAttnB: Spearmanās Ļā=ā0.169, pbonf.ā=ā0.004), limbic (LimbicB: Spearmanās Ļā=ā0.166, pbonf.ā=ā0.005), visual (VisCent: Spearmanās Ļā=ā0.156, pbonf.ā=ā0.01), and dorsal attention (DorsAttnA: Spearmanās Ļā=ā0.15, pbonf.ā=ā0.008) networks. These networks exhibited significant positive relationships across all thresholds tested (Supplementary TablesĀ S45āS48). However, other networks including the temporal parietal (TempPar), default mode (DefaultA), salience/ventral attention (SalVentAttnA) and somatomotor (SomMotA) networks also showed significant positive relationships at higher thresholds of 0.005, 0.010, and 0.015 (Supplementary TablesĀ S46āS48).
Hippocampal grey matter volume is positively associated with processing speed in older participants
Next, we investigated the association between hippocampal grey matter (GM) volume, one of the most commonly used measures of brain reserve, and processing speed. We only expected hippocampal volume to be a mechanism of brain reserve when declines in volume began in normal aging. To determine when decline starts within our participants, we used a piece-wise linear regression that identifies breakpoints in a data-driven manner. We found that hippocampal volume experiences a non-significant but positive trend between the ages of 40ā66.92 (rā=ā0.103, pā=ā0.063), after which hippocampal volume in our participants showed significant age-associated decline (rā=āā0.36, pā=ā5.70eā06) (Fig.Ā 4c). Total subcortical GM volume showed a similar trajectory, with a breakpoint at age 65 (Supplementary Fig.Ā S3a), and total cortical GM volume showed continual decline throughout ages 40ā90, with the most rapid decline occurring after age 75.33 (Supplementary Fig.Ā S3b). Then, we used the residual method65 to determine if total hippocampal volume was a marker of cognitive reserve for participants with age >66.92. We found that total hippocampal volume, when adjusting for total intracranial volume66, was positively associated with processing speed in this older subset of subjects (rā=ā0.187, pā=ā0.021) (Fig.Ā 4d).
Controllability, redundancy, and grey matter volume are synergistically associated with cognitive performance
Finally, we examined if mean network average controllability, and GM volume served as complementary predictors of cognitive function in our participants. With the subset of participants older than the previously identified breakpoint for hippocampal volume (ages >66.92, nā=ā151, M/Fā=ā67/84) we trained GLMs to predict processing speed using various combinations of GM volume, mean network average controllability, and average network redundancy for each of the 17 functional networks (Fig.Ā 4e). For GM volume, we used total hippocampal volume, as well as subcortical and cortical volume. GM volume and average controllability did appear to have an almost entirely complementary effect on predicting cognition, yielding an R2ā=ā0.115, versus an R2ā=ā0.036 for GM alone, and R2ā=ā0.081 for average controllability alone. However, GM volume and redundancy showed better performance (R2ā=ā0.171), although there was more overlap in the predictive power between these features (redundancy: R2ā=ā0.155). The best model included all three sets of features (R2ā=ā0.233) (Fig.Ā 4f and TableĀ 1). These results were similar across all global network thresholds tested (TableĀ 1, and Supplementary Tables S49āS51), for 0.001, 0.005, 0.010, and 0.015, respectively).
Discussion
In this study we examined whether age-associated differences in the average controllability of brain networks is mitigated by redundancy. We found age-associated differences in the average controllability of structural networks within our functional parcellation in the default mode (DefaultB), frontoparietal control (ContB), and limbic (LimbicB) networks. Additionally, two control hubs within the default mode network showed declines in average controllability among old-aged participants. Furthermore, we investigated the extent to which these differences were influenced by the presence of single-step and multi-step pathways between brain regions. Degree, our measure of single-step connectivity, influenced age-associated differences in average controllability in 14 of the 17 functional networks. However, multi -step paths indicative of redundancy in the system19,23, mediated the relationships between age and average controllability in 3 of 17 networks, these included the visual (VisPeri), dorsal attention (DorsAttnB, SalVentAttnB), and default mode (DefaultB) networks. Finally, we investigated a previously posed hypothesis, that network controllability and GM volume, a more traditional measure of brain reserve, should each be partial proxies of cognitive function36. When using simple linear models, our results were consistent with this hypothesis. However, both redundancy and average controllability appeared to provide complementary predictive power when predicting the processing speed abilities of healthy older adults.
Age related differences in average controllability
Structural networks reorganize in brain aging16. Despite these differences in network organization associated with age, mechanisms that mitigate these differences to maintain average controllability have been relatively understudied. In the present study, we evaluated differences in average controllability associated with aging in control hubs and in the structural connectivity of large-scale brain networks. We identified 15 hubs of average controllability, the first 14 of which were robust to threshold selection. The top 13 exhibited similar levels ofĀ average controllability between middle-aged and old-aged participants. While the 14th and 15th hubs, which were within the default mode network, exhibited less average controllability in old-aged participants. Many of these hubs were in the precuneus, and posterior cingulate, overlapping with previously identified average control hubs28, and regions identified as the structural core67. However, for two hubs in the default mode network, old-aged participants showed less average controllability than middle-aged participants. Both of these hubs were in the prefrontal cortex (PFC), one in the medial prefrontal cortex (PFCm), and the other in the dorsal prefrontal cortex (PFCd). The PFC experiences age-related declines in brain volume68,69 and white matter integrity68. Increased task-based brain activity in the PFC is commonly reported as a potential mechanism to compensate for declines in brain volume and white matter44 (for a review see refs. 48,70). Our results suggest that increased compensatory PFC activation could also be related to declines in the average controllability of hubs within the default mode network, providing further support to the possibility of network controllability as a measure linking brain and cognitive reserve36.
Multi-step connectivity (redundancy) influences age-associated differences in average controllability
Nodal degree, a measure of the number edges connected to a particular node, has been shown to strongly predict nodal controllability within subjects24,28,71,72,73. However, the additional properties that influence controllability are largely unknown. In this study we investigated the relevance of redundant multi-step paths to brain network controllability40,41,42. We found that redundancy was positively associated with average controllability, when adjusted for degree, suggesting that multi-step pathways could play a crucial role in the control profiles of complex networks. Furthermore, our mediation analyses indicated that redundancy, while holding degree as a covariate, supported the average controllability of several key networks for cognitive function in aging. These included the default mode (DefaultB), dorsal attention DorsAttnB),Ā and visual (VisPeri) networks. Of these networks, the default and limbic networks identified showed age-associated declines in average controllability, which indicates that redundancy could be a neuroprotective mechanism to mitigate these declines43. This work aligns with findings in other complex systems suggesting that edge redundancy can promote robust average controllability24, particularly in the context of changing network topologies, such as edge removal50,51, which is similar to weakening white matter connectivity observed in aging74,75. Our work suggests that the existence of multi-step pathways in brain networks could provide bridges of connectivity that preserve average network controllability63, to support dynamic brain activity in aging.
Average controllability, redundancy, and processing speed
Processing speed in our study was assessed via the speed of pattern comparison64. This task requires several cognitive processes, such as visual search, working memory, and decision making. For both average controllability and redundancy, we found a positive association within the same subnetwork of the frontoparietal control network (ContB). The frontoparietal control network is important for allocation of attention, flexible goal-driven behavior, working memory, and decision making76,77. Furthermore, the global connectivity of the frontoparietal control network may allow it to influence brain-wide dynamics78. Our results suggest that the average controllability, and redundancy, of edges within the frontoparietal network could be important for enabling the diverse cognitive functions relevant in processing speed and other similar tasks. Furthermore, we found that processing speed was positively associated with redundancy in the dorsal attention (DorsAttnA), visual (VisCent), salience/ventral attention (SalVentAttnB) and limbic (LimbicB) networks, suggesting that increased number of communication pathways involving each of these networks could support guidance of top-down attention and discrimination in this visually-based processing speed task79,80,81,82.
Complementary effects of grey matter volume, average controllability, and redundancy on cognitive performance
In support of the hypothesis that GM volume and network controllability could each be partial proxies of cognitive function36, we found that GM volume and average controllability had almost complementary effects on the goodness of fit for our modelās prediction of processing speed. GM volume and redundancy also improved performance together versus when considering either of them alone, but the additive effect on the goodness of fit was not as dramatic. This is not surprising, as hippocampal volume has been previously associated with redundancy in brain networks23. Additionally, we found that average controllability and redundancy showed highly complementary effects in predicting processing speed, despite similarities in their calculation. We propose that this is primarily due to the within-subject normalization performed when calculating average controllability which could mask age-associated variance between subjects. However, model performance was the best when including GM volume, average controllability, and redundancy in a single model, suggesting that they each could play an important role in cognitive function in healthy-aging.
Mathematical considerations regarding the similarity between average controllability and redundancy
There are similarities in the computation of redundancy42 and average controllability28 that should be considered while interpreting our results. In particular, redundancy and average controllability can both be viewed as measuring the number ofĀ indirect paths associated with a node83,84. From this perspective, it is possible that redundancy mediating the relationship between age and mean network average controllability in several networks could be fundamentally aĀ mathematical, rather than an empirical observation driven by age-associated variance in brain networks. However, when controlling for age within our participants, we did not find consistent relationships between average controllability and redundancy within the default mode network (DefaultB), suggesting that the observed indirect effect of DefaultB redundancy on the relationship between age and average controllability in this network is dependent on age-associated differences. Furthermore, if redundancy and average controllability indexed extremely similar features, their ability to predict processing speed should not have been as complementary as observed. There are two key differences in the computation of redundancy and average controllability. The first is that during the computation of average controllability, adjacency matrices are normalized by dividing by a constant plus the largest eigenvector. Second, average controllability approximates the diffusion of information across an infinite time scale, whereas redundancy is finite. Infinite time-scale diffusion on other message-passing systems has been proven to cause over-smoothing85, leading to a steady state that primarily consists of information about the number of connected components86 and the loss of information related node-specific features85,87,88. This over-smoothing problem is present in controllability measures, as well as measures of communicability89, and other metrics that consider paths of infinite length on a network.
Greater mean network average controllability in males within the default mode and salience/ventral attention networks
Males and females exhibit different white matter network connectivity topologies in development90,91, adulthood92,93, and in aging94. In development, white matter differences contribute to differing network controllability profiles that are predictive of poorer executive function in males90. In adulthood, differing hemispheric connectivity contributes to males having higher average controllability in regions involving motor and auditory function, which could facilitate motor activation 93. In our study, males had higher mean network average controllability of the default mode (DefaultA), and salience/ventral attention (SalVentAttnB) networks of middle- and old-aged adults. We did not find that average controllability in these networks was related to processing speed, the only cognitive function assessed in this study. However, future studies could investigate if these differences are associated with measures of executive function in aging, where females tend to show more initial reserve, but faster decline after a tipping point is reached95,96.
Limitations and future directions
The goals of our study included assessing the extent to which redundancy could mitigate age-associated differences in network control, and evaluate network control in the context of traditional measures of brain reserve. We used mediation analyses within our study which relied on a cross-sectional sample. Cross-sectional age-associated variance does not always hold in longitudinal settings97, thus replication of our findings in a longitudinal setting would be ideal. When studying a traditional measure of reserve, we used the residual method65 to assess if increased hippocampal volume was positively associated with processing speed. While we did observe hippocampal atrophy (reduction in GM volume) in older participants, this method is primarily used in the context of neurodegenerative diseases65. Future studies may consider including participants with later stages of dementia-associated atrophy to further evaluate network controllability in the context of reserve. Another limitation of our study is that we performed limited investigation into sex-related differences in our results. Our initial analyses in this direction yielded minimal sex-related differences in mean network average controllability, and no significant differences in the relationships between mean network controllability and age for each of the 17 networks. Future work could expand on these results to consider if differences in network controllability drive observed sex-related differences in the rates of cognitive decline95,96, particularly for executive function, which has been associated with faster degradation of white matter tracts in females94. Finally, while we focused on average controllability in this study, future work could expand upon existing studies of modal controllability in aging, to further our understanding of how age-associated changes in network topology influence network controllability, and the relevance of these changes in cognitive and brain reserve.
Methods
Dataset and participants
We used preprocessed dMRI data obtained from the 2.0 release of the Human Connectome Project ā Aging database61. Ages of participants ranged from 40 to 90 (nā=ā646), of these we used data from a subset of 545 participants with valid cognitive assessments for the cognitive scores we used in our study. We restricted participants to those with normal cognitive function as assessed by the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA)98. For subjects older than 65, we used a cutoff of 23/30, which has been found to limit false diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment99. There are forms of dementia that the MoCA can be insensitive to, such as vascular dementia100, semantic dementia101 and frontotemporal dementia102, each with differing patterns of cognitive impairment. To reduce the likelihood of inclusion of participants with early stages of these forms of dementia, we also excluded subjects between the ages of 65ā90 with poor performance on measures of cognitive flexibility103,104, vocabulary comprehension103,105,106, and executive function17,103,104,107. Poor performance was defined as a performance level worse than two standard deviations below the mean. These restrictions removed another 65 participants from eligibility, leaving us with data from 480 (281 females, 199 males) participants for this study. All participants gave written informed consent and all procedures had been pre-approved by local Institutional Review Boards. All ethical regulations relevant to human research participants were followed.
Image acquisition and processing
T1-weighted structural images were acquired in a 3 Tesla Siemens Prisma Scanner. A multi-echo magnetization prepared rapid gradient echo (MPRAGE) sequence (voxel size: 0.8āĆā0.8āĆā0.8āmm, TEā=ā1.8/3.6/5.4/7.2āms, TRā=ā2500āms, flip angleā=ā8 degrees) was used. Diffusion MRI (dMRI) images were generated from multi-shell diffusion with b-values of 1500 and 3000ās/mm2, with 93 and 92 sampling directions, a slice thickness of 1.5āmm, and an in-plane resolution of 1.5āmm. We used preprocessed dMRI data for our study. For details on the preprocessing pipeline see: https://brain.labsolver.org/hcp_a.html. Briefly, the pipeline involved susceptibility artifact detection with the TOPOP, from the Tiny FSL package (http://github.com/frankyeh/TinyFSL), alignment with the AC-PC line, restricted diffusion imaging108, and generalized q-sampling109. These analyses were conducted at Extreme Science and Engineering Discovery Environment (XSEDE)110 resources using the allocation TG-CIS200026.
Network construction
Preprocessed dMRI data was reconstructed in DSI Studio (http://dsi-studio.labsolver.org). We performed whole-brain fiber tracking with 5,000,000 streamlines. (Fig.Ā 1a). Structural networks were constructed according to the Schaefer Local-Global cortical parcellation with 400 cortical regions62, which subdivides the human cortex into 17 large-scale (functional) networks (Fig.Ā 1b). Each brain parcel was considered a node, with the number of streamlines between any pair of parcels used as the weighted edge. Edges were removed if they were below 0.001 of the maximum edge weight per network. Additionally, we repeated major analyses with thresholds of 0.005, 0.01, 0.015, to assess the robustness of our results.
Average controllability calculations
Average controllability, defined as the average energy from a set of control nodes on dynamic state trajectory over all possible states, was calculated using the trace of the finite time controllability Gramian111. The finite time controllability Gramian is computed via:
Where A is the adjacency matrix, normalized by dividing my one-plus the largest absolute eigenvector, \({B}_{k}\) is an input matrix of dimension 1āĆānROIs, and k represents the set of nodes specified as control nodes. In our study we calculated the average controllability of each of the nodes within the 400 node parcellation. In several analyses we averaged these values at the level of large-scale networks, which we referred to as the mean average controllability of the respective large-scale network.
Redundancy calculations
Redundancy was calculated as the number of simple (non-circular) paths between a pair of nodes up to a specified length (here we used Lā=ā4)42, according to the equation:
Where P(i, j, k) was the number of paths non-circular paths between nodes i, and j, calculated with the all_simple_paths function in NetworkX112. To get nodal redundancy, we summed the total number of paths from each node to all other nodes. After calculation of nodal redundancy for all nodes in each subjectās structural networks, we calculated the average redundancy in the structural connectivity of each of the 17 large-scale networks per subject. We used the binarized structural connectivity matrices for these calculations.
Cognitive measures
We focused on the cognitive measure of processing speed within our study because processing speed is believed to be limited by communication along white-matter tracts52,53. Processing speed was assessed via the Pattern Comparison Processing Speed Test64. Subjects were shown pairs of objects and asked to judge whether two objects, presented simultaneously, were the same or different. They were given 85ās to judge as many objects as possible. We used participantās MoCA scores to determine if they were healthy (scoreā>=ā23/30). Additionally, we used measures of cognitive flexibility, assessed via the Dimensional Card Sort Test113, executive control, assessed via the Flanker Inhibitory Control and Attention Test114, and vocabulary comprehension, assessed via the Picture Vocabulary Test107, to exclude subjects with forms of dementia that the MoCA is insensitive to17.
Grey matter volume extraction
From the T1-weighted images, we extracted grey matter volume using the run_first_all command within Freesurfer115. This included extraction of hippocampal volume, and the volume of subcortical structures, in the Aseg atlas116, as well cortical grey matter volume and estimated total intracranial volume.
Statistics and reproducibility
We performed group comparisons of average controllability in middle-aged (40āā¤āageā<ā65, nā=ā305, M/Fā=ā120/185) and old-aged (65āā¤āageā<ā90, nā=ā175, M/Fā=ā79/96) adults. We performed rank-correlations between features of network controllability and redundancy with age and processing speed, as well as rank-correlations between redundancy, degree, and network controllability measures. We then performed parallel mediation analyses to investigate the effects of degree and redundancy on the relationships between age and average controllability. In the mediation analyses with redundancy, ranked-degree was included as a covariate to highlight influence of multi-step pathways on the relationships between age and average controllability. For all mediation analyses, participant education was included as a covariate. Inferential statistics were estimated with 10,000 bootstraps. The correlations and mediation analyses were performed using the entire sample of participants (nā=ā480, M/Fā=ā199/281). Following these experiments, we performed a breakpoint analysis using a piece-wise linear regression to determine the starting point of hippocampal atrophy in our healthy cross-sectional sample. Using participants older than the discovered breakpoint (age >66.92, nā=ā151, M/Fā=ā67/84), we used general linear models (GLMs) to investigate the extent to which linear combinations of GM and network features aided in the prediction of processing speed. ANCOVAs, Spearmanās correlations, Pearsonās correlations, and mediation analysis, were performed using the python package Pingouin117. Welchās ANOVAsĀ were used to compare average controllability in 15 identified hubs in middle-aged and old-aged adults. We used the Bonferroni method to correct for multiple comparisons which set the p-value necessary for significance to pā<ā0.05/15. For Spearman and Pearson correlations, we required pā<ā0.05/17 to correct for the number of functional networks analyzed. Mediation analyses were performed with the Ī±ā=ā0.05/17 for the confidence intervals, with significance determined by whether or not the confidence intervals for each coefficient crossed the value of zero. Piece-wise linear regression to determine breakpoints in rates of change for grey matter volume was performed using the pwlf python package118. Our GLMs were constructed using the python package Statsmodels119. Additional stats derived from these models (R2, log-likelihood, AIC120, BIC121) were also computed using the Statsmodels package.
Plotting
We used custom python scripts for plotting and data visualization based on the Matplotlib122, Pandas123, and Seaborn124, packages.
Data availability
All data used in this study is publicly available via the Human Connectome Project ā Aging dataset61.
Code availability
Supporting code for statistical analyses can be found here: https://github.com/WilliamStanford/ControllabilityInAging/.
References
Population Division, Department of Economic and Social Affairs, United Nations. World Population Ageing 2020 Highlights: Living Arrangements of Older Persons (United Nations, 2020).
Sleeman, K. E. et al. The escalating global burden of serious health-related suffering: projections to 2060 by world regions, age groups, and health conditions. Lancet Glob. Health 7, e883āe892 (2019).
NovotnĆ½, J.S. et al. Physiological pattern of cognitive aging. J. Alzheimers Dis. 88, 1147ā1155 (2022).
Mhaske, R. Happiness and aging. J. Psychosoc. Res. 12, 71 (2017).
Anton, S. D. et al. Successful aging: advancing the science of physical independence in older adults. Ageing Res. Rev. 24, 304ā327 (2015).
Le Carret, N. et al. The effect of education on cognitive performances and its implication for the constitution of the cognitive reserve. Dev. Neuropsychol. 23, 317ā337 (2003).
Wilson, R. S. et al. Education and cognitive reserve in old age. Neurology 92, e1041āe1050 (2019).
Rubinov, M. & Sporns, O. Complex network measures of brain connectivity: uses and interpretations. Neuroimage 52, 1059ā1069 (2010).
Fair, D. A. et al. Development of distinct control networks through segregation and integration. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. 104, 13507ā13512 (2007).
Chan, M. Y., Park, D. C., Savalia, N. K., Petersen, S. E. & Wig, G. S. Decreased segregation of brain systems across the healthy adult lifespan. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. 111, E4997āE5006 (2014).
Betzel, R. F. et al. Changes in structural and functional connectivity among resting-state networks across the human lifespan. Neuroimage 102, 345ā357 (2014).
Sadiq, M. U., Langella, S., Giovanello, K. S., Mucha, P. J. & Dayan, E. Accrual of functional redundancy along the lifespan and its effects on cognition. Neuroimage 229, 117737 (2021).
Sala-Llonch, R., BartrƩs-Faz, D. & JunquƩ, C. Reorganization of brain networks in aging: a review of functional connectivity studies. Front. Psychol. 6, 663 (2015).
Cohen, J. R. & DāEsposito, M. The segregation and integration of distinct brain networks and their relationship to cognition. J. Neurosci. 36, 12083ā12094 (2016).
Malagurski, B., Liem, F., Oschwald, J., MĆ©rillat, S. & JƤncke, L. Functional dedifferentiation of associative resting state networks in older adultsāa longitudinal study. Neuroimage 214, 116680 (2020).
Coelho, A. et al. Reorganization of brain structural networks in aging: A longitudinal study. J. Neurosci. Res. 99, 1354ā1376 (2021).
Stanford, W. C., Mucha, P. J. & Dayan, E. A robust core architecture of functional brain networks supports topological resilience and cognitive performance in middle-and old-aged adults. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. 119, e2203682119 (2022).
Chen, X. et al. The functional brain favours segregated modular connectivity at old age unless affected by neurodegeneration. Commun. Biol. 4, 1ā16 (2021).
Langella, S., Sadiq, M. U., Mucha, P. J., Giovanello, K. S. & Dayan, E. Lower functional hippocampal redundancy in mild cognitive impairment. Transl. Psychiatry 11, 1ā12 (2021).
Malek-Ahmadi, M. et al. Age-and education-adjusted normative data for the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) in older adults age 70ā99. Aging Neuropsychol. Cognition 22, 755ā761 (2015).
Chan, M. Y. et al. Long-term prognosis and educational determinants of brain network decline in older adult individuals. Nat. Aging 1, 1053ā1067 (2021).
Ewers, M. et al. Segregation of functional networks is associated with cognitive resilience in Alzheimerās disease. Brain 144, 2176ā2185 (2021).
Langella, S., Mucha, P. J., Giovanello, K. S., Dayan, E. & Initiative, As. D. N. The association between hippocampal volume and memory in pathological aging is mediated by functional redundancy. Neurobiol. Aging 108, 179ā188 (2021).
Liu, Y.-Y., Slotine, J.-J. & BarabĆ”si, A.-L. Controllability of complex networks. nature 473, 167ā173 (2011).
Kalman, R. E. Mathematical description of linear dynamical systems. J. Soc. Ind. Appl. Math. Ser. A Control 1, 152ā192 (1963).
Luenberger, D. G. Introduction to dynamic systems: theory, models, and applications 1 (Wiley, New York, 1979).
Slotine, J.-J. E. & Li, W. Applied nonlinear control 199 (Prentice hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ, 1991).
Gu, S. et al. Controllability of structural brain networks. Nat. Commun. 6, 1ā10 (2015).
Greicius, M. D., Krasnow, B., Reiss, A. L. & Menon, V. Functional connectivity in the resting brain: a network analysis of the default mode hypothesis. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. 100, 253ā258 (2003).
Raichle, M. E. & Snyder, A. Z. A default mode of brain function: a brief history of an evolving idea. Neuroimage 37, 1083ā1090 (2007).
Sulpizio, V., Galati, G., Fattori, P., Galletti, C. & Pitzalis, S. A common neural substrate for processing scenes and egomotion-compatible visual motion. Brain Struct. Funct. 225, 2091ā2110 (2020).
Bassett, D. S., Yang, M., Wymbs, N. F. & Grafton, S. T. Learning-induced autonomy of sensorimotor systems. Nat. Neurosci. 18, 744ā751 (2015).
Hamdan, A. & Nayfeh, A. Measures of modal controllability and observability for first-and second-order linear systems. J. Guidance Control Dyn. 12, 421ā428 (1989).
Pasqualetti, F., Zampieri, S. & Bullo, F. Controllability metrics, limitations and algorithms for complex networks. IEEE Trans. Control Netw. Syst. 1, 40ā52 (2014).
Medaglia, J. D. Clarifying cognitive control and the controllable connectome. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Cogn. Sci. 10, e1471 (2019).
Medaglia, J. D., Pasqualetti, F., Hamilton, R. H., Thompson-Schill, S. L. & Bassett, D. S. Brain and cognitive reserve: Translation via network control theory. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 75, 53ā64 (2017).
Tang, R. et al. Longitudinal association of executive function and structural network controllability in the aging brain. GeroScience, 45, 837ā849 (2022).
Tang, R. et al. Brain Controllability of Cognitive Control Networks is Associated with Executive Functions in Older Adults. Alzheimerās Dement. 18, e060583 (2022).
Bassignana, G., Lacidogna, G., Bartolomeo, P., Colliot, O. & De Vico Fallani, F. The impact of aging on human brain network target controllability. Brain Structure and Function, 227, 3001ā3015 (2022).
Tononi, G., Sporns, O. & Edelman, G. M. Measures of degeneracy and redundancy in biological networks. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. 96, 3257ā3262 (1999).
Navlakha, S., He, X., Faloutsos, C. & Bar-Joseph, Z. Topological properties of robust biological and computational networks. J. R. Soc. Interface 11, 20140283 (2014).
Di Lanzo, C., Marzetti, L., Zappasodi, F., De Vico Fallani, F. & Pizzella, V. Redundancy as a graph-based index of frequency specific MEG functional connectivity. Comput. Math. Methods Med. 2012, 207305 (2012).
Glassman, R. B. An hypothesis about redundancy and reliability in the brains of higher species: Analogies with genes, internal organs, and engineering systems. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 11, 275ā285 (1987).
Billinton, R. & Allan, R.N. Reliability evaluation of engineering systems (Springer, 1992).
Kafri, R., Springer, M. & Pilpel, Y. Genetic redundancy: new tricks for old genes. Cell 136, 389ā392 (2009).
Pitkow, X. & Angelaki, D. E. Inference in the brain: statistics flowing in redundant population codes. Neuron 94, 943ā953 (2017).
Barulli, D. & Stern, Y. Efficiency, capacity, compensation, maintenance, plasticity: emerging concepts in cognitive reserve. Trends Cogn. Sci. 17, 502ā509 (2013).
Cabeza, R. et al. Maintenance, reserve and compensation: the cognitive neuroscience of healthy ageing. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 19, 701ā710 (2018).
Stern, Y., Barnes, C. A., Grady, C., Jones, R. N. & Raz, N. Brain reserve, cognitive reserve, compensation, and maintenance: operationalization, validity, and mechanisms of cognitive resilience. Neurobiol. aging 83, 124ā129 (2019).
Zhang, Z., Yin, Y., Zhang, X. & Liu, L. Optimization of robustness of interdependent network controllability by redundant design. PloS one 13, e0192874 (2018).
Sun, P., Kooij, R. E. & Van Mieghem, P. Reachability-based robustness of controllability in sparse communication networks. IEEE Trans. Netw. Serv. Manag. 18, 2764ā2775 (2021).
Bullmore, E. & Sporns, O. The economy of brain network organization. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 336ā349 (2012).
Lynn, C. W. & Bassett, D. S. The physics of brain network structure, function and control. Nat. Rev. Phys. 1, 318ā332 (2019).
Carlozzi, N. E., Beaumont, J. L., Tulsky, D. S. & Gershon, R. C. The NIH toolbox pattern comparison processing speed test: normative data. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 30, 359ā368 (2015).
Madden, D. J. et al. Diffusion tensor imaging of adult age differences in cerebral white matter: relation to response time. Neuroimage 21, 1174ā1181 (2004).
Nilsson, J., Thomas, A. J., OāBrien, J. T. & Gallagher, P. White matter and cognitive decline in aging: A focus on processing speed and variability. J. Int. Neuropsychological Soc. 20, 262ā267 (2014).
OāShea, A., Cohen, R. A., Porges, E. C., Nissim, N. R. & Woods, A. J. Cognitive aging and the hippocampus in older adults. Front. Aging Neurosci. 8, 298 (2016).
Papp, K. V. et al. Processing speed in normal aging: effects of white matter hyperintensities and hippocampal volume loss. Aging Neuropsychol. Cognition 21, 197ā213 (2014).
Ng, K. K., Lo, J. C., Lim, J. K., Chee, M. W. & Zhou, J. Reduced functional segregation between the default mode network and the executive control network in healthy older adults: a longitudinal study. Neuroimage 133, 321ā330 (2016).
Imms, P. et al. Navigating the link between processing speed and network communication in the human brain. Brain Struct. Funct. 226, 1281ā1302 (2021).
Harms, M. P. et al. Extending the Human Connectome Project across ages: Imaging protocols for the Lifespan Development and Aging projects. Neuroimage 183, 972ā984 (2018).
Schaefer, A. et al. Local-global parcellation of the human cerebral cortex from intrinsic functional connectivity MRI. Cereb. Cortex 28, 3095ā3114 (2018).
Wang, L., Zhao, G., Kong, Z. & Zhao, Y. Controllability and optimization of complex networks based on bridges. Complexity 2020, 1ā10 (2020).
Carlozzi, N. E. et al. NIH toolbox cognitive battery (NIHTB-CB): the NIHTB pattern comparison processing speed test. J. Int. Neuropsychological Soc. 20, 630ā641 (2014).
Reed, B. R. et al. Measuring cognitive reserve based on the decomposition of episodic memory variance. Brain 133, 2196ā2209 (2010).
Elman, J. A. et al. Issues and recommendations for the residual approach to quantifying cognitive resilience and reserve. Alzheimers Res. Ther. 14, 1ā10 (2022).
Hagmann, P. et al. Mapping the structural core of human cerebral cortex. PLoS Biol. 6, e159 (2008).
Park, D. C. & Reuter-Lorenz, P. The adaptive brain: aging and neurocognitive scaffolding. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 60, 173ā196 (2009).
Resnick, S. M., Pham, D. L., Kraut, M. A., Zonderman, A. B. & Davatzikos, C. Longitudinal magnetic resonance imaging studies of older adults: a shrinking brain. J. Neurosci. 23, 3295ā3301 (2003).
Grady, C. L. Cognitive neuroscience of aging. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1124, 127ā144 (2008).
Tu, C. et al. Warnings and caveats in brain controllability. NeuroImage 176, 83ā91 (2018).
Medaglia, J. D. et al. Network controllability in the inferior frontal gyrus relates to controlled language variability and susceptibility to TMS. J. Neurosci. 38, 6399ā6410 (2018).
Beynel, L. et al. Structural controllability predicts functional patterns and brain stimulation benefits associated with working memory. J. Neurosci. 40, 6770ā6778 (2020).
Bennett, I. J. & Madden, D. J. Disconnected aging: cerebral white matter integrity and age-related differences in cognition. Neuroscience 276, 187ā205 (2014).
Liu, H. et al. Aging of cerebral white matter. Ageing Res. Rev. 34, 64ā76 (2017).
Cole, M. W., RepovÅ”, G. & Anticevic, A. The frontoparietal control system: a central role in mental health. Neuroscientist 20, 652ā664 (2014).
Marek, S. & Dosenbach, N.U. The frontoparietal network: function, electrophysiology, and importance of individual precision mapping. Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 20, 133ā140 (2018).
Cole, M. W., Pathak, S. & Schneider, W. Identifying the brainās most globally connected regions. Neuroimage 49, 3132ā3148 (2010).
Wong, C.H. et al. Causal influences of salience/cerebellar networks on dorsal attention network subserved age-related cognitive slowing. GeroScience, 45, 889ā899 (2023).
Seeley, W. W. et al. Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. J. Neurosci. 27, 2349ā2356 (2007).
Corbetta, M. & Shulman, G. L. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 201ā215 (2002).
Ruiz-Rizzo, A. L. et al. Decreased cingulo-opercular network functional connectivity mediates the impact of aging on visual processing speed. Neurobiol. aging 73, 50ā60 (2019).
Parkes, L. et al. Network controllability in transmodal cortex predicts positive psychosis spectrum symptoms. Biol. Psychiatry 90, 409ā418 (2021).
Parkes, L. et al. Asymmetric signaling across the hierarchy of cytoarchitecture within the human connectome. Sci. Adv. 8, eadd2185 (2022).
Li, Q., Han, Z. & Wu, X.-M. in Proceedings of the AAAI conference on artificial intelligence, Vol. 32 (2018).
Oono, K. & Suzuki, T. Graph neural networks exponentially lose expressive power for node classification. Preprint at https://arxiv.org/abs/1905.10947 (2019).
Kipf, T. N. & Welling, M. Semi-supervised classification with graph convolutional networks. Preprint at https://arxiv.org/abs/1609.02907 (2016).
Wu, Z. et al. A comprehensive survey on graph neural networks. IEEE Trans. Neural Netw. Learn. Syst. 32, 4ā24 (2020).
Estrada, E. & Hatano, N. Communicability in complex networks. Phys. Rev. E 77, 036111 (2008).
Cornblath, E. J. et al. Sex differences in network controllability as a predictor of executive function in youth. NeuroImage 188, 122ā134 (2019).
Perrin, J. S. et al. Sex differences in the growth of white matter during adolescence. Neuroimage 45, 1055ā1066 (2009).
Gur, R. C. et al. Sex differences in brain gray and white matter in healthy young adults: correlations with cognitive performance. J. Neurosci. 19, 4065ā4072 (1999).
Li, D. et al. Gender effects on the controllability of hemispheric white matter networks. Cereb. Cortex 33, 1643ā1658 (2023).
Hsu, C.-C. H. et al. Differential age trajectories of white matter changes between sexes correlate with cognitive performances. Brain Connectivity 11, 759ā771 (2021).
Levine, D. A. et al. Sex differences in cognitive decline among US adults. JAMA Netw. open 4, e210169āe210169 (2021).
Lee, B.H., Richard, J.E., de Leon, R.G., Yagi, S. & Galea, L.A. Sex differences in cognition across aging. In Sex Differences in Brain Function and Dysfunction, 235ā284 (2022).
Lindenberger, U., Von Oertzen, T., Ghisletta, P. & Hertzog, C. Cross-sectional age variance extraction: whatās change got to do with it? Psychol. Aging 26, 34 (2011).
Nasreddine, Z. S. et al. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J. Am. Geriatrics Soc. 53, 695ā699 (2005).
Carson, N., Leach, L. & Murphy, K. J. A reāexamination of Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) cutoff scores. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 33, 379ā388 (2018).
T OāBrien, J. & Thomas, A. Vascular dementia. Lancet 386, 1698ā1706 (2015).
Hodges, J. R. & Patterson, K. Semantic dementia: a unique clinicopathological syndrome. Lancet Neurol. 6, 1004ā1014 (2007).
Bang, J., Spina, S. & Miller, B. L. Frontotemporal dementia. Lancet 386, 1672ā1682 (2015).
Harciarek, M. & Cosentino, S. Language, executive function and social cognition in the diagnosis of frontotemporal dementia syndromes. Int. Rev. Psychiatry 25, 178ā196 (2013).
Pantsiou, K. et al. Inhibitory control, task/rule switching, and cognitive planning in vascular dementia: are there any differences from vascular aging? Front. Aging Neurosci. 10, 330 (2018).
Adlam, A.-L. R., Patterson, K., Bozeat, S. & Hodges, J. R. The Cambridge Semantic Memory Test Battery: Detection of semantic deficits in semantic dementia and Alzheimerās disease. Neurocase 16, 193ā207 (2010).
Hodges, J. R., Graham, N. & Patterson, K. Charting the progression in semantic dementia: Implications for the organisation of semantic memory. Memory 3, 463ā495 (1995).
Gershon, R. C. et al. IV. NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery (CB): measuring language (vocabulary comprehension and reading decoding). Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 78, 49ā69 (2013).
Yeh, F. C., Liu, L., Hitchens, T. K. & Wu, Y. L. Mapping immune cell infiltration using restricted diffusion MRI. Magn. Reson. Med. 77, 603ā612 (2017).
Yeh, F.-C., Wedeen, V. J. & Tseng, W.-Y. I. Generalized ${q} $-sampling imaging. IEEE Trans. Med. imaging 29, 1626ā1635 (2010).
Towns, J. et al. XSEDE: accelerating scientific discovery. Comput. Sci. Eng. 16, 62ā74 (2014).
Kailath, T. Linear systems, Vol. 156. (Prentice-Hall Englewood Cliffs, NJ, 1980).
Hagberg, A., Swart, P. & Chult, D. S. (Los Alamos National Lab.(LANL), Los Alamos, NM (United States), 2008).
Zelazo, P. D. The Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS): A method of assessing executive function in children. Nat. Protoc. 1, 297ā301 (2006).
Zelazo, P. D. et al. II. NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery (CB): Measuring executive function and attention. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 78, 16ā33 (2013).
Fischl, B. FreeSurfer. Neuroimage 62, 774ā781 (2012).
Fischl, B. et al. Whole brain segmentation: automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron 33, 341ā355 (2002).
Vallat, R. Pingouin: statistics in Python. J. Open Source Softw. 3, 1026 (2018).
Jekel, C. & Venter, G. (2019).
Seabold, S. & Perktold, J. Statsmodels: econometric and modeling with Python. In Proc. 9th Python in Science Conference 57ā61 (SCIPY, 2010).
Akaike, H. in Selected Papers of Hirotugu Akaike. Springer Series in Statistics (eds Parzen, E., Tanabe, K. & Kitagawa, G.) 199ā213 (Springer, 1998).
Schwarz, G. Estimating the dimension of a model. Ann. Stat. 6, 461ā464 (1978).
Hunter, J. D. Matplotlib: a 2D graphics environment. Comput. Sci. Eng. 9, 90ā95 (2007).
McKinney, W. in Proceedings of the 9th Python in Science Conference, Vol. 445, 51ā56 (2010).
Waskom, M. L. Seaborn: statistical data visualization. J. Open Source Softw. 6, 3021 (2021).
Acknowledgements
Research reported in this publication was supported by the National Institute On Aging of the National Institutes of Health under Award Number R01AG062590. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health. Additionally, we thank Dr. Kelly Giovanello for helpful suggestions.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
W.S.: Conceptualization, methodology, investigation, visualization, supervision, writing, review & editing. E.D.: Conceptualization, supervision, writing, review & editing. P.J.M.: Conceptualization, supervision, writing, review & editing.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Ethics approval
All ethical regulations relevant to human research participants were followed. Data collection procedures in the original HCP-Aging study were approved by local Institutional Review Boards.
Peer review
Peer review information
Communications Biology thanks Fabrizio de Vico Fallani and the other, anonymous, reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work. Primary Handling Editors: George Inglis, Aylin Bircan, Joao Manuel de Sousa Valente.
Additional information
Publisherās note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the articleās Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the articleās Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Stanford, W., Mucha, P.J. & Dayan, E. Age-related differences in network controllability are mitigated by redundancy in large-scale brain networks. Commun Biol 7, 701 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1038/s42003-024-06392-2
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s42003-024-06392-2
Comments
By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.