(Im)mobility and climate change

The diverse terms describing immobility or immobile people includes everything from involuntary immobility, stayers, non-migrants, staying put, and left behind (Carling, 2002; Toyota et al., 2007; Gray, 2011; Hjälm, 2014; Mata-Codesal, 2018). Immobility in the context of climate change is not as widespread of an idea as mobility, but it is an emerging area of research interest (Black et al., 2013; Baldwin, 2016; Suliman et al., 2019). The notion of Trapped Populations, first introduced by the UK Government’s 2011 Foresight report on Migration and Global Environmental Change (MGEC), referred to vulnerable populations lacking the resources (mainly financial) to escape environmental stress although wanting to do so (Foresight, 2011; Black et al., 2011). The concept was thereafter extended by various migration scholars to include those ‘trapped’ by legal protocols, border situations and social barriers including gender and place attachment (Black and Collyer, 2014; Ayeb-Karlsson et al., 2018). The importance of non-financial immobilising elements was also raised within a UNFCCC climate policy context through the conceptual creation of ‘Non-Economic Losses and Damages’ (UNFCCC, 2013, 2015; Barnett et al., 2016; Boyd et al., 2017; Tschakert et al., 2019).

The climate-induced immobility literature, however, somewhat stagnated in economic immobility framings despite these efforts. Previous narratives on ’trapped’ populations also mostly cover rural people facing environmental hazards. This although the Foresight report almost a decade ago stressed how cities in low-income countries should be considered high-risk areas for involuntary immobility. Surprisingly little empirical evidence examines climatic immobility, and even less so, urban immobile people (Ayeb-Karlsson et al., 2018; Schewel, 2019). Underlying these narratives is a normative framing of decision-making as rational and linear whereby behavioural intention (in this case the desire to migrate) is assumed to lead to the desired behaviour. The idea of a mobility bias within migration research focussing on the drivers and flows of migration while neglecting immobility outcomes, serves as another explanation for the lacking research perspective (Adey, 2006; Beratan, 2007; Schewel, 2019).

This article provides a valuable contribution to this gap in the literature as it focusses on climate-induced immobility in an urban informal settlementFootnote 1, Bhola Slum, in Dhaka, Bangladesh, instead of the more common rural ‘trapped’ perspective. The name of the settlement arose as it housed the arriving migrants from Bhola Island after the devastating 1970 Bhola cyclone, and more recently due to riverbank erosion (McNamara et al., 2016; Ayeb-Karlsson et al., 2016; Ayeb-Karlsson, 2018). The study showcases why (im)mobility decision-making is highly complex, or less rational and linear, through a Q-based Discourse Analysis. If we are to better understand the apparent inability of people to move away from places that involve risky situations, we need to analyse the deeply contextual psychosocial aspects that affect a person’s state of mind, wellbeing, and thereby their (im)mobility decision-making (see Fig. 1). These include feelings of belonging, identity-constructions, attitudes to risk, and emotional or mental wellbeing. In other words, this study will give us a better understanding of why individuals with similar socio-cultural, economic and legal status can exhibit different (im)mobility and wellbeing outcomes.

Fig. 1: Discursive decision-making model.
figure 1

The figure illustrates a conceptual idea of how the decision-making process links to discursive and social-norms through the interaction of power (through punishment), knowledge (through discipline), feelings, emotions and wellbeing (Ayeb-Karlsson, 2018, p. 24).

Urban (im)mobility and mental wellbeing

Even though studies of urban immobility are limited, there is a literature body elaborating around urban (and slum) wellbeing or mental health. Similar to the climate immobility scope, there are more empirical research investigating mental wellbeing in rural than urban areas, and slum settings are in particular neglected. This is a critical knowledge gap as about a billion people around the world live in slums, a steadily rising number (Cook and Kirke, 2003; Sclar et al., 2005; Lilford et al., 2017). People trying to escape environmental changes often end up in slum areas upon arrival in the cities (Hunter et al., 2015; Etzold, 2016; Adri and Simon, 2018). The environment and life in these informal settlements (globally as well as in Bangladesh) often places people in higher risk of developing health issues (Ezeh et al., 2017; Schwerdtle et al., 2018). People living here are also more vulnerable to climatic changes, such as heat strikes and flooding than people living in housing providing shade and protection from direct sunlight, high temperatures and standing water (De Sherbinin et al., 2007; Woodward et al., 2014; Khan et al., 2014).

More research efforts on the connections between health (particularly mental health) and climate change are therefore urgently required to better protect the world’s urban and most vulnerable populations (Nahar et al., 2014; Blanchet et al., 2017; Watts et al., 2017, 2018, 2019). These vulnerable populations include those who live in over-crowded slum households that lack the infrastructure to protect them from environmental stress, while also lacking access to clean water, good sanitation, and public health services (Unger and Riley, 2007; Butala et al., 2010; Wekesa et al., 2011). As a result, these urban settlers are more at risk of developing mental ill-health or disorders (such as depression, anxiety, bipolar disorder, violent or abusive behaviour and schizophrenia) or even dying from suicides and unnatural deaths (Cattaneo et al., 2009; Gruebner et al., 2011, 2012; Mberu et al., 2015).

In the context of slums in Bangladesh, there is a strong focus on female experiences linking garment factories, violence, depression and PTSD (Akhter et al., 2017; Parvin et al., 2018; De and Murshid, 2018; Fitch et al., 2017, 2018). People living in slums here, and particularly women, adolescents and children, report struggling more with mental health issues and lower quality of life than people in other urban areas (Islam et al., 2003; Mullick and Goodman, 2005; Izutsu et al., 2006; Khan and Flora, 2017). A few investigations even link mental ill-health and life dissatisfaction in the slums of Dhaka and Khulna to climate-induced mobility (Ruback et al., 2002, 2004; Rahaman et al., 2018).

Method: Q-methodology and Discourse Analysis

This Q-based Discourse Analysis will examine urban (im)mobility decisions and wellbeing through people’s subjective attitudes and perceptions (see Table 1). The interdisciplinary and innovative empirical mixed-method was carried out over 3 years and involved Q and Discourse Analysis (DA). The 62 participants were not randomly selected, but efforts were made to ensure that they reflected the overall representation of the socio-economic and religious groups, as well as the distinction of age, gender, and livelihood backgrounds in Bhola Slum. The study applied respondent driven sampling (or snowball sampling) to select the participants. This non-probability sampling technique encourages existing informants to recruit additional participants through their social networks (Goodman, 1961; Goel and Salganik, 2010; Heckathorn, 2014). As with any research method or sampling technique there are strengths and weaknesses with respondent driven sampling. One potential weakness is the risk for biases. For example, social individuals are more likely to be recruited as they will have a wider social network. To improve the final study sample, it has been suggested to begin the sampling with an initial informant group from diverse backgrounds (Brace-Govan, 2004; Kurant et al., 2011). Our initial sample group therefore included informants from different religious, political, social, livelihood, and ethnical backgrounds. Since the hierarchical power structures and social groups in the settlement traced back to people’s migration history (e.g. time period since migration from origin village), efforts were made to ensure that the initial sample included a good balance here. Besides this, a sampling route ascertained a geographical spread of the final sample (Heckathorn, 2002; Browne, 2005).

Table 1 Q-statement overview.

Q-methodology (Stephenson, 1935, 1986; Brown, 1980, 1996; Stenner, 2008; Watts and Stenner, 2012) captures people’s subjective attitudes through a sorting exercise of Q-statements (see Fig. 2). The selected Q-statements used in this study were based on qualitative fieldwork sessions conducted between 2014 and 2015. 100 statements or quotes describing prominent (im)mobility narratives were pulled out from the previously conducted qualitative individual and group session transcripts. The statements described values and behaviours around migration and non-migration behaviour. These statements were then grouped into themes, storylines and narratives that appeared to be re-occurring. The statement sampling process continued by making sure that each Q-sample presented a good coverage and balance of the concourse. Out of the 100 statements, 40 were selected for the final Q-set.

Fig. 2: Q-grid used in study.
figure 2

As recommended by Watts and Stenner (2012), an 11-point (−5 to +5) distribution Q-grid was used for the Q-sorting activities in this study (Ayeb-Karlsson, 2018, p. 45).

A good Q-sample must be broadly representative of the overall opinions in the concourse, while presenting a balanced set of statements. This does not imply that half of the statements ought to be negative (con) and the other half of them positive (pro). Balance has a wider meaning, which is to ensure that the statements are not biased towards a specific opinion or viewpoint. It is important that a few statements from each thematic group are selected. This is because it will help reveal the patterns of several statements being sorted in a similar, or different, way. This consequently helps increase the analytical nuances and supports the summary of each discourse group’s collective storyline. A good Q-sample should provoke and invite a range of different reactions, while holding onto minimal research assumptions around what reactions they will create and why (Brown, 1980; Watts and Stenner, 2012).

Each Q-statement was read out loud and was edited by the research team to ensure that no statement presented any confusion. A good Q-statement is clear and simple. Technical or complicated language was therefore avoided. It is also important that each statement provides the participant with one meaning. A Q-statement should therefore not be double-barrelled. If a statement presents a Q-participant with two or more propositions, meanings, or qualifications, it will be impossible for the researcher to know which one(s) the participant is agreeing or disagreeing with. For example, let us take the hypothetic statement ‘A person needs commitment and compromise to be able to migrate’. If a participant disagrees with the statement, one cannot know if (s)he agrees with the suggestion that migration requires commitment, but disagrees with the suggestion that it requires compromise. Other problematic phrasing involves words such as regularly or because, or negatively structured items.

The recorded Q-sorts were factor analysed in PQ MethodFootnote 2, a commonly used DOS-based software in Q-circles, to identify different discourse (or factor) groups (see Table 2). Centroid factor analysis was used to detect factor patterns or inter-correlation between the Q-sorts (Watts and Stenner, 2012, pp. 96–100). Varimax rotation then supported in ensuring that each Q-sort (e.g. each participant sorting of Q-statements) only loaded on, or reflected the viewpoint of, one factor group. The significant factor loading was calculated through the equation (2.58 × (1/√ no. of Q-sorts in Q-set, e.g. (2.58 × (1/√62) = 0.33). Q-sorts loading on or reflecting more than one factor group (cofounded) as well as Q-sorts that were non-significant (below 0.33) were not selected for further analysis. Eigenvalue above 1.00 served as selection criteria for factor extraction. The selected un-rotated factors explain 41% of the study variance and 46 of the 62 Q-sorts loaded significantly on one or another factor (Watts and Stenner, 2012, pp. 127–128, pp. 197–199).

Table 2 Discourse group overview.

The Q-sorting exercise was accompanied by a post-sorting interview around the statement extremes, and a survey questionnaire to gain background information of the 62 Q-participants. The questionnaires, kept to 10 questions per surveyFootnote 3, were designed to capture a quantitative understanding of people’s (im)mobility. The survey was structured in two parts where the first part focused on the informant’s background, and the second part on people’s (im)mobility decisions. This was because a person’s successful, and unsuccessful, migration history can give us valuable insights in their current (im)mobility status, or mobility desires and aspirations. After the two survey questionnaire parts, the researcher continued to the Q-sorting exercise.

A common problem in many Q-studies is that the post-sorting interview does not result in enough details. The analysis then often fails to explain why the participants sorted, or felt the way they did around the Q-statement(s). In an attempt to improve this, and ensure more detailed insights in people’s discursive reasoning, the Q-sorting activity in this study was combined with the survey questionnaire and a DA. Discourse studies or Discourse Analysis is a general term for a number of approaches used to analyse vocal, written, sign language or any semiotic (meaning-making) event. The main difference between text-linguistic analysis and DA is that it aims to identify and comprehend the socio-psychological characteristics of a person rather than the text structure. During the 1960s and 1970s, a diverse set of cross-disciplinary methods of DA appeared within the social sciences. These related to a wide range of disciplines such as sociolinguistics, psycholinguistics, semiotics and pragmatics. Many of the approaches favoured a more dynamic analysis of talk-in-interaction which set the foundation for discourse analytical techniques such as Conversation Analysis (CA). This was later expanded by Michel Foucault and others who pushed the concept beyond linguistics and towards structural patterns operating through the relationship between knowledge and power (Foucault, 1972; Garfinkel, 1974; Fairclough, 2013). Meanwhile, critical discourse analysis (CDA) is an interdisciplinary way of understanding language as a form of social practice. Discourse scholars working from a CDA approach generally claim that linguistic practice and social practice (non-linguistic) account for one another. Focus therefore ought to be on investigating how societal power relations are created and confirmed through the use of language (Fairclough, 2003, 2013; Wodak, 2011).

When it comes to Q-methodology, a mathematical factor analysis of subjectivity in a specific concourse (or discourse), it is important to understand how it compares to discourse analysed through (C)DA. Concourse theory within Q was a manifest by Stephenson (1978, 1986) to move away from mental concepts such as mind and consciousness. The definition of concourse as “[a] universe of statements for [and about] any situation or context” (Stephenson, 1986, p. 44) shares many similarities with the discourse concept. According to Stephenson there is a concourse for every concept, wish and object when viewed subjectively. All the statements of a concourse can be understood as common [or cultural] knowledge. A concourse is also likely to be shaped and defined by a selection of statements spoken by the participants active in this universe. The nature of the concourse to be sampled will therefore not become clear until it has been framed by particular research questions within a specific research study.

Q uses a statistical model to detect sorting patterns of Q-statements which in turn identifies the subjective attitudes, discourse or factor groups. However, the critical depth of its analytical approach has been criticised. Q has been accused of generalising, lacking transparency, and for suggesting to present subjective data in a more objective way than other qualitative DA approaches (Brown, 1996; Previte et al., 2007; Kanim, 2000). A person’s subjectivity within Q is, as Brown (1980, p. 46) describes it, fundamentally a person’s point of view. It is explained as behaviour of the type that we encounter during the normal course of the day. What a person feels, conceives and perceives is a reflection of this viewpoint (Brown, 1980; Watts and Stenner, 2012). The advantage of using Q in this analysis is that it supports the identification of such subjectivities in the study site. The way that Q systemises and quantifies the grouping of people’s experiences or viewpoints will be useful. However, some of the nuances and complex links to contexts beyond the Q-statements, that DA of language captures, are often lost in Q-studies. It is important to remind oneself that a Q-analysis is topic, group and time specific. The captured Q-viewpoints therefore only make sense in relation to these elements.

The analysis will draw conclusions around the discourse groups’ perceptions around (im)mobility and wellbeing. ‘Discourse group’ refers to the Q-factor groups identified through the Q-analysis which groups people’s subjective responses in relation to the Q-set in such a way that it reflect the broader discourses in the study area (as described in Watts and Stenner, 2012). The analysis will tell us more about whether people want to move, and why they feel like they cannot leave the settlement, or how people understand their immobility, why they perceive themselves as immobile, and what this means for their wellbeing.

Q (originally coming out of psychology) in combination with DA has proved to be a successful way of analysing people’s perceptions and viewpoints around climate change and migration (e.g. Dryzek, 1994; Barry and Proops, 1999; Niemeyer et al., 2005; Ockwell, 2008; Wolf et al., 2009; Morinière and Hamza, 2012; Hugé et al., 2016). The power of discourses to produce ‘knowledge regimes’ is the main focus of a Foucauldian Discourse Analysis (Foucault, 1977, 1981; Hajer, 1995; Adger et al., 2001). The power and knowledge concepts can give us important insights into climate–human relations. This is because it is power and knowledge that lock people into social discourses who simply respond to their feelings and emotions (Morales and Harris, 2014; Eriksen et al., 2015; Owusu-Dakuu et al., 2019). Knowledge can, for example, maintain people in a discourse by disciplining their actions, and power by socially punishing those who step outside the discursive norm (Foucault, 1977, 1982; Butler, 2011; Ayeb-Karlsson et al., 2019). In this article, the concepts will serve as valuable analytical tools to understand subjective climate-induced (im)mobility, or why people sometimes do not manage to escape environmentally and socially risky situations (see Fig. 1).

Applying Q to understand urban (im)mobility wellbeing

Five discourse groups were identified through the factor analysis. Each factor (or discourse) group represents a different perspective on (im)mobility and wellbeing in the settlement (see Table 2). After the Q-sorting exercise, the participants were asked to explain why, or how they felt and thought when they ranked the Q-statement extremes (+/−5, +/−4, 0). The following analysis of the discourse groups include participant information from the survey, as well as the Q and a DA of the responses from the post-sorting interview. The heading of each discourse group represents a summary of the analysis and indicates the Q-statement(s) ranked as most important for the overall group e.g. distinguishing Q-statements and ranking extremes. The informant number, statement number and sort value are referenced in brackets. Here the article follows a Q-referencing system that indicates (informant: statement sort value), or to give an example, (46:2 +5) for individual informant ranking, and (statement sort value) or (2 +5) for discourse group ranking.

The Landless (Discourse A): I want to return, but the erosion took my land

Discourse A explains 11% of the study variance or 15 out of 46 participants are highly associated with this discourse group. The female dominated Landless group, with an average age of 33, employs a narrative concerning the riverbank erosion that the participants faced on Bhola Island. The Landless agrees the most with statement ‘The riverbank erosion forced us to move here (2 +5)’, and disagreed the most with ‘Things would have been better if I never moved here (5 −5)’. The erosion is described as a problem much deeper than a temporary stress back on the island. It marks a turning point that has ended up cursing their lives and future:

Q1: The river sucked everything out of us (46:2 +5).

Q2: I lost everything. To be able to survive I had to come here (12:2 +5).

Q3: The riverbank erosion is the only thing that drove us here (45:2 +4).Footnote 4

The Landless expressed living in great fear due to the risk of eviction, and feeling disappointed about how the move turned out:

Q9: We know that we can get evicted anytime. We have to be ready to go (6:1 +4).

Q10: Many people leave thinking that [their living conditions would improve]. That is what I thought too, before I came to this place (26:27 +4).

Q11: I came here with a lot of dreams and expectations (60:27 +5).Footnote 5

Next to the disappointment, the Landless convey a feeling of meaninglessness or emotional emptiness. This is, for example, captured in the way happiness and honour are referred to as luxury items:

Q13: Poor people cannot afford happiness (7:32 0).

Q14: What am I supposed to do with happiness? (6:32 0).

Q15: Poor people do not need to be honoured (7:4 0).

Interestingly, given this dissatisfaction the Landless appear mentally and emotionally ill-prepared to move on. The group emphasises that the lack of financial resources and land, prevents them from moving, or returning to Bhola Island:

Q16: I cannot afford to go to a better place (21:3 −4).

Q17: I can maybe afford to buy a bus ticket, but I do not have anywhere to live in my homeland (38:26 −5).

Q18: If I could buy some land and build a house, then I would go home (52:30 0).Footnote 6

The Landless refer to Bhola Island as home, and most of them would have stayed, or returned, if it was not because of the erosion. The group does not express similar attachments to Bhola Slum or to its social environment. This comes through in how the settlement is portrayed. It is not described as a place where people want to be. Nonetheless, the Landless seem to temporary have come to terms with the idea that they will need to stay:

Q20: This is a very dirty and crowded place, but we have nowhere else to go (59:19 0).

Q21: There are drug addicts here so we cannot bring up our children properly, but we have nowhere else to go (52:35 0).

Q22: I have to stay here I guess. That is just it (60:32 0).Footnote 7

The Displaced (Discourse B): This is not where I belong, I want to go home

Discourse B explains 8% of the study variance or 11 out of 46 participants are highly associated with this discourse group. The male dominated Displaced group, with an average age of 35, expresses a strong feeling of being displaced or not belonging in the settlement. This is reflected in the Q-statement ranked as most agree; ‘I would like to return to my home district (30 +5)’, and in the distinguishing statements ranked higher than by the other groups:

Q27: I want to go back to my village as it is a wonderful place for me to live in (40:30 +4).

Q28: I do not want to live here. I want to go back to my village (43:23 +4).

Q29: I am homesick. I enjoyed life in my hometown so much (15:5 +4).Footnote 8

Similarly, this is observed in how the Displaced describe the settlement and its social environment:

Q33: I feel no connection to this place (15:25 −4).

Q34: Back in the village there was honour, but in this place all people do is counting money (29:4 +5).

Q35: The island was so much safer. I do not like this place. There are dangers here at night, thieves and kidnappers (15:8 −5).Footnote 9

One important difference from the Landless, is that the Displaced explain their migration decision by economic reasonings rather than due to environmental stress. This is expressed in the Q-statements they disagree with the most ‘The cyclones were the main reason why I moved here (12 −5)’ and in the overall narrative. Lack of financial resources is described as the main constraining factor keeping them in a place where they do not want to be:

Q40: I was hoping to increase my income, but I have lost hope (36:27 +4).

Q41: I will go to a better place when I have enough money (35:16 +4).

Q42: If I had enough money I would go, but the lack of money is the problem (38:16 +5).Footnote 10

The Sacrificed (Discourse C): Lost health and honour for economic gain

Discourse C explains 8% of the study variance or 9 out of 46 participants are highly associated with this discourse group. The Sacrificed group has an average age of 42 years which makes it the oldest group out of the five. Interestingly, most women are household decision-makers due to different circumstances such as their husband’s abandonment, illness or death. Poor health and difficulties to support the household financially are common elements of the men. Most of them report earning about 300–400 tk per day (at the time of writing, this was about £3 to £4). This is relatively low for a male day-labourer in Dhaka. The Sacrificed group report having some similar attitudes to the Landless. The group has strong feelings around the past impact of the erosion and the current fear of eviction:

Q47: I lost everything. To survive I was forced to come here (8:2 +5).

Q48: The erosion is the only reason why I am here (31:2 +5).

Q49: This is not our land. There are no papers or documentation allowing us to live here. If the government wants to, they can ask us to leave anytime. There is no security (1:1 +5).Footnote 11

The Sacrificed refers to their loss of wellbeing, or sacrifices made, by having to live in the settlement. The narrative of the Landless circulated around emotional numbness and hopelessness, while the Sacrificed’s storyline moves even further into the darkness. People are aware that they are forced to stay in the slum, although they express being fed-up and that they would like to escape:

Q53: Bhola Slum is not a good place, but what can we do? There is no other option (1:18 0).

Q54: I need to make sure to move. If I find a good opportunity, I will do so (3:9 −5).

Q55: I had to live here so obviously as a result I had to sacrifice my honour (33:4 +4).Footnote 12

The loss of honour (including religious norms) seems to affect women more, and is related back to the toxic social environment that creates in an unsafe female space. Q-statement ‘Women live a better life here (13 −5)’ is ranked as the statement the Sacrificed disagrees with the most:

Q62: There is not enough security here for women (20:13 −5).

Q63: Women face various problems here. Problems that make it difficult for them to maintain their religious obligations (3:13 −4).

Q64: The manner here is not to cover up. Women do not follow any religious values here (1:13 −4).Footnote 13

The Sacrificed explains their immobility with the lack of land and financial resources, but ill-health is mentioned as another important factor:

Q66: The lack of land is the main reason why I cannot leave (62:14 +4).

Q67: My family wants to go to a better place, but I cannot afford it (20:6 −4).

Q68: There are more job opportunities here, and my husband is sick (3:12 −4).Footnote 14

The Returners (Discourse D): I came here to save up money, after that I will return home

Discourse D explains 6% of the study variance or 6 out of 46 participants are highly associated with this discourse group. The Returners, with an average age just under 33, is the youngest group out of the five. Increased job opportunity was the main reason why the Returners decided to move to Bhola Slum. This is also reflected in the Q-interviews:

Q70: I came to Dhaka to earn money (41:31 +5).

Q71: I am here because of poverty (10:3 −5).

Q72: I came here to get a better life (25:5 +4).Footnote 15

The move was supposed to be temporary - to save up money and leave - people want to return home eventually:

Q75: We are all here to save up some money. Money that will enable us to buy a piece of land and get a house (25:30 +5).

Q76: I hope I will be able to go back, back to Bhola (56:30 +4).

Q77: If I can arrange enough money, I will go back to my birthplace, Bhola (56:10 +5).

An important difference from the Sacrificed and the Displaced, is the disagreement of Q-statements on ill-health, loss of honour and feelings of not belonging:

Q78: No one has to sacrifice their honour, but they have to work hard (10:4 −4).

Q79: We are physically well by the grace of God almighty, but we are lacking money (51:3 −4).

Q80: I know this is not a good place, but I live here. I have the right to be here (41:23 −4).Footnote 16

The Returners emphasise having the right to be here, but they do not express a strong attachment to the settlement:

Q83: They are not my people. Not everyone is that helpful (56:33 −5).

Q84: I have not invested anything into this place (25:22 −4).

Q85: If we could find a better place, we would move (51:23 0).Footnote 17

The Returners convey a complex perspective of immobility or being ‘trapped’. People want to leave the settlement in a few years and return home. Additionally, they would not have migrated here if they would have gotten by financially in their home villages. The Returners came here with a clear purpose; to save up money and return. This is a state of limbo—they must, need and are fairly comfortable here—but this temporary satisfaction heavily depends on the hope of returning home to a better future. The satisfactory status quo could change if they do not manage to return home with some savings in a few years. This uncertainty is expressed in the interviews:

Q92: It all depends on the situation (51:30 0).

Q93: I do not know anything about what will happen. Allah knows better than us all (41:29 0).

The Dreamers (Discourse E): Urban dreams of betterment

Discourse E explains 7% of the study variance or 5 out of 46 participants are highly associated with this discourse group. The Dreamers has an average age of 40 years. This is the only group that does not identify themselves as landless. Most women stopped working once they had children or when their children became of age to care for them financially. Three households have a TV and two a fridge. These symbols of wealth were not found in the other groups. Most women interestingly moved from the island to escape family issues with their step-mothers. This is also the only group that want to move abroad. In Mauritius or Saudi Arabia, they can make good money, create a better life and fulfil their dreams. The Dreamers top-rank Q-statements around eviction, women’s safety and men’s decision-making rights:

Q94: We are always afraid of eviction (5:2 +5).

Q95: Ladies live a secure life here (19:13 +4).

Q96: He is the head of the family so we will have to follow his decisions (48:39 +5).Footnote 18

This is the only group that ranks Q-statement ‘I would like to return to my home district (30 −5)’ as most disagree:

Q98: My children are studying here and we do not have anything left in our village (5:30 −5).

Q99: My husband has no land in our home district. My step-mother is also there and my father’s condition is not very well (48:30 −5).

Q100: All has been taken away by the river. I do not want to go there (4:30 −4).

The Dreamers do not believe that things would be better if they never migrated here, nor that Bhola Slum is a bad place:

Q101: It is not that Bhola Slum is not a good place, it is just that there are too many people here (4:18 −4).

Q102: Anything can happen anywhere. This place is safe (19:28 −4).

Q103: I am happy here because our relatives are here and life is not that expensive (19:32 −4).

The migration from the island is explained by domestic abuse and trauma, as well as environmental stress:

Q104: I was tortured by my step-mother. I came here to make a better life, and to get a job in the garment factories (48:27 +4).

Q105: If the riverbank erosion would not have grabbed the land from us, we may be happier living on our own land (19:2 +4).

Q106: If I had my land, and my father would not have died, then we might be living well (32:5 −4).Footnote 19

The Dreamers do not want to return to Bhola Island, nor do they want to stay in Bhola Slum forever. People dream of betterment, but they currently do not manage to move. A number of reasons keep them here such as ill-health and weak household economy:

Q111: I carry several diseases, so it is hard for me to move to another place (32:3 +4).

Q112: My mental strength allows me to move, but we are not economically stable. Our relatives are also here (19:17 0).

Q113: I am a widow, and I have to think about my children so I cannot move (5:34 +4).Footnote 20

Discussion

All groups expressed a desire to leave Bhola Slum. However, why, how, when and to where strongly differentiated between the discourse groups. The Landless, Displaced and Sacrificed wanted to return home to Bhola Island, while the Returners mentioned other rural places, and the Dreamers other urban areas or countries. Some would stay in the settlement for a few years, while others wanted to escape immediately. Some said that their mobility was restricted by the lack of land, others financial resources, poor physical or mental health, and emotional wellbeing.

The captured notions around the (im)mobility state were in this way most diverse. If anything, the analysis illustrated how the complex state of climate-induced (im)mobility interlinks with people’s wellbeing. The findings outlined a long line of climate-induced non-economic losses and damages that people faced through the rural-urban move from the island, and through the displacement in the slum. These included the loss of identity, honour, sense of belonging, physical and mental health or wellbeing. It is important to acknowledge that people faced these losses although many of them ‘decided’ to migrate (Barnett et al., 2016; Tschakert et al., 2019). These are crucial findings for the upcoming UNFCCC climate policy discussions that are to shape the conceptual development of Loss and Damage, and advise on how to best support vulnerable people facing such losses.

A suggestion of how to widen our understanding of the interlinkages between (im)mobility and wellbeing, is to frame more empirical studies around the Non-Economic Loss and Damage concept and its links to mental health. The lack of empirical insights investigating the emotional and mental aspects of climate change impacts otherwise risk being a costly public health inaction (Watts et al., 2017, 2018, 2019). Bhola Slum captured a long line of mental ill-health descriptions, such as anxiety and acute stress reaction to the eviction risk, depression and apathy due to the loss of identity and belonging, or trauma and PTSD in relation to physical and psychological abuse. The lack of wellbeing often related to new urban (and gendered) risks such as the work conditions in the garment factories, or the living conditions in the slum (Ezeh et al., 2017; Parvin et al., 2018; Fitch et al., 2017, 2018). The study clearly illustrated how people’s, and in particularly women’s, immobility go far beyond economic constraints.

We know how to treat mental ill-health and disorders, such as trauma, depression and anxiety. More political and financial efforts must be made to ensure that climate-induced migrants, displaced and immobile populations have immediate access to psychological support upon their arrival. People must have a chance to heal any trauma related to what may have forced them to leave, or to what they found when they arrived. People need support to adjust themselves to their new living conditions. At the same time, the root causes to people’s interrupted wellbeing can be traced back to deeper structural, political and societal disfunction, such as poverty, unhealthy living conditions, labour- and human rights violations. The recommendation of a family doctor in such a situation would surely be that ‘we must treat the problem rather than the symptoms’.

Whenever mobility is framed as an adaptive policy solution for ‘trapped’ populations, one must question whom the solution is for, and by whom it is raised (Black and Collyer, 2014; Ayeb-Karlsson et al., 2018). Similarly, we must ask why there has been a rural overrepresentation of populations deemed ‘trapped’, why the rural perspective of rural–urban migration is more widespread within environmental migration research, and why climate-induced migration or mobility is more commonly investigated than climate-induced immobility (Baldwin, 2016; Schewel, 2019).

For decades migration scholars have debated around what defines, and who is to define forced or involuntary and voluntary migration. This study however showed that more focus is needed on the diverse ways that climate-induced (im)mobility may damage and erode someone’s wellbeing. We need to know more about how to reduce, minimise and address these damages to protect people’s wellbeing. It is time that we acknowledge that not only people who are forced to migrate face eroding wellbeing, but also people who choose to migrate. This became evident as some of the discourse groups clearly fell more under an involuntary migration scenario than others. Similarly, the study showcased that researchers need to refrain from searching for ‘permanently immobile rural populations’ and open up to wider scenarios where mobility can lead to immobility, and where climate-induced immobility can be urbanely placed and short-termed or temporary rather than permanent.

Conclusion

This empirical study has illustrated a range of modes related to urban-immobility. The findings demonstrate the need to widen our understanding of immobility and ‘trapped’ populations from simply being financial, practical and functional towards a more complex subjective and psychosocial process. Psychosocial processes (such as identity loss and place attachment) may generate or reinforce someone’s subjective immobility. Mental health was indicated as a contributing factor to people’s immobility status. For example, people who have experienced traumatic events, such as violence, abuse, or dealing with depressive thoughts, strongly aligned their compromised wellbeing with their immobility status. To our knowledge, this study is pioneering in the sense that it first in investigating mental health and wellbeing as an element of ‘trapped’ populations or immobility. However, further research explicitly investigating mental disorders and ill-health in climate-induced (im)mobility settings must follow. We urgently need more research investigations of the mental health impacts of migration, but also of the urban immobility state. This will help us comprehend what the mental health impacts are, why people develop them—drawing out the longer health pathways, as well as how to support vulnerable individuals.

Similarly, we need more in-depth people-centred studies from different geographical, cultural and social research settings to reveal the similarities and differences in states of immobility. It is clear that some of the presented findings in this study are location specific, while others potentially can be generalised to a wider context. The understanding of psychosocial processes and their relationship to subjective immobility, for example, as well as the discursive decision-making model are possibly human related rather than socially and culturally specific. This article also provides some valuable and replicable research tools. The research method will likely prove useful and effective for further studies in similar research areas. The hope is that the detailed and transparent methods section will effectively support and facilitate the application for researchers.

A commonly embraced idea around migration is that it is a rationale decision based around a combination of push and pull factors. For example, factors such as pay differentials between migration origin and destination, a desire for household livelihood and risk diversification, and education and health service opportunities are often cited as factors that push and pull people away from and to locations. This study illustrates that the process of decision making around migration and particularly immobility can also be thought of as a function of a complex and delicate network of elements (as the proposed model outline). Subjective and psychosocial feelings and emotions boosting or reducing people’s wellbeing status, for example, often relate to whether an intention, desire or aspiration to migrate, leads to migrating. The state of an individual’s mind plays a crucial role here. This study sheds light on the relationship between thinking and feeling that one is trapped and being trapped, as well as widening the ways that people may identify themselves as trapped.