Abstract
During sleep our brain switches between two starkly different brain states - slow wave sleep (SWS) and rapid eye movement (REM) sleep. While this two-state sleep pattern is abundant across birds and mammals, its existence in other vertebrates is not universally accepted, its evolutionary emergence is unclear and it is undetermined whether it is a fundamental property of vertebrate brains or an adaptation specific to homeotherms. To address these questions, we conducted electrophysiological recordings in the Agamid lizard, Laudakia vulgaris during sleep. We found clear signatures of two-state sleep that resemble the mammalian and avian sleep patterns. These states switched periodically throughout the night with a cycle of ~90 seconds and were remarkably similar to the states previously reported in Pogona vitticeps. Interestingly, in contrast to the high temperature sensitivity of mammalian states, state switches were robust to large variations in temperature. We also found that breathing rate, micro-movements and eye movements were locked to the REM state as they are in mammals. Collectively, these findings suggest that two-state sleep is abundant across the agamid family, shares physiological similarity to mammalian sleep, and can be maintain in poikilothems, increasing the probability that it existed in the cold-blooded ancestor of amniotes.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
Sleep is a fundamental behavior across the animal kingdom and is essential to normal animal function and survival1,2. This conserved behavior rely on the patterned activation of neuronal circuits throughout the brain. Yet, we still do not fully understand the functional role played by these activation patterns. A valuable strategy for generalizing principles of sleep function and their link to neurophysiology is by conducting comparative investigations across species3,4. This comparative approach has the added benefit of placing sleep within an evolutionary context, providing further explanatory power5,6. While behavioral characterization of sleep exists across vertebrate classes, the neurophysiological correlates of sleep have mostly been studied in mammals.
These electrophysiological investigations have revealed that sleep is characterized by two primary brain states: slow-wave sleep (SWS) characterized by high-amplitude low-frequency fluctuation in the EEG δ band (0.5–4 Hz), and rapid eye movement (REM) sleep, defined by low-voltage, high-frequency fluctuations7. Cortical δ waves are underlined by periodic switches between two excitability states: up states during which large populations are synchronously active, and down states with only sparse spiking8. Concomitant with thalamo-cortical SWS, the hippocampus engages in a similar shift between long epochs of sparse firing separated by sharp waves (ShW) during which activity is drastically elevated9, and exhibits oscillatory ripples (SWR) at 110–200 Hz10. Conversely, during REM sleep, overall firing rates increase, and activity is typified by more desynchronized firing patterns within both the cortex and the hippocampus11,12. These brain dynamics are accompanied by an additional set of physiological properties such as rapid eye movement, loss of muscle tone, muscle twitches, and elevated respiration rates13,14.
Sleep states are controlled by nuclei in the brain stem7, basal forebrain15, and the hypothalamus16,17. These areas have clear homologs across vertebrates and are tightly linked to thermo-regulation18,19,20,21. Recent studies report that cortical sleep patterns are also coordinated by higher brain areas within the pallium. Specifically, the claustrum has recently been shown to be an important coordinator of SW activity in mice, mediating cortical transitions to SWS22,23. While the details of this pallial coordinating network are not entirely understood, it is likely to play a major role in sleep functions such as memory consolidation 24,25,26.
While the large majority of studies focused on mammals, the presence of sleep has also been well established in birds27,28 prompting researchers to hypothesize that such sleep patterns existed in stem amniotes, the common ancestors of mammals and birds. If this is the case, two-state sleep should also be found across reptiles. Indeed, several studies (mostly in the 1970s) conducted in non-avian reptiles, set to investigate this question29. Within this class, lepidosaurs are particularly interesting as they are the most distant from aves30. While these studies showed that behavioral sleep was abundant across lepidosaurs, evidence of electrophysiological sleep remained largely inconclusive (Fig. 1). All studies found a decrease in EEG frequency as animals entered a state of quiescence, however, EEG amplitude during this transition varied between studies31,32,33,34. Many reported only one EEG pattern during behavioral sleep, others reported two different EEG sleep patterns. Most of these reports named one of the patterns “quiet sleep”, defined by its similarity to quiet wakefulness, but lower in amplitude and frequency. The second reported pattern was usually very short, named “active sleep” and was defined by the similarity to EEG patterns during active wakefulness, e.g. high amplitudes and frequency. Note that these definitions are not identical to the ones used in current literature to describe behavioral sleep in young mammals35,36. Interestingly, the active sleep patterns were, in some cases, linked to one or more REM-like physiological patterns37,38,39,40,41,42. Two of the studies that showed two EEG patterns, reported that they were similar to mammalian SWS and REM in term of amplitude and frequency43,44. Taken together, insufficient and contradicting evidence in different species, and even in the same species33,42,45, led to ambiguity regarding the existence of two-state sleep in reptiles29,46.
Recent evidence from the lizard Pogona vitticeps tells a possibly different story on reptilian sleep. This study reported two periodically-alternating sleep states which bear high similarities to mammalian and avian states48. In contrast to previous studies, brain states in this study were found in the dorsal ventricular ridge (DVR). This may explain why these brain states were previously missed in most other studies that used EEG recordings from above the dorsal pallium. In addition, the authors used modern electrophysiological techniques and data analysis methods allowing them to track long-term LFP dynamics and identify slow oscillations, as well as to measure spiking patterns, thus revealing the underling organization of single cell population patterns. A follow up study revealed that the sources of SWR that occur during SWS in P. vitticeps originated from the reptilian homolog of the claustrum, suggesting sleep-control similarities between mammals and reptiles49.
These results were compared with recordings from another lizard species, the Argentine Tegu (Salvator merianae)50. While two states of electrophysiological sleep (defined by a transition in frequency profiles) were detected in the Argentinian Tegu, they lacked the high similarity with mammalian states and the alternating periodicity observed in P. vitticeps. The Tegu mostly exhibited a SWS-like brain state with transient activations of a REM-like state typified by 15 Hz oscillations appearing at the beginning and the end of the sleep period. Interestingly, both species exhibited ShW activity preferentially during the SWS-like state. Since the highly-structured sleep-state manifestation in P. vitticeps was never reported in any other species, it has hitherto remained unclear whether these mammalian-like sleep patterns are unique to P. vitticeps (possibly due to a specific adaptation) or whether they exist in other species. Furthermore, since only a limited number of physiological parameters associated with sleep transitions were reported in P. vitticeps, the functional homology between two-state sleep in P. vitticeps and mammals remains to be determined. Finally, if indeed brain state transitions exist in ectotherms, it is unclear how they can be robustly maintained given the high sensitivity of sleep states to temperature fluctuations reported in mammals51.
To answer these questions, we investigated sleep patterns in the rough-tail rock agama (Laudakia vulgaris, previously known as Laudakia stellio), a member of the Agamid lizard family. L. vulgaris and P. vitticeps belong to separate monophyletic groups that diverged more than 100 million years ago, making L. vulgaris a prime candidate for investigating two-state sleep across Agamids (Fig. 152). L. vulgaris are diurnal, rock-dweller heliotherms. Behaviorally, these lizards sun-bask to control body temperature and tend to sit-and-wait for prey53. We found that L. vulgaris showed a prolonged period of behavioral sleep during the night with a clear division into two states with different spectral and amplitude profiles similar to mammalian SWS and REM sleep states. These states shifted periodically and showed a high abundance of SWR in the SWS-like state. Interestingly, we found that eye movements, breathing rate and the probability of muscle twitches increased predominantly during the REM-like state. Finally, sleep states transitions were observed across a wide range of temperatures and showed frequency scaling consistent with the Arrhenius equation confirming that they can be robustly maintained in poikilotherms.
Results
Behavioral sleep in L. vulgaris
We first examined the behavioral sleep patterns of wild-caught L. vulgaris under lab conditions. After a few days of accommodation to their home terrarium, the lizards showed clear entrainment to diurnal cycles (12/12 light/dark). Before the beginning of the lights-off period (19:00–7:00), the animals reduced their movement, lay down (assuming a typical sleep posture), and closed their eyes (Fig. 2a). To quantitatively evaluate movement around sleep time, we placed the lizards in the recording arena 3 h before the lights-off period and measured their movement using accelerometers (see Methods). The lizards gradually decreased their movement in anticipation of the lights-off period. Their movement levels remained low for the duration of the night and increased again in the morning (Fig. 2b). This movement profile was consistent across animals (Fig. 2c, 89 nights from 7 animals) and consistent with previous results from P. vitticeps48.
Two-state neurophysiological sleep in L. vulgaris
After verifying behavioral sleep, we turned to characterizing the neurophysiological correlates of sleep. We implanted 32 channels, thin (4 µm thick) and flexible polyamide electrode arrays (Fig. 3a) in the anterior DVR (Fig. 3b, c) by lowering the highest electrode to a position 1 mm below the cortical surface. This choice was motivated by the robust manifestation of sleep states48 and the proximity to the source of ShW49 in P. vitticeps. Raw data traces recorded from the DVR revealed two distinct activity patterns (Fig. 3d). One pattern (Fig. 3d, blue shade) was characterized by high-amplitude ShW, and the second by lower amplitude fluctuations (Fig. 3d, orange shade). To quantify this division into states, we analyzed the spectral profiles during a 2-h interval of sleep. We first divided the recording into consecutive 10 s segments and calculated the spectral context of each segment. We next correlated the spectral context between all segment pairs and clustered them using a dendrogram algorithm. This analysis revealed a clear separation to two different spectral profiles (Fig. 3e): one state with relatively high power in lower frequencies (Fig. 3g, blue trace) within the δ band (0–4 Hz), and one with predominantly greater power in higher frequencies (Fig. 3g, orange trace) within the β band (10-40 Hz). The transition frequency between these states was at ~4 Hz (Fig. 3g).
To track the dynamics of sleep states across the night, we calculated the ratio between the δ and β bands for the entire recording (Fig. 3f). We observed oscillations in the δ/β ratio beginning shortly after the dark period and ending after several hours. To extract the periodicity of these oscillations, we calculated the autocorrelation function of the δ/β time series. The oscillation period was 89 s (Fig. 3h). This value slightly varied across nights and between animals (Fig. 3j, 92.8 ± 9.5, mean ± std, 27 nights in 9 animals). To quantify the robustness of the oscillation, we calculated the peak to valley (P2V) difference in the cross-correlation function. The calculated P2V values for all recordings were high (Fig. 3i, avg. P2V = 0.65 ± 0.25) and well above the confidence bound for significant autocorrelation value (defined as zero), suggesting clear oscillations for all recorded nights. To investigate the ShW dynamics during these two states, we next identified ShW using a matched filter (Fig. S1a, b) and monitored their dynamics throughout our recordings. ShW rate gradually increased following the onset of sleep and decreased again towards the onset of wakefulness (Fig. S1c). ShW rate was high during the entire night and showed a clear locking to the δ/β cycle (Fig. 3k). This difference in ShW rate between states was highly significant (p value < 0.001) when pooling across animals and nights (Fig. 3l, 14,999 sleep cycles from 50 nights in 9 animals, t-test, p value = 1.5e−22). ShW were accompanied by a transient increase in high-frequency oscillations (60–200 Hz) occurring preferentially during the trough of the ShW (Fig. S1d–g). While such an increase was not observed in all recorded animals, it was most prominent in good recordings with high total power (Fig. S1h). Taken together, these findings constitute clear evidence of two-state sleep in L. vulgaris with characteristics that are remarkably similar to the REM and SWS states found in the lizard P. vitticeps, that likely diverged from the L. vulgaris more than 100MYA 47,52.
We next sought to examine the spiking patterns underlying the recorded LFP signals. Recording single units for long periods of time is a challenge in reptiles, because of the relatively large gap between the brain and the cranium. Still, we were able to measure spiking activity in four animals for long periods post-implantation. Spiking activity was tightly locked to the δ/β dynamics (Fig. 4a, b). The spiking rate markedly increased during REM-like and decreased during SWS-like states. To quantify these dynamics, we first segmented the sleep period to REM-like and SWS-like epochs using phase analysis based on the Hilbert transform (Fig. S2) and then calculated firing rates for each state. Overall, the distribution of spike rates during the SWS-like state was skewed towards lower values relative to the REM-like state (Fig. 4g). This increase was steepest at the onset of the REM-like state, as evident from averaging rates across all sleep cycles (Fig. 4h). Thus, while the spiking rate is maximal during the REM-like state (low δ/β), its increase starts already during the SWS-like state together with the decline in δ/β, and it reaches a maximum shortly after the REM-like state onset (Fig. 4j and S2). Correspondingly, δ/β reached its average and peak phases at 0.45 ± 0.02π and 0.59 ± 0.06π radians, respectively, while spiking activity reached its average and peak phases at 1.29 ± 0.05π and 1.15 ± 0.03π radians, respectively (Fig. 4i, j, 14 nights in 4 animals).
The spiking population dynamics between states corresponded to differences between LFP profiles. During the SWS-like state, spiking activity was sparse but synchronized within short and intense neural activation time windows. These activation epochs were separated by longer periods of relative quiescence (Fig. 4c) and were well aligned to the times of SWR occurrences (Fig. 4d). In contrast, during the REM-like state, spiking activity was more tonic with spontaneous spikes constantly occurring (Fig. 4e). Correspondingly, firing rates were more evenly distributed over a wider range as indicated by the kurtosis of firing rates distributions (14 nights, 4 animals, Fig. 4k). These spiking patterns during the REM-like state were in line with LFP profiles characterized by higher frequencies (Fig. 4f, 3g). While activity was more tonic relative to SWS-like state, network spiking rates were not uniform over time and exhibited constant fluctuations aligned with fluctuations in LFP signals (Fig. 4f).
Eye movement, breathing and muscle twitches are synchronized with sleep states
Previous studies have shown that eye movements in P. vitticeps are locked to REM48,50. We examined if such locking occurs also in L. vulgaris and whether there are additional physiological features that are associated with specific brain states during sleep. For this analysis, we only included recordings with robustly detectable state transitions (P2V > 0.2). Having defined REM-like/SWS-like state onsets and offsets (Fig. S2), we next identified (using an optic flow algorithm, see Methods) eye movements from video recordings during sleep. This analysis revealed that eye movements are tightly locked to the δ/β oscillations and occur mostly during the REM-like state (Fig. 5a, Video S1). This locking was consistent across nights and animals (14 nights, 5 animals, Fig. 5b). Since respiration has been linked to cortical activity in both mammals59 and lizards60, we used video recordings to track the movements of the ribcage area, which is a proxy for respiration dynamics during sleep (Fig. S3, Video S2). The region of the ribcage was manually marked at recording onset and tracked using the Kanade-Lucas-Tomasi algorithm to extract breathing rate. This analysis indicated that during the REM-like state (low δ/β), the breathing rate and amplitude increased compared to the SWS-like state (Fig. 5c). These increases were consistent across nights and animals (18 nights, 5 animals, Fig. 5d, e, t-test, P values: 2.03e−4 and 8.5e−3 correspondingly). Next, we analyzed recordings in which we acquired accelerometer data and could identify micro-movements (Fig. S4). Our analysis revealed that micro-movements were locked to the δ/β oscillation cycles and increased predominantly during the REM-like state (Fig. 5f). This increase was consistent across animals and nights (75 nights, 7 animals, Fig. 5g).
Head movements may be affected by or conjugated with multiple movement sources. To assess the interaction between head micro-movements, eye movements and breathing movement, we performed correlation and co-occurrence analyses (Supplementary Methods and Fig. S5). We found that head micro-movements and eye movements were significantly correlated (Fig. S5a, b). However, the majority of detected eye movements did not co-occur with head movements and vice versa (Fig. S5c, d). Since this co-occurrence may have resulted from eye movements detected by the head accelerometers, we examined the correlation between eye movement and head movement amplitudes. This correlation was not significant (P value > 0.05) for 83% of the recordings. Breathing was also significantly cross-correlated with head movements, yet, the maximal cross-correlation value in all recordings was very low (<0.02), indicating that these two movement types are largely independent during sleep (Fig. S5e–g).
Sleep states across temperatures
In mammals, sleep states are temperature-sensitive and switch following a temperature perturbation of a few degrees51,61. Therefore, we examined how temperature affects neural states in a poikilotherm experiencing a wide range of temperatures. To examine this, we increased the ambient temperature to 34 °C and measured brain dynamics during sleep. At this temperature, clear oscillatory activity was evident (Fig. 6a), but the period of oscillations shortened to 48 s (Fig. 6c). In contrast, when we reduced the temperature to 21 °C, the oscillation period dramatically increased to 158 s (Fig. 6b, d). To systematically study the oscillation temperature scaling properties, we exposed the lizards to recording nights with ambient temperatures ranging from 17°–35 °C. Interestingly, the lizards were able to preserve oscillatory dynamics across the entire temperature range and maintain clear transitions between the REM-like and the SWS-like states as indicated by the distribution of P2V (Fig. 6e). Plotting the frequency of oscillations as a function of the temperature for different recordings revealed a clear trend according to which oscillation frequencies increase with temperature increases (Fig. 6f). Assuming an exponential dependence of the oscillation rate on temperature (Arrhenius equation), we fit the data with \({R}_{T}={R}_{T0}{Q}_{10}^{(T-T0)/10}\), where RT is the oscillation rate (cycles/min) at temperature T, T0 = 17 °C, and Q10 is the temperature coefficient62. This fit revealed a Q10 coefficient of 2.3 (Fig. 6f). We also observed that the transition frequency (as calculated in Fig. 3g) between the SWS-like and the REM-like state increased with temperature and fitted a Q10 coefficient of 2.0 (Fig. S6).
Previous studies have shown that neural tissues are more excitable in higher temperatures63. We therefore monitored LFPs (a proxy for synaptic currents and excitability) for different frequencies (0–30 Hz range corresponding to the δ and β frequency bands) across different nights. We observed that the total power in the frequency domain (corresponding to amplitude fluctuations in the time domain) increased with temperature (Fig. 6g). To quantify this, while considering the variability in recording quality (and LFP amplitudes) between animals, we first normalized the power by the mean power (across all temperatures recorded from a given lizard) and then calculated the correlation between the normalized power and temperature for all animals (Fig. 6h). This correlation was positive and consistent across the population (correlation coefficient = 0.49, t-test, P value 5e−8), despite expected changes in the tissue-electrode coupling over weeks of recording. In addition to the general increase in power, a clear shift towards higher frequencies, including the peak frequency, was observed in higher temperatures (Fig. 6i). Calculating the center of mass across the power spectra also revealed a significant increase with temperature (Fig. 6j, correlation coefficient = 0.28, t-test, P value 4e−3). Thus, sleep oscillations were preserved despite changes in tissue excitability. Finally, we examined if the relative duration of SWS-like and REM-like sleep changed for different temperatures (Fig. 6k). In general, the SWS-like state occupied a higher fraction of the sleep period and this fraction slightly decreased as the temperature increased (C = −0.28, t-test, P value = 4e-3).
We next examined how ShW dynamics are affected by temperature. The ShW rate fluctuated periodically and was locked to the δ/β cycles (Fig. 6l). The ShW oscillation period increased with temperature (Fig. 6l, n) and was fitted with a Q10 coefficient of 2.24. The ShW duration was also affected by temperature. ShW width markedly decreased with temperature (Fig. 6m) and Z-scoring the widths per animal showed a significant correlation with temperature across nights (Fig. 6o, Correlation coefficient = −0.62, t-test, P value = 2e−6). Furthermore, ShW amplitude increased with temperature, unlike the ShW widths (Fig. S7). This analysis suggests that as temperature increases, population activity during ShW becomes more synchronized (higher firing rates during shorter time windows). Although we observed significant oscillations across all measured temperatures, the robustness of the oscillation period across cycles (quantified by the robust coefficient of variation, CV) was mildly dependent on temperature. Overall, CV was better fitted with a second-order parabolic equation relative to a linear fit (P value of 0.0197 vs 0.8450, respectively based on an F-test on the model, fitnlm Matlab), indicating the possible existence of an optimal sleep temperature (Fig. 6p).
Discussion
In this work, we demonstrated the existence of two sleep states resembling REM and SWS in the lizard L. vulgaris. These states are sustained for several hours during the night and switch periodically with a cycle of ~90 s (Figs. 2, 3). State switches are marked by changes in LFP frequencies and corresponding changes in the organization of population spiking patterns in the DVR. The SWS-like state is characterized by sporadic firing periods interrupted by short epochs of elevated synchronized activity, whereas the REM-like state is typified by overall higher and more sustained firing rates (Figs. 3, 4). We also found that eye movements, muscular twitches and increased respiration were preferentially locked to the REM-like state, whereas ShWs transiently increased during the SWS-like state (Figs. 3, 5). These brain activity profiles and associated physiological parameters are highly reminiscent of the sleep states found in mammals and birds64,65,66,67. We henceforth refer to these states as REM sleep and SWS. Finally, state switches were robust to ambient temperature fluctuations: increases in temperature resulted in increases in switching frequency while conserving the periodic transitions between states (Fig. 6).
The neurophysiological characteristics of sleep states we measured in wild-caught L. vulgaris were remarkably similar to the ones reported in P. vitticeps. These two species belong to separate sub-families of Agamid lizards that diverged more than 100 million years ago (Fig. 147,52). Yet, both are characterized by very similar spectral sleep profiles with the transition between profiles occurring at ~4 Hz (Fig. 3g, S648). Furthermore, in both species, states oscillated periodically for several hours during behavioral sleep with a very similar oscillation cycle (85 s in P. vitticeps vs 93 s in L. vulgaris). Finally, in both species, SWS was composed of long periods of sporadic low firing interrupted by transient increases in DVR spiking activity that appear as SWR in the LFP traces. REM sleep in both species was characterized by increased and tonic spike rates corresponding to the high-frequency-low-amplitude LFP traces (Fig. 4).
The high degree feature conservation between Agamid lizards is even more striking in light of the high inconsistency in neurophysiological sleep properties reported in past studies across reptiles (Fig. 129). For example, the lizard Ctenosaura pectinata was studied by three different research groups, each reaching a different result33,42,45. Some of these inconsistencies may be the result of differences in experimental strategy and recording location. The majority of previous studies were conducted using EEG recordings from above the pallium in variable locations but not directly from the DVR (Fig. 1). The choice to record from the pallium was probably motivated by the homology between reptilian and mammalian dorsal cortices68,69. In contrast, in our study and those from P. vitticeps, sleep states were recorded from the anterior part of the DVR, and could not be detected robustly from dorsal cortex (and therefore also not from EEG recording from above the pallium).
This straightforward explanation is complicated by a recent study that used the same recording methodology in both Argentinian Tegu and P. vitticeps. The authors of this study replicated the sleep profile in P. vitticeps but did not find similar states in the Argentinian Tegu. However, only LFP data was acquired in this study, preventing the identification of changes in population spiking patterns. Indeed, while we found that LFP signals were good indicators of the underlying spiking patterns (Fig. 4), this may not always be the case as LFP patterns change depending on neuroarchitecture and the organization of return currents70. In addition, it is possible that Agamid lizards have particularly strong transients in population patterns, and that such transients are subtler in other lizards, making them harder to detect with LFP recordings. Good electrode-tissue coupling is critical for detecting small transitions in synchrony and such coupling is drastically reduced in lizards with large gaps between the brain and the skull due to increased brain movement71,72, as is the case in the Argentinian Tegu50. Correspondingly, our LFP sleep signatures were most prominent in recordings with clear spiking activity indicating good tissue-electrode coupling. Thus, further research is needed to determine whether technical issues can explain between-species differences, or whether there is large diversity in the sleep manifestation in lizards.
While our results strengthen the hypothesis that REM sleep and SWS existed in the common ancestor of mammals and reptiles, the homology of such states could also be the result of convergent evolution in mammals, aves and a few non-avian reptiles29,48,73. Our findings clarify that oscillatory two-state sleep shifts are widespread and strongly conserved across, at least, agamid lizards. However, further characterization of LFP and spiking patterns in DVR across other lizard families (Fig. 1) is required to determine if REM and SWS are more widespread than previously thought and therefore more likely to be homologous. Alternatively, we would have to explore the cognitive similarities between mammals, aves and Agamids that could explain such a strong convergent evolution. Such exploration could lead to a better understanding of sleep function across species.
We found that both eye movements and head movements were locked to REM. Most of these movements were independent, but some were temporally synchronous. One possibility is that these synchronous events resulted from eye movement being detected by the head accelerometers. This is unlikely however, given that during synchronous movements, the intensity of eye movement was not significantly correlated with the intensity of head movements. Thus, it is more likely that synchronous movements are part of physiological events that are synchronously activated during REM.
We found that, similarly to mammals, changes in respiration are coupled to sleep states. In humans, respiration rates increase and become irregular during REM sleep59,74,75, yet this increased breathing rate is correlated with lower breathing amplitude, resulting in lower respiration volume76. Interestingly, this reduction in respiration volume is accompanied by fast metabolic activations: an increase in cerebral O259,77 and glucose metabolic rates78, increased ATP usage79, increased blood flow78 and increased glutamatergic activation78,80. In L. vulgaris, on the other hand, there is an increase in both breathing rates and breathing amplitude during REM (Fig. 5d,e). This increase in breathing volume fits the increased firing rates we measured during REM (Fig. 4), but is contrary to the reduction in mammalian respiration volume. This difference between mammals and reptiles could be related to the REM-induced muscle atonia in mammals81, which was only reported in two Squamata studies29, and absent from most other reports41,42,43,44,45,50. Consistent with this, a recent study in rats showed that the recruitment of the expiratory abdominal muscles during REM increases the respiration volume and reduces breathing variability (despite basal muscle atonia)82. This can explain how the lack of muscle atonia in reptiles can result in higher respiration volumes. The functional importance of this difference in breathing during sleep remains unclear. Nonetheless, it seems that eye movements, breathing changes and twitches are a part of a comprehensive physiological trend observed during REM, further strengthening the similarity between patterns in mammals and Agamid lizards.
The similarities between brain states in mammals and Agamids are surprising given that reptiles experience large circadian and seasonal fluctuations in their brain temperature. In mammals, slight changes in the brain temperature result in dramatic changes in the organization of neuronal dynamics during sleep83. Cooling the cortex of sleeping mice by 5 °C impaired the down states of slow oscillations and shifted neurons to persistent activity patterns reminiscent of REM or wakefulness61. Heating the thalamus by only 2 °C, significantly increased the frequency of sleep spindles51 whereas increasing the hippocampal temperature by 3 °C significantly changed the peak frequency, rate, and width of ShW84. These findings highlight the temperature sensitivity of the mammalian brain.
In birds, which are also endothermic, temperature manipulations seem to keep the coding scheme intact. Long et al. found that cooling the HVC nucleus by 5–10 °C slows the mating song speed of male zebra finches while preserving its general structure85. Correspondingly, natural changes in brain temperature explain faster female-directed singing (relative to non-directed singing) and song-tempo fluctuations during the day86. Such a temperature scaling scheme, conserving specific neural activity properties, aligns with our results in L. vulgaris (Fig. 6). Like the song structure in zebra finch, the switch between two activity patterns is conserved even as the transition rate changes. However, the power of this comparison is limited as it involved different brain circuits with different functions in birds and reptiles. Resolving these issues awaits measurements of brain state changes as a function of temperature in birds. Such data will also help determine whether the spiking patterns in birds are conserved despite population organization changes or whether information in specific areas (e.g. HVC) is state-independent. Alternatively, it may be the case that birds inherited a set of temperature regulating mechanisms that were lost in mammals. To resolve this question, data from an amphibian out-group will be needed.
The temperature robustness of L. vulgaris sleep highlights the importance of the two-state activity organization. But how can this organization be maintained and regulated? This question was investigated in the stomato-gastric ganglion of the Jonah crab (Cancer borealis). This crab exhibits a pyloric rhythm whose frequency increases 4-fold with temperature (7–31°C)87. Despite this increase, the phase relations between circuit components remained constant88. Interestingly, modeling work indicates that keeping this temperature robustness is challenging since neural systems are composed of multiple temperature-bound components with different Q10 values. In fact, robustness can be achieved only for specific component parameters and requires tight regulation. Moreover, not all activity attributes can be maintained simultaneously, thus, only the “most important” attributes can be temperature-robust89. Our results indicate that also in the larger and more complex networks of the central nervous system, such robustness can be maintained (Fig. 6). This is manifested by preserving state transitions and the abundance of each state while changing oscillation frequency. This may indicate which sleep property in L. vulgaris is more important: the presence of both states during each cycle rather than the cycle duration.
What are the limits of temperature robustness? We identified clear state transitions across a temperature range of 17–35 °C. Significantly, the temperatures we examined are those encountered during the summer in the natural habitat of L. vulgaris. The question remains - what happens to sleep states during the winter when night temperatures drop to 5 °C? This temperature would correspond to a δ/β period of ~21 min (Fig. 6f). One option is that brain states persist as long as lizards can find sleeping locations with adequate temperatures90, and that sleeping patterns stop completely during the cold part of the year. Correspondingly, we observed a slight increase in oscillation cycle variability at the edges of our temperature range, which may indicate state instability at temperatures higher than 35 °C or lower than 17 °C (Fig. 6p). If this is the case, will all cognitive sleep-dependent functions be impaired in extreme temperatures? As L. vulgaris (similarly to most lizards) hibernates during winter, most cognitive abilities may not be required. Interestingly, in mammals, sleeping patterns are mostly absent during hibernation at low temperatures (<20 °C) and thus hibernation is periodically interrupted for sleep83,91. Whether this is the case in reptiles awaits neurophysiological measurements from hibernating reptiles, but our results indicate that the lack of sleep during hibernation is not an outcome of low temperature as suggested for squirrels91. During spring and autumn in L. vulgaris’s natural habitat, temperatures are higher but can easily reach 10 °C and may thus still impair neurophysiological sleep patterns. Resolving this question awaits development of adequate behavioral paradigms and corresponding neurophysiological measurements that will allow us to link sleep patterns with behavioral impairment, or lack thereof.
Exploring the link between behavior and neurophysiology in reptiles may also help us understand the ancestral function of sleep states and guide us towards the fundamental functions of state transitions. Our findings suggest that repeated periodic transitions between active and silent states followed by a high continuous firing state are key features of sleep. Indeed, studies in humans highlight the importance of such periodic transitions in stabilizing memories92,93. In contrast to rodent model systems, sleep cycles in Agamid lizards are highly organized and can thus serve as a good model for studying the function of repetitive state transitions. Interestingly, the silent states during reptilian SWS are longer and the active states are shorter (~500 ms), much like the ShW in the hippocampus10. Thus, the ancestral sleep state may have manifested as short events that quickly propagate throughout the neural tissue49,67,94,95. Such propagation may therefore be a fundamental requirement of sleeping circuits that is fulfilled by up and down states in mammals96,97. To promote a comparative view of sleep and its functions, further work exploring the behavioral commonalities between lizard species exhibiting two-state sleep and mammals will be required.
Methods
Animals
Experiments were conducted on 9 adult (age unknown) rough-tail rock lizards (Laudakia vulgaris) from both sexes weighing 60–70 g. All animals were captured in the wild under the approval of the Israel Nature and Parks Authority (Approval number: 2021/42698). Once captured, lizards were moved to an animal house in Tel Aviv University’s zoological gardens and kept in a 12–12 h light (07:00–19:00) and dark cycle and an external temperature of 24 °C. The animal’s terrariums were equipped with a heat lamp (50 W), resulting in a local hot spot of 33 °C, and a UV light source that was active only during the day. Animals were provided with water ad libitum and fed with leafy green and a dose of protein in the form of mealworm and cockroaches covered in calcium and multi-vitamin powder. All experiments were approved by Tel Aviv University ethical committee (Approval number: 04-21-034).
Surgery
Twenty-four hours before surgery, analgesics (Meloxicam: 0.2 mg/kg or Carprofen 2 mg/kg) and antibiotics (Baytril: 5 mg/kg) were administered. On the day of the surgery, the animal was initially anesthetized with inhalation of Isoflurane in an induction box and later intubated and connected to a ventilation system (AWS 100, Hallowell EMC) maintaining a constant flow of 4% Isoflurane. Once deep anesthesia was verified, the lizards were placed in a stereotactic apparatus (RWD 68409). Body temperature was maintained via a heating pad attached to the stereotactic table at 40 °C. Eyes were protected by covering with ointment (Doratears). The skin covering the skull was disinfected with Povidone Iodine 10% and coated with a Lidocaine ointment (2%) for local analgesia. The skin on the skull was removed with a scalpel and residual tissue was removed with a micro curette and dissolved with 30% hydrogen peroxide. A small craniotomy was drilled and the dura and arachnoid layers covering the forebrain were removed with fine forceps, and the pia was gently opened over the area of electrode insertion. A small incision in the brain was made with a 22 G needle lowered with a needle holder stereotaxic arm. A 32-channel flexible (4um thick) polyamide probe with TiN electrodes (custom fabricated by NMI, Germany) was inserted in the same location using a guide needle with manual adjustment. To target the DVR, probes were lowered to a depth of 1.5 mm below the cortical surface. The brain was then covered with Dora - gel (Cambridge neurotech). The remaining exposed skull was covered with UV glue (Transbond™ XT 3 M) and dental cement (Coral Fix) to secure the electrode and reference wire. Two holes were drilled contralaterally to the craniotomy and used for the placement of anchoring screws. The lizard was then removed from the stereotaxic table to a recovery box where it remained ventilated with Oxygen until it self-extubated. Following the surgery, the lizard received a daily dose (injected subcutaneously) of analgesics (Meloxicam: 0.2 mg/kg SC), antibiotics (Baytril: 5 mg/kg SC), and 1 ml of Saline for 5 days. Sleep recordings began after the lizard resumed normal behavior.
Experimental paradigm
All experiments were performed in a specialized noise and temperature isolated room. The temperature in the room was controlled by an air conditioning system and monitored using a temperature sensor. At approx. 3 h before the beginning of the dark cycle (16:00), the animals were moved from their home terrarium to the behavioral room terrarium and were connected to the neurophysiological acquisition system. At approx. 3 h after the end of the dark period (10:00), animals were returned to their home cage. Light and dark cycles in the experimental arena were maintained as in the home cage (7:00–19:00).
Electrophysiological recordings
Recordings were performed with an Open EPyhs acquisition system (acquisition board v2.2) and Intan amplifier head stages (RHD2132 - #C3324, or RHD2132 - #C3314 that included a 3-axis accelerometer) connected using an ultra-thin SPI cable (RHD2000-#C3216). The Open EPhys GUI54, was used for recording. Electrode recordings were referenced to a special on-probe reference electrode or an additionally implanted chlorinated silver wire (a-m systems, cat - 786000). During the recording, the weight of the Intan head stage was balanced using a pulley system.
Video recording
In a subset of sleep nights, videos were recorded using a FLIR camera (Firefly DL) with an accompanying Moritex lens (ML-M1218UR). Images were collected using a custom script based on FLIR’s SDK “Spinnaker C++” at a rate of 50 fps or 10 fps. For synchronizing the recording and electrophysiology, frame time stamps were generated by Arduino Nano and sent to the camera and the Open Ephys data acquisition board. During the entire recording, the arena was illuminated with an IR projector. Video with missing frames (resulting in synchronization lags of more than 0.5 s) was rejected.
Histology
After the experimental procedure was over, the lizard was deeply anesthetized with prolonged inhalation of Isoflurane in an induction chamber and decapitated with surgical sheers. The brain was then extracted with careful removal of bone and tissue with forceps and a micro-spatula and submerged in 4% PFA for 2 days. Afterward, the tissue was submerged in 15% sucrose in PBS solution for 24 h and then submerged for an additional 24 h in 30% sucrose in PBS solution. Following fixation, the tissue was embedded in optimal cutting temperature (OCT) embedding medium (Scigen - 4586) and was kept at −80 °C. The embedded brain tissue was sliced using a cryostat (Leica CM 1950) in 30 µm thick slices and Cresyl violet stained (Sigma - C5042). Stained brain slices were imaged using an Olympus epifluorescence microscope (model IX-81).
Data analysis
Analysis was performed using custom scripts in Matlab (R2020b) or Python (3.9.0) unless specified otherwise.
Spectral analysis of LFP
Spectral analysis was performed as in48. Briefly, voltage traces were low-pass filtered (100 Hz), downsampled (200 Hz), and binned (10 s). The average normalized power spectrum (spectrum in each bin divided by the average over the entire dataset) for each bin was calculated using the Welch method (1 s windows, 50% overlap), and the correlation matrix between all bin pairs (frequency range 0–30 Hz) was evaluated. The correlation matrix was separated into two groups using agglomerative clustering (ward linkage method over Euclidean distances). The intersection between the average (normalized) spectra of the two groups was defined as the transition frequency Ftrans. For calculating the δ/β ratio over the night, the extracted power spectra were calculated on 10 s bins (1 s steps). Based on the frequency transition values obtained before, we divided the mean non-normalized spectrum over frequencies lower than 4 Hz by the mean non-normalized spectrum over frequencies between 10 and 40 Hz in each bin.
Extracting oscillation cycles, their P2V, and total sleep duration
Electrophysiological sleep duration was calculated using the autocorrelation of the δ/β ratio. First, the autocorrelation was calculated for a 4-h window during the center of the night. This gave an initial estimate of the periodicity in the recording. From this autocorrelation function, the global oscillation period and the global anti-phase lag were extracted by locating the first positive and negative non-zero peaks, respectively. Correspondingly, the global peak to valley (P2V) was defined as the difference between the autocorrelation’s first peak and first valley after subtracting two times the confidence value of the cross-correlation function (to indicate the value above this confidence bound). To evaluate the oscillation period dynamics, a floating autocorrelation was calculated on the δ/β time series (1000-s bins, 100-s steps) and smoothed using a moving average (10-s duration). For each bin, the local period and anti-phase lag were calculated by taking the maxima and minima during a 20-s interval surrounding the global period and global anti-phase lag (calculated before). This allowed identifying a consecutive period of sleep for each night. Sleep bins were defined as ones with P2V greater than 0.25 to avoid discontinuities due to brief decreases in autocorrelation amplitudes, the sleep time series was median filtered (1-h window) and binarized (threshold = 0.5) to indicate sleep or wakefulness. Since the global estimation of the oscillation period was calculated on a random section of the recording, we repeated this calculation on a 2-h window with the strongest oscillations evaluated by identifying the window with the highest P2V in the oscillation period dynamics function calculated above.
Fitting temperature dependence
To extract the temperature scaling coefficient, we utilized the Q10 temperature coefficient formula to fit (least squares) F0 and Q10: \(={F}_{0}{{Q}_{10}}^{\left(\frac{\left(T-{T}_{0}\right)}{10}\right)}\), where F denotes the sleep cycle oscillation frequency [1/min], \({F}_{0}\) the frequency at a temperature of \({T}_{0}\) = 18 °C and T the median temperature [°C] extracted from the ambient temperature logger.
Timing of sleep cycles
The onset of the sleep oscillation cycles was calculated using the Hilbert transform over the sleep period (Fig. S2). First, the δ/β ratio was median filtered (20 s window) and Hilbert transformed. The instantaneous phase was extracted and its peaks were identified (findpeaks, Matlab) resulting in a series of timestamps of REM onsets. SWS onsets were defined as the time at which the Hilbert transforms between two consecutive REM onsets is minimum. Irregular cycles that were 1.5 times larger or smaller than the average cycle duration were removed from further sleep cycle analysis. We found this method to detect sleep cycles more robustly than the one previously used in P. vitticeps48.
Respiration analysis
Respiration analysis (Fig. S3) was performed on videos that were synced with the electrophysiological recordings and included at least one long (>2 h) consecutive segment with minimal animal movements. The ribcage area was first manually marked by a rectangle and its position was tracked using the Kanade-Lucas-Tomasi (KLT) algorithm (pointTracker, Matlab). Points were detected using the minimum eigenvalue algorithm56. These points were re-detected every 100 s or on the following conditions: (1) the number of tracked points was <40, (2) there was a >10% decrease in the number of tracked points. The frames during which points were re-detected were not analyzed. The respiration time series (Fig. S3b), was defined as the first principal component (PCA, Matlab) of movement (over the x and y axes of the image) after applying a median filter (2 s window). To robustly extract the breathing rate (Fig. S3c), the autocorrelation function was used. First, the global respiration rate throughout the video was calculated by extracting the first peak (and valley) of the autocorrelation function (findpeaks, Matlab). Next, the respiration time series was binned (24 s windows with 50% overlap) and the first peak (in the segment [GP-2.5, GP + 5] where GP is the global peak), of the autocorrelation in each bin, was extracted to evaluate the local periodicity in respiration. Bins in which the valley lag exceeded the peak lag (which sometimes was the case in noisy segments) were rejected. The respiration rate was then resampled (as a function of cycle phase [0, 2π]), averaged over consecutive oscillation cycles and normalized between [0, 1] to evaluate the respiration dynamics as a function phase.
Movement detection
Instances of body movement were extracted from the accelerometer attached to the Intan head stage (Fig. S4). Movements were detected along the X, Y, and Z-axis from the decimated (at 250 Hz after Chebyshev filter) accelerometer data. First, the difference between the upper and lower envelope was calculated using spline interpolation over local maxima (envelope, Matlab) and summed over all three axes. Micro-movements were detected as threshold crossings of the summed traces and their amplitude was recorded. The threshold was defined as 4 standard deviations above the noise level as calculated on noise segments. These segments were selected if their kurtosis (over 2 s bins) was ≤3. To quantify the degree of movements across sleep and wakefulness, the amplitude of supra-threshold movements was integrated over bins of 1 h. To identify the temporal distribution of micro-movements during SWS and REM, the number of micro-movements as a function of cycle phase [0, 2π] were counted for all oscillation cycles. For each night the average movement phase was calculated using CircStat 55.
ShW detection and ripple analysis
The detection of ShW was based on a matched filter built from averaging 10,000 manually curated ShW from the recorded signals of different animals. Data was first low-pass filtered (scipy.signal, pass band 400 Hz) and resampled at 400 Hz (scipy.signal.resample) and then convoluted with the manually curated ShW template. The time of ShW events were extracted using peak detection (scipy.signal.find_peaks) over the convolved output with a height threshold of 0.25 (Fig. S1B). This number was chosen using an optimization scheme of F1-score over a labeled dataset with true and false ShW57. Nights with weak oscillations were rejected (P2V < 0.2) and events with detected artefacts (amplitudes > 1 mV) were removed. ShW autocorrelation and phase analysis was performed on binned (1.2 s) ShW rate vectors. For ripple spectral analysis, ShW were downsampled to 1000 Hz and the continuous wavelet transform (scipy.signal.cwt) with morlet wavelet was used to calculate the spectrogram. The amplitude in the ripple band (Fig. S1h) was extracted by calculating the Hilbert envelop over the bandpass-filtered (60-200 Hz butterworth) signals. This analysis was performed on recordings at 26–28 °C. Recordings were sorted according to decreasing signal quality (estimated by the averaged wide-band power over a 2 h epoch) and nights with average low power (<40 dB) were rejected. A total of 22 nights from 8 animals analyzed. Analysis of ShW across temperatures (Fig. 6) was performed on 71 nights in 9 animals. All ShW analysis was written in python 3.7.
Eye movement analysis
Eye movements were calculated as in48 using the Lucas-Kanade method for optic flow estimation58 on tracked rectangular sub-frame segments surrounding the eye at a rate of 5 Hz. The position of the rectangle was manually annotated for the first frame in each video and tracked automatically throughout the rest of the night using the Kanade-Lucas-Tomasi (KLT) algorithm (pointTracker, Matlab) at an update rate of 0.6 Hz. The rectangle position used for optical flow calculations was updated if the tracked rectangle deviated from the current rectangle by more than 25% in either the vertical or the horizontal dimension. If the number of tracked points was smaller than 3 a new rectangle surrounding the eye was manually re-selected. The average optical flow was calculated by averaging the magnitude of all flow vectors in the rectangular frame (except for the frame edges, 5 pixels). Eye movements were considered significant only if their magnitude exceeded a floating threshold of 6 median absolute deviations (MAD) above the median calculated on a 3 min floating window. A shuffled control of these supra-threshold events was generated by replacing the event times within each cycle with ones drawn from a uniform distribution over the cycle duration.
Statistics and reproducibility
Experiments were conducted until further experiments yielded no new knowledge. To reach this point, the number of experiments varied depending on the features tested and the difficulty of the experiment. Sample sizes are specified for each figure and defined accordingly to animal, recorded night or sleep cycles. Experiments focus on measurements from animals implanted with electrodes. Animals that died during the implantation procedure or ones for which the implantation procedure failed, were not included in the analysis. Other exclusions were made on the basis of stringent criteria, which are specified in the relevant method section. In cases where videos were analyzed, only videos with reliable syncing with electrophysiological data were included.
Reporting summary
Further information on research design is available in the Nature Portfolio Reporting Summary linked to this article.
Data availability
The datasets generated during and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request. Source data for the figures can be found at https://github.com/EvolutionaryNeuralCodingLab/Temperature-robust-REM-and-SWS-in-Laudakia-vulgaris.
Code availability
The code used to analyze the data can be found at https://github.com/EvolutionaryNeuralCodingLab/Temperature-robust-REM-and-SWS-in-Laudakia-vulgaris.
References
Tononi, G. & Cirelli, C. Sleep function and synaptic homeostasis. Sleep. Med. Rev. 10, 49–62 (2006).
Campbell, S. S., Toblew, I. & Tobler, I. Animal sleep: a review of sleep duration across phylogeny. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 8, 269–300 (1984).
Nicolau, M. C., Akaârir, M., Gamundí, A., González, J. & Rial, R. V. Why we sleep: the evolutionary pathway to the mammalian sleep. Prog. Neurobiol. 62, 379–406 (2000).
Siegel, J. M. Clues to the functions of mammalian sleep. Nature 437, 1264–1271 (2005).
McNaramara, P., Nunn, C. L. & Barton, R. A. Evolution of Sleep: Phylogenetic and Functional Perspectives (Cambridge University Press, 2010).
Joiner, W. J. Unraveling the Evolutionary Determinants of Sleep. Curr. Biol. 26, R1073–R1087 (2016).
Saper, C. B., Fuller, P. M., Pedersen, N. P., Lu, J. & Scammell, T. E. Sleep state switching. Neuron 68, 1023–1042 (2010).
Susin, E. & Destexhe, A. Cellular correlates of wakefulness and slow-wave sleep: evidence for a key role of inhibition. Curr. Opin. Physiol. 15, 68–73 (2020).
Levenstein, D., Buzsáki, G. & Rinzel, J. NREM sleep in the rodent neocortex and hippocampus reflects excitable dynamics. Nat. Commun. 10, 1–12 (2019).
Buzsáki, G. Hippocampal sharp wave-ripple: a cognitive biomarker for episodic memory and planning. Hippocampus 25, 1073–1188 (2015).
Blumberg, M. S., Lesku, J. A., Libourel, P. A., Schmidt, M. H. & Rattenborg, N. C. What Is REM Sleep? Curr. Biol. 30, R38–R49 (2020).
Destexhe, A., Contreras, D. & Steriade, M. Spatiotemporal analysis of local field potentials and unit discharges in cat cerebral cortex during natural wake and sleep states. J. Neurosci. 19, 4595–4608 (1999).
Orem, J., Netick, A. & Dement, W. C. Breathing during sleep and wakefulness in the cat. Respir. Physiol. 30, 265–289 (1977).
Peever, J. & Fuller, P. M. The biology of REM sleep. Curr. Biol. 27, R1237–R1248 (2017).
Xu, M. et al. Basal forebrain circuit for sleep-wake control. Nat. Neurosci. 18, 1641–1647 (2015). 18:11.
Scammell, T. E., Arrigoni, E. & Lipton, J. O. Neural circuitry of wakefulness and sleep. Neuron 93, 747–765 (2017).
Herrera, C. G. et al. Hypothalamic feedforward inhibition of thalamocortical network controls arousal and consciousness. Nat. Neurosci. 19, 290–298 (2015).
Amini-Sereshki, L. & Zarrindast, M. R. Brain stem tonic inhibition of thermoregulation in the rat. Am. J. Physiol. 247, R154–R159 (1984).
Halvorson, I. & Thornhill, J. Posterior hypothalamic stimulation of anesthetized normothermic and hypothermic rats evokes shivering thermogenesis. Brain Res. 610, 208–215 (1993).
Nagashima, K., Nakai, S., Tanaka, M. & Kanosue, K. Neuronal circuitries involved in thermoregulation. Autonomic Neurosci. 85, 18–25 (2000).
Naumann, R. K. et al. The reptilian brain. Curr. Biol. 25, R317–R321 (2015).
Narikiyo, K. et al. The claustrum coordinates cortical slow-wave activity. Nat. Neurosci. 23, 741–753 (2020).
Smith, J. B., Lee, A. K. & Jackson, J. The claustrum. Curr. Biol. 30, R1401–R1406 (2020).
Vorster, A. P. & Born, J. Sleep and memory in mammals, birds and invertebrates. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 50, 103–119 (2015).
Rattenborg, N. C., Martinez-Gonzalez, D., Roth, T. C., & Pravosudo, V. V. Hippocampal memory consolidation during sleep: a comparison of mammals and birds. Biol. Rev. 86, 658–691 (2011).
Maingret, N., Girardeau, G., Todorova, R., Goutierre, M. & Zugaro, M. Hippocampo-cortical coupling mediates memory consolidation during sleep. Nat. Neurosci. 19, 959–964 (2016).
Rattenborg, N. C., Amlaner, C. J. & Lima, S. L. Behavioral, neurophysiological and evolutionary perspectives on unihemispheric sleep. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 24, 817–842 (2000).
Rattenborg, N. C., Lesku, J. A. & Libourel, P. A. Sleep in Nonmammalian vertebrates. in Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine. Vol 1 (eds. Kryger, M. H. et al.) 106–120 (Elsevier, 2022).
Libourel, P. A. & Herrel, A. Sleep in amphibians and reptiles: a review and a preliminary analysis of evolutionary patterns. Biol. Rev. Camb. Philos. Soc. 91, 833–866 (2016).
Striedter, G. F. Evolution of the hippocampus in reptiles and birds. J. Comp. Neurol. 524, 496–517 (2016).
Peyrethon, J. & Dusan-Peyrethon, D. Polygraphic study of the wakefulness-sleep cycle of a teleostean (Tinca tinca). C. R. Seances Soc. Biol. Fil. 161, 2533–2537 (1967).
Tauber, E. S., Roffwarg, H. P. & Weitzman, E. D. Eye movements and electroencephalogram activity during sleep in diurnal lizards. Nature 212, 1612–1613 (1966).
Flanigan, W. F. Sleep and wakefulness in iguanid lizards, Ctenosaura pectinata and Iguana iguana. Brain Behav. Evol. 8, 401–436 (1973).
Karmanova, I. G., Belekhova, M. G. & Churnosov, E. VBehavioral and electrographic expression of natural sleep and wakefulness in reptiles. Fiziologicheskii Zh . SSSR Im. I. M. Sechenova 57, 504–511 (1971).
Heraghty, J. L., Hilliard, T. N., Henderson, A. J. & Fleming, P. J. The physiology of sleep in infants. Arch. Dis. Child 93, 982–985 (2008).
Sokoloff, G. et al. Twitches emerge postnatally during quiet sleep in human infants and are synchronized with sleep spindles. Curr. Biol. 31, 3426–3432.e4 (2021).
Huntley, A. C. Electrophysiological and Behavioral Correlates of Sleep in The Desert Iguana, Dipsosaurus Dorsalis Hallowell. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. A Comp. Physiol. 86, 325–330 (1987).
Huntley, A., Donnelly, M. & Cohen, H. Sleep in an Iguanid Lizard, Dipsosaurus doralis. Sleep Res. 6, 143 (1977).
Ayala-Guerrero, F. & Mexicano, G. Sleep and wakefulness in the green iguanid lizard (Iguana iguana). Comp. Biochem. Physiol. A Mol. Integr. Physiol. 151, 305–312 (2008).
Stropes, S. L., Hunsaker, D. I. & Johnson, L. C. Electrographic and Behavioral Correlates of Sleep in the Fringe-Toed Lizard, Uma notata (San Diego University, 1971).
Ayala-Guerrero, F. & Reyna, L. V. Sleep and Wakefulness in the Lizard Ctenosaura similis. Bol. Estud Med Biol. 35, 25–33 (1987).
Ayala-Guerrero, F. & Huitron-Resindiz, S. Sleep patterns in the lizard Ctenosaura pectinata. Physiol. Behav. 49, 1305–1307 (1991).
Romo, R., Cepeda, C. & Velasco, M. Behavioral and electrophysiological patterns of wakefulness-sleep states in the lizard (Phrinosoma regali). Bol. Estud Med. Biol. 30, 13–18 (1978).
Stropes, S. L. Sleep Behavior In Reptiles (University of Arizona, 1975).
Tauber, E. S., Rojas-Ramírez, J. & Peón, R. H. Electrophysiological and behavioral correlates of wakefulness and sleep in the lizard, Ctenosaura pectinata. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 24, 424–433 (1968).
Rattenborg, N. C., Lesku, J. A., Martinez-Gonzalez, D. & Lima, S. L. The non-trivial functions of sleep. Sleep. Med Rev. 11, 405–409 (2007).
Zheng, Y. & Wiens, J. J. Combining phylogenomic and supermatrix approaches, and a time-calibrated phylogeny for squamate reptiles (lizards and snakes) based on 52 genes and 4162 species. Mol. Phylogenet Evol. 94, 537–547 (2016).
Shein-Idelson, M., Ondracek, J. M., Liaw, H.-P., Reiter, S. & Laurent, G. Slow waves, sharp waves, ripples, and REM in sleeping dragons. Science 352, 590–595 (2016).
Norimoto, H. et al. A claustrum in reptiles and its role in slow-wave sleep. Nature 578, 413–418 (2020).
Libourel, P. A. et al. Partial homologies between sleep states in lizards, mammals, and birds suggest a complex evolution of sleep states in amniotes. PLoS Biol. 16 (2018).
Csernai, M. et al. Dynamics of sleep oscillations is coupled to brain temperature on multiple scales. J. Physiol. 597, 4069–4086 (2019).
Honda, Y. et al. Phylogenetic relationships of the family Agamidae (Reptilia: Iguania) inferred from mitochondrial DNA sequences. Zool. Sci. 17, 527–537 (2000).
Karameta, E., Papadopoulos, V. V. & Pafilis, P. First record of ophiophagy in the roughtail rock agama (stellagama stellio): One of the most rare feeding behaviors among european lizards. Herpetol. Notes 8, 111–113 (2015).
Siegle, J. H. et al. Open Ephys: an open-source, plugin-based platform for multichannel electrophysiology. J. Neural Eng. 14, 045003 (2017).
Berens, P. CircStat: a MATLAB toolbox for circular statistics. J. Stat. Softw. 31, 1–21 (2009).
Shi, J. & Tomasi, C. Good features to track. In Proc IEEE Computer Society Conference on Computer Vision and Pattern Recognition 593–600 (IEEE, 1994)
Lipton, Z. C., Elkan, C. & Narayanaswamy, B. Optimal Thresholding of Classifiers to Maximize F1 Measure. In Machine Learning and Knowledge Discovery in Databases (eds Calders, T., Esposito, F., Hüllermeier, E. & Meo, R.) vol. 8725 (Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg, 2014) https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-662-44851-9_15.
Lucas, B. D. & Kanade, T. An iterative image registration technique with an application to stereo vision. IJCAI 121–130 (proceedings of the IJCAI 7th conference in Vancouver, 1981).
Douglas, N. J., White, D. P., Pickett, C. K., Weil, J. V. & Zwillich, C. W. Respiration during sleep in normal man. Thorax 37, 840–844 (1982).
de Vera, L. et al. Time-related interdependence between low-frequency cortical electrical activity and respiratory activity in lizard, Gallotia galloti. J. Exp. Zool. 303, 217–226 (2005).
Sheroziya, M. & Timofeev, I. Moderate cortical cooling eliminates thalamocortical silent states during slow oscillation. J. Neurosci. 35, 13006–13019 (2015).
Sterratt, D. C. Q10: the effect of temperature on ion channel kinetics. in Encyclopedia of Computational Neuroscience 2551–2552 (Springer, New York, NY, 2015)
Fillafer, C., Paeger, A. & Schneider, M. F. The Living State: How cellular excitability is controlled by the thermodynamic state of the membrane. Prog. Biophys. Mol. Biol. 162, 57–68 (2021).
Lesku, J. A., Vyssotski, A. L., Martinez-Gonzalez, D., Wilzeck, C. & Rattenborg, N. C. Local sleep homeostasis in the avian brain: convergence of sleep function in mammals and birds? Proc. R. Soc. B: Biol. Sci. 278, 2419–2428 (2011).
Martinez, D. & Rattenborg, N. Ontogeny and phylogeny of sleep. in The Neuroscience of Sleep 61–69 (Elsevier Inc, 2009).
Zepelin, H., Siegel, J. M. & Tobler, I. Mammalian sleep. in Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine (Elsevier Saunders, 1994).
van der Meij, J., Martinez-Gonzalez, D., Beckers, G. J. L. & Rattenborg, N. C. Intra-“cortical” activity during avian non-REM and REM sleep: variant and invariant traits between birds and mammals. Sleep 42 (2019).
Butler, A. B., Reiner, A. & Karten, H. J. Evolution of the amniote pallium and the origins of mammalian neocortex. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1225, 14–27 (2011).
Tosches, M. A. et al. Evolution of pallium, hippocampus, and cortical cell types revealed by single-cell transcriptomics in reptiles. Science. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aar4237 (2018).
Buzsáki, G., Anastassiou, C. A. & Koch, C. The origin of extracellular fields and currents-EEG, ECoG, LFP and spikes. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 407–420 (2012).
Chen, R., Canales, A. & Anikeeva, P. Neural recording and modulation technologies. Nat. Rev. Mater. 2017 2:2 2, 1–16 (2017).
Kozai, T. D. Y., Jaquins-Gerstl, A. S., Vazquez, A. L., Michael, A. C. & Cui, X. T. Brain tissue responses to neural implants impact signal sensitivity and intervention strategies. ACS Chem. Neurosci. 6, 48–67 (2015).
Hartse, K. M. The phylogeny of sleep. Handb. Clin. Neurol. 98, 97–109 (2011).
Sowho, M., Amatoury, J., Kirkness, J. P. & Patil, S. P. Sleep and respiratory physiology in adults. Clin. Chest Med. 35, 469–481 (2014).
Gould, G. A. et al. Breathing pattern and eye movement density during REM sleep in humans. Am. Rev. Respir. Dis. 138, 874–877 (1988).
Santiago, T. V., Guerra, E., Neubauer, J. A. & Edelman, N. H. Correlation between ventilation and brain blood flow during sleep. J. Clin. Investig. 73, 497–506 (1984).
Madsen, P. L. et al. Cerebral O2 metabolism and cerebral blood flow in humans during deep and rapid-eye-movement sleep. J. Appl Physiol. 70, 2597–2601 (1991).
Aalling, N. N., Nedergaard, M. & DiNuzzo, M. Cerebral metabolic changes during sleep. Curr. Neurol. Neurosci. Rep. 18, 1–17 (2018).
Cespuglio, R., Netchiporouk, L. & Shram, N. Glucose and lactate monitoring across the rat sleep-wake cycle. in Neuromethods, vol. 80, pp. 241–256 (Humana Press Inc., 2013).
Dash, M. B., Douglas, C. L., Vyazovskiy, V. V., Cirelli, C. & Tononi, G. Long-term homeostasis of extracellular glutamate in the rat cerebral cortex across sleep and waking states. J. Neurosci. 29, 620–629 (2009).
Blumberg, M. S., Lesku, J. A., Libourel, P. A., Schmidt, M. H. & Rattenborg, N. C. What Is REM sleep? Curr. Biol. 30, R38–R49 (2020).
Andrews, C. G. & Pagliardini, S. Expiratory activation of abdominal muscle is associated with improved respiratory stability and an increase in minute ventilation in REM epochs of adult rats. J. Appl Physiol. 119, 968–974 (2015).
DeBoer, T. Brain temperature dependent changes in the electroencephalogram power spectrum of humans and animals. J. Sleep. Res. 7, 254–262 (1998).
Petersen, P. C., Vöröslakos, M. & Buzsáki, G. Brain temperature affects quantitative features of hippocampal sharp wave ripples. J. Neurosci. 127, 1417–1425 (2022).
Long, M. A. & Fee, M. S. Using temperature to analyse temporal dynamics in the songbird motor pathway. Nature 456, 189–194 (2008).
Aronov, D. & Fee, M. S. Natural changes in brain temperature underlie variations in song tempo during a mating behavior. PLoS ONE 7, e47856 (2012).
Tang, L. S. et al. Precise temperature compensation of phase in a rhythmic motor pattern. PLoS Biol. 8, e1000469 (2010).
Rinberg, A., Taylor, A. L. & Marder, E. The effects of temperature on the stability of a neuronal oscillator. PLoS Comput. Biol. 9, e1002857 (2013).
O’Leary, T. & Marder, E. Temperature-robust neural function from activity-dependent ion channel regulation. Curr. Biol. 26, 2935–2941 (2016).
Regal, P. J. Voluntary hypothermia in reptiles. Science 155, 1551–1553 (1967).
Trachsel, L., Edgar, D. M. & Heller, H. C. Are ground squirrels sleep deprived during hibernation? Am. J. Physiol. 260, R1123–R1129 (1991).
Born, J., Rasch, B. & Gais, S. Sleep to remember. Neuroscientist 12, 410–424 (2006).
Tamaki, M. et al. Complementary contributions of non-REM and REM sleep to visual learning. Nat. Neurosci. 23, 1150–1156 (2020).
Tisdale, R. K., Lesku, J. A., Beckers, G. J. L. & Rattenborg, N. C. Bird-like propagating brain activity in anesthetized Nile crocodiles. Sleep 41, 1–11 (2018).
Leung, L. C. et al. Neural signatures of sleep in zebrafish. Nature 571, 198–204 (2019).
Sheroziya, M. & Timofeev, I. Global intracellular slow-wave dynamics of the thalamocortical system. J. Neurosci. 34, 8875–8893 (2014).
Mitra, A. et al. Human cortical-hippocampal dialogue in wake and slow-wave sleep. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 113, E6868–E6876 (2016).
Acknowledgements
This project has received funding from the European Research Council (ERC) under the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation program (Grant agreement No. 949838) and from the Israel Science Foundation (ISF, grant No. 1133/20). The authors are most grateful S. Jamison for obtaining the lizards; the animal caretaker crew for lizard care; the Shein-Idelson laboratory for their suggestions during the course of this work; G. Laurent for comments on the manuscript.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
M.S.I. initiates and designed the project. D.I.U., N.A. and A.S. collected data. R.E., M.S.I. and N.A. analyzed the data, designed and preformed the statistical analysis. N.A., D.I.U and M.S.I wrote the paper. N.A., R.E. and M.S.I. revised the manuscript. M.S.I acquired the funding. M.S.I supervised the work.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Peer review
Peer review information
Communications Biology thanks Niels Rattenborg and the other, anonymous, reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work. Primary handling editor: Luke R. Grinham.
Additional information
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Albeck, N., Udi, D.I., Eyal, R. et al. Temperature-robust rapid eye movement and slow wave sleep in the lizard Laudakia vulgaris. Commun Biol 5, 1310 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1038/s42003-022-04261-4
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s42003-022-04261-4
This article is cited by
-
A flexible implant for acute intrapancreatic electrophysiology
Biomedical Microdevices (2023)