Introduction

Earthquake early warning (EEW) is becoming more utilised around the world. EEW systems are designed to detect earthquakes at their source or detect the arrival of P-waves before shaking from the S-waves arrives (Given et al., 2018). After detection, a warning notification is sent out to locations that can expect shaking. This notification may give anywhere from a few seconds to two minutes warning before shaking occurs. If a location is close to the epicentre of an earthquake, it will not likely receive a warning prior to the shaking arriving. Additionally, the amount of warning time available and the eventual shaking intensities felt, depend on the nature of the fault rupture that produces the earthquake. Even though longer warning times are possible, many places are only likely to get <10 s warning of strong shaking (Minson et al., 2018).

EEWs are beneficial, as they allow organisations to take either automated or procedural actions to counter the impacts of the shaking. Examples of organisational actions include slowing trains, stopping surgeries, or turning off dangerous or essential equipment (Johnson et al., 2016; Allen and Melgar, 2019). A warning can also give employees and the public enough time to take protective actions such as moving away from dangerous areas or performing the ‘drop, cover and hold’ action. Consequently, EEWs have the potential to reduce damage, minimise injuries and save lives (Sutton et al., 2020).

A number of countries have functioning EEW systems including Japan, Taiwan, South Korea, Mexico, and the USA (Allen and Melgar, 2019). Every country’s system is slightly different—some countries have national systems of sensors, some use stand-alone alerting devices (often networked), and others rely on a combination of both. The source, channels and messages provided for the warnings also differ depending on the country context.

In Aotearoa New Zealand, EEW is still in its infancy, with no comprehensive national system in place. While some countries have undertaken research on organisational uses of EEW before system development (e.g. USA (Goltz, 2001; Johnson et al., 2016); Mexico (Espinosa-Aranda et al., 2011); Japan (Horiuchi, 2009; Hoshiba, 2014); various European nations (Clinton et al., 2016)) and recently in New Zealand (Becker et al., 2020)), exploratory research on public utility of earthquake warnings has rarely been undertaken before developing EEW technology. Consequently, we took a different approach; using surveys to investigate whether EEW is considered useful and worth the investment by the public.

As New Zealand has limited experience of EEW, the researchers looked to Japan, where a national EEW system has been officially operated by the Japan Meteorological Agency (JMA) since 2007 (Fujinawa and Noda, 2013). Given many years of public exposure to EEWs and limited research on people’s response to these warnings, we, the authors, sought to understand how the Japanese public react by undertaking two public surveys following warnings for earthquakes that occurred in Gunma and Chiba Prefectures in 2018 (Nakayachi et al., 2019). While the aim of these surveys was to better understand how the Japanese public responds to EEWs, it also had a secondary purpose of developing questions for a survey in New Zealand.

Using the Japan survey questions (Nakayachi et al., 2019) as a starting point, we modified the survey for the New Zealand context. In the survey we sought to understand the public’s perspectives on whether an EEW system would be useful for New Zealand. We asked about their preferences for such a system, including desired warning thresholds, response timeframes, channel for receiving a warning, and message preferences. To understand people’s likely behavioural response to an EEW, we also used a hypothetical scenario to enquire about what people anticipated doing on receipt of a warning. This scenario was based on similarities to the real EEWs experienced by Gunma and Chiba Prefectures in 2018. Our paper presents the survey data and provides a commentary on how these data might inform future directions for EEW, both in New Zealand and internationally.

Methodology

We chose to use a survey to collect empirical data on people’s perspectives about EEW, situating our research primarily in a positivist philosophy, with a deductive approach (Melnikovas, 2018). Survey data is useful as it can provide an overview of people’s thoughts and actions, is generalisable across populations, and can be applied to existing theoretical frameworks. Both the Japan and New Zealand surveys were developed based on published theoretical research on people’s behaviour to warnings (Mileti and Sorensen, 1990; Goltz, 2001; Lindell and Perry, 2012; Dunn et al., 2016; Wood et al., 2017). Aspects that informed our EEW questions included: the importance of information receipt, interpretation and confirmation; previous experience of events; threat perceptions and perceptions of protective action; the effect of biases (e.g. optimism bias); environmental and social influences on the warning process; trust in warnings; and the availability of personal resources (including self- and response-efficacy, and practical skills gained through undertaking earthquake preparedness activities and exercises).

Materials

For the New Zealand survey we adapted the Japanese questions for relevance to the cultural context and added others about warning content and timeframes. The New Zealand survey contained 20 questions in total, with18 quantitative (single response, multiple response, and Likert scale) and 2 qualitative (free response) questions (see Supplementary Information). The questions focused on previous earthquake experience, anticipated behavioural response to EEWs (within the context of a hypothetical scenario based on the 2018 Gunma and Chiba earthquakes), reasons for those anticipated responses, perceived usefulness of EEWs, preferred attributes of a warning system (i.e., warning thresholds, timeframes, channels, and messages), earthquake preparedness, and demographics.

We followed Massey University of Aotearoa New Zealand human ethics procedures and our research was deemed to be low risk, resulting in the submission of a Low-risk Human Ethics Notification (Ethics Notification Number: 4000019302). The survey ran online via SurveyMonkey from 22 March 2019 to 30 April 2019. The survey was promoted by press release (resulting in a newspaper article in a national paper and radio interviews) and social media (e.g. Facebook, Twitter).

Data

We received a total of 3084 responses from across New Zealand. Basic frequency analyses were undertaken using the SPSS and Excel software programmes, and frequency tables showing percentages were constructed from the quantitative data. Tests of significance were undertaken, but are not reported in this paper, and will be focus of future work. Themes were coded from the free-response qualitative data and analysed using methods recommended by Corbin and Strauss (2014). Quotes reflecting these themes are presented throughout this paper. Given we had over 3000 respondents, the themes for Question 15, which asked about people’s anticipated responses for different warning timeframes, were also further organised into categories and analysed quantitatively (see section “Warning time versus actions”).

Participants

Large numbers of responses came from the regional areas of Auckland, Canterbury, East Coast North Island (Hawke’s Bay/Gisborne), Wellington, and Otago/Southland (Table 1). Respondents were more likely than average to be female (65%) and to consider their ethnicity to be New Zealander (40%) or New Zealand European (46%). Males (33%) and other specific ethnic identities, including Māori (4%), were underrepresented in the survey. A range of people from different age groups filled out the survey (Table 1) similar to the age profile of New Zealand where the median age is 37.4 years and ~45% of the population is between 30 and 64 years (NZ.Stat., n.d.).

Table 1 Location and demographic characteristics of the EEW survey participants.

Limitations

The gender and ethnicity bias in the data pose potential limitations. Previous work exploring the role of demographics in earthquake preparedness behaviour suggests mixed impact of such biases. For example, some researchers have found gender to influence earthquake preparedness (Mulilis, 1999), while others suggest demographics has only limited impact on the preparedness process (Becker et al., 2015). In addition to demographic bias, the survey self-selection process might mean the survey was answered by people with more interest or experience in earthquake hazards, skewing the answers toward a position of greater knowledge. Use of a hypothetical scenario may have influenced participants’ answers about their intended responses, and preferred attributes of a warning system. Finally, the survey might also have been impacted by social desirability bias (Phillips and Clancy, 1972) whereby respondents answered questions in ways they thought favourable to the researchers to appear more compliant with social norms. Consequently, EEW survey responses should be interpreted in the context of potential demographic, personal and contextual biases.

Results and discussion

Usefulness of EEW

Overall, respondents were supportive of having an EEW system. Nearly 97% thought that a system would be either “Useful” (76%) or “Somewhat useful” (19%). These results are similar to those from Japan and Mexico where somewhere between 64–75% of the public in those countries perceive their EEW systems to be useful, despite experiencing occasional false or missed alarms (Nakayachi et al., 2019; Santos-Reyes, 2019). Reasons New Zealanders thought that EEW would be useful included that they could prepare for physical actions they needed to take (91%) including protecting themselves (79%; aligning with 75% in a previous USA study (Dunn et al., 2016) and helping or saving others nearby (79%; Table 2). Interestingly, as per the Japanese experience, New Zealanders were also very supportive of the fact that EEW could help them mentally prepare for impending shaking. The percentage of support for this benefit was identical to results from the Japanese study (83%), confirming support for EEW’s value for psychological preparedness. In the “Other” category (6%) there were also some unique reasons people gave as qualitative comments for supporting EEW including a focus on ensuring a warning would be given to essential services (n = 17), assessing tsunami risk (n = 15), and protecting pets (n = 14).

Table 2 Reasons why New Zealand respondents thought EEW would be useful, compared with Japan respondents (Nakayachi et al., 2019, p.11).

Of the small proportion (3%, n = 87) who thought EEW would be “Somewhat useless” or “Useless”, their primary reason related to the fact that they might not be able to react to a warning in time (59%), a reason not shared as strongly by Japanese participants. In Japan only a third or less of people from both Gunma and Chiba prefectures who had experienced a real EEW indicated that they had been unable to respond to the warnings they had received (Nakayachi et al., 2019—see also Table 3). In contrast, Japanese participants were more likely to think that EEW might be useless because they might not have received a warning for past earthquakes until after the shaking had started (Study 1 Gunma: 68%; Study 2 Chiba 64%; New Zealand 53%). New Zealanders additionally thought that EEW effectiveness might be questionable because they might not notice a warning (51%), or despite receiving a warning that says “strong shaking” might only receive minor shaking (39%) or no shaking at all (31%). In the “Other” category (30%) some responses purported that EEW was a waste of time (n = 13) while one person highlighted that patchy mobile phone coverage might prove an issue for effective warning in New Zealand.

Table 3 Reasons why respondents thought EEW might be useless, compared with Japan respondents (Nakayachi et al., 2019, p.11).

Preferred attributes of a warning system

Modified Mercalli intensity thresholds

New Zealand has its own Modified Mercalli (MM) intensity shaking scale (Dowrick, 1996). Participants reported the minimum level of MM intensity for which they thought a warning would be useful. They were provided with a table showing the descriptions for MM3–MM8+ (Table 4) along with a cartoon picture from GeoNet Felt Rapid that reflects each level (Haubrock et al., 2017; GeoNet, n.d. a) and were asked to select their preferred minimum intensity. The most popular intensity level was MM5 or moderate shaking (51%) followed by MM6 or strong shaking (25%) (Table 5).

Table 4 The New Zealand Modified Mercalli (MM) intensity shaking scale (from GeoNet (Geonet, n.d. b), based on Dowrick (1996)).
Table 5 Minimum MM intensity levels for warnings suggested by respondents.

Because MM scales vary internationally, it is difficult to tell whether the most popular choices of category for the New Zealand survey align with thresholds already used for warnings in other countries. New Zealand MM5-6 potentially aligns with thresholds for public warnings in places like Japan (5L on the Japanese intensity scale, strong shaking (Nishimae, 2018)) but appears to be slightly higher than the MM thresholds used in the USA. Previously US thresholds have been identified as MMI 4 (light shaking: the threshold for light building damage) (Given et al., 2018), however debate has recently arisen following the 2019 Ridgecrest earthquake in California, about lowering thresholds for alerting the public to US MMI 3. Public who felt the shaking but did not receive a warning due to a higher threshold suggested a desire to be notified about large earthquakes even with low shaking intensities at certain locations (Cochrane and Husker, 2019).

Our data suggests that the public support higher alerting thresholds; however, this is within a New Zealand specific context where 73% of participants had experienced strong shaking previously (e.g. 2010–2011 Canterbury earthquakes) and where there is no operating EEW infrastructure. It is possible that influences such as previous experience, individual perspectives, and social and environmental factors may place additional demands on threshold setting.

Minimum warning time needed to respond

We asked survey participants the minimum time that they would need to undertake a response to the warning (i.e., between receiving an alert and feeling the shaking). Interestingly, we received a wide range of responses to this question. Few people (2%) felt they could do anything with 0–2 s of warning. The greatest proportion of people (21%) felt that 6–10 s of warning was the minimum time needed, similar to what is reported in an EEW study by Goltz (2001). Others’ perspectives on the minimum amount of warning needed were evenly distributed over the other timeframes (Table 6). We further explore what people think they might do within these minimum timeframes in the section “Warning time versus actions”.

Table 6 Minimum amount of time respondents felt they needed to undertake a response to the warning presented in the scenario.

Warning channel

People’s preferred channel for receiving a warning was via mobile phone (83%), consistent with data from Japan, where following the Gunma and Chiba prefecture earthquakes, most people received warnings via their mobile phones (~89%) (Nakayachi et al., 2019). ShakeAlert, the EEW system for the West Coast of the USA, is exploring options for delivery of warnings through mobile phones, including via smartphone apps and Wireless Emergency Alert (WEA) cell broadcasts (Given et al., 2018), both of which have been utilised in tests and real events such as the Ridgecrest earthquake (Cochrane and Husker, 2019). Similarly, the New Zealand National Emergency Management Agency (NEMA) currently use cell broadcasts to send Emergency Mobile Alerts (EMA) as warnings about different hazard events (National Emergency Management Agency, n.d.a) and this channel could be a viable option for sending alerts.

Research highlights the importance of multiple channels to deliver warnings (Sorensen, 2000; McGinley et al., 2006; Wright et al., 2014). Accordingly, additional to mobile phones, there were also several other channels indicated by participants as useful, including television (47%), radio (39%), public announcement (33%), and social media (31%). Given the short warning timeframes for EEW, social media is probably the most challenging warning notification channel, however even with very short delivery timeframes, research into use of crowdsourced Twitter data (Steed et al., 2019) suggests that social media might present an opportunity for EEW. Platforms such as Twitter have been used effectively to deliver warnings during disaster events in Indonesia (Chatfield and Brajawidagda, 2013) with some cross-cultural evidence suggesting that social media is used during disasters similarly in different countries (e.g. Australia and China (Kulemeka, 2014)). However, Twitter may be a useful post-alert messaging channel, to explain what occurred e.g. false alert, late alert, missed alert, inaccurate alert (McBride et al., 2020).

Message content

We suggested to participants that on receipt of an EEW alert they might receive the following message: “Earthquake early warning, strong shaking expected soon”, along with the name of the agency issuing the warning. We then asked participants to indicate on a five-point scale how strongly they agreed or disagreed that certain types of information should also be included (Table 7). Participants supported the inclusion of three additional types of information in an earthquake warning message: (1) Information on additional geohazards that might occur such as tsunami or landslides; (2) further information about the earthquake that has occurred; and (3) guidance on what action to take.

Table 7 People’s agreement or disagreement about additional information that could be included in an EEW alert.

Support for the inclusion of additional information about likely geohazards points to the need to integrate EEWs with other related hazards. Tsunami is particularly relevant, given the length of coastline in New Zealand and the potential for a near-shore tsunami to be generated, putting coastal communities at significant risk. Japan has integrated its earthquake and tsunami warnings into one system to provide a chain of information to users to which they can respond accordingly (Japan Meteorological Agency, 2019).

New Zealanders also had an appetite to receive further information about the earthquake itself (e.g., location and magnitude). Such data is currently provided via the GeoNet system via rapid post-earthquake notifications but could also be integrated into an EEW chain of alerts. Further information about an earthquake could potentially come after the initial warning as it does in Japan, either directly or by provision of a link as supported by 72% of participants.

Finally, participants suggested that an EEW message might also include some information about what actions to take in response to the alert. There has been debate about whether such information should be included in warning messages, as actions might differ depending on the context a person is in (Wood, 2018; Goltz et al., 2020). However short action messages may be useful as a reminder for action, providing that people are also familiar with these actions prior to receiving a warning (e.g., through education, training and exercises). Guidance messages for action are a crucial aspect of short warning messages to achieve an optimal behavioural response (Mileti and Sorensen, 1990; Mileti and Darlington, 1995; Potter, 2018). New Zealand’s current guidance is to ‘drop, cover and hold’ (McBride et al., 2019); this message, or an associated image of this action if the channel allows it, could be included in future EEW messages. A reminder about noticing whether the earthquake shaking is ‘long or strong’ could also help with rapid tsunami self-evacuations after the shaking.

Interestingly, there was less support for including a countdown timer in an alert (65%), providing a description of potential impacts from the shaking (60%), or including a reassuring messaging (e.g., “Stay calm”). While more participants than not liked the idea of using a countdown timer to know when shaking will begin, some felt that this might cause anxiety. Perhaps the same could be said of knowing what the potential impacts might be. Reassuring messages may also not be seen as useful by participants as reassurance may be better gained by a sense of control of knowing what is going to happen and what actions are needed in response; for example, action coping (i.e., coping with a threat by focusing on actions which can be taken rather than on the threat itself) increases protective behaviours for earthquakes (McIvor and Paton, 2007).

In terms of participants’ free responses, several people (n = 9) suggested it was important to include information that notified people if it was a false alarm or if the event had ended, for example if the threat of shaking had passed. Additionally, 43 people cautioned that there should not be too much information in a message as there might be limited time available to read it all. The latter is an important point; initial EEW messages should be succinct given the short timeframes available for warnings. Recent research has identified, however, that longer mobile phone alerts can reduce the tendency for receivers to delay actions by searching for additional information, than if they receive shorter warning messages for imminent threats (Wood et al., 2017). The diversity of interest in information suggests that EEW should perhaps not be thought of as a single alert but a continuum of linked messages in which the first alert is the beginning of a chain of advice.

Intended responses to an EEW

We used an earthquake scenario to understand the anticipated responses to an EEW by New Zealand public survey participants. The scenario was based on the timing and nature of Gunma and Chiba earthquakes in Japan (Nakayachi et al., 2019), so that we could make comparisons with the public response from these two real earthquakes that activated Japan’s EEW.

The scenario read as follows:

“It is Saturday evening at 8.30 p.m. An earthquake occurs somewhere near you, but you don’t know the location of the earthquake or how big it is (the magnitude). The earthquake automatically triggers an earthquake early warning. You receive this warning as a mobile phone alert before shaking occurs. The alert says “Earthquake Early Warning. Strong shaking expected soon”. You don’t know how long you have before the shaking starts, or how strong the shaking will be in your location. However, you could assume with “strong shaking” it would be hard to walk, and there would be falling objects and moving furniture”.

Participants were asked to think about where they were on the previous Saturday evening, so as to provide a real context from which to answer the questions. The scenario was set on a weekend evening so that most people would likely be inside at home, as per the events in Gunma and Chiba. Indeed, most stated that they were inside at home (77%), or inside at another location such as a restaurant, bar, movie theatre, educational facility, or friend’s house (17%). Only a small proportion were outside (2%), in a vehicle such as a car or bus (1%), or in some other location (3%).

We asked participants to state how likely they would be to undertake a number of listed actions on receipt of an EEW. These anticipated actions aligned with real actions asked about in the Japan surveys (Nakayachi et al., 2019) but instead of using a yes/no response, and to reflect the uncertainty associated with future intentions, participants were asked to select from options on a 5-point scale from extremely unlikely to extremely likely.

The most likely intended action (i.e. “extremely likely” and “likely” combined) was a motionless reaction, where people said they would “mentally prepare for the shaking” (91%) (Table 8). Sixteen percent of survey respondents in the Gunma earthquake and a quarter of people in the Chiba earthquake stated that this is what they did in response to an actual earthquake warning, showing rates of New Zealanders’ intending to mentally prepare for the shaking is much higher than rates during an actual Japan EEW. Other intended motionless reactions by the New Zealand participants with high rates included “look for further information about the warning” (67%) and “stop and stay still, awaiting shaking on the spot” (34%). The proportion of people who suggested that they would “stop and stay still” aligns with previous data on observed behaviour during shaking (Prati et al., 2013; Lindell et al., 2016), and perhaps is a function of the high number of New Zealand participants who have experienced strong shaking previously (73%). Both the reactions “look for further information” and “stop and stay still” were reported at lower levels for the Gunma and Chiba warnings.

Table 8 Actions New Zealand survey participants thought they were “extremely likely” or “likely” to take on receipt of an EEW (with the two categories combined), compared with “what respondents did” in the Gunma and Chiba Earthquakes, Japan (Nakayachi et al., 2019, p. 7).

We asked participants to provide free responses on the reasons why they thought it was “extremely likely” that they would undertake these motionless reactions. People suggested they might stay still for different reasons including because they were afraid, had done so in a past event (e.g., 2010–2011 Canterbury earthquakes), or because they were mentally preparing themselves for the impending shaking. The benefits of mentally preparing were articulated by respondents, many of whom suggested that a few seconds of mental preparedness could help with responding appropriately:

“Being able to mentally prepare for an earthquake beforehand would mean a clearer head when it actually occurs, and not fumbling around thinking of what to do.”

There were some who articulated that while they might mentally prepare during the warning timeframe, they might only take an action once shaking starts:

“Giving myself mental preparation and not panicking is what I [would] do, if I had just a bit of time [to] prepare I would feel calmer and more likely to act quickly when the shaking actually started.”

Looking for further earthquake information was linked to the desire to confirm the warning information, as highlighted in comments such as “to check and confirm the news”. This is a common reaction to warning messages, and causes a delay in responding, referred to as ‘milling’ (Wood et al., 2017). The key way to shorten the duration of milling is to ensure that all elements of a warning message are included (Wood et al., 2015; Potter, 2018). Looking for further information is also linked with trust in the system (e.g., one person suggested they would seek information because, “I wouldn’t trust the warning system to be providing me relevant information”). Trust in the information source is an important factor in interpretation and response (Mileti and Sorensen, 1990; Wogalter et al., 1999; Becker et al., 2019). Even with extremely short timeframes for EEW, looking for information appeared to be part of the process in determining what action to take (e.g., “…[look for] more information to make an informed decision on what to do”).

Overall, participants were optimistic in their intentions to take physical action on receipt of an EEW. More than 80% of survey participants suggested it was extremely likely or likely they would move nearby to where they thought it would be safe (89%), help others or act to protect others (89%), take specific behaviours to protect themselves on the spot such as drop, cover, hold (86%), or tell other people the earthquake is coming (84%). Many also thought they would undertake safety actions (67%). Reasons for taking such actions included a desire to protect themselves so that they were safe and survived shaking without injury or death. Some also had real-life experience of these actions (e.g., in the 2010–2011 Canterbury or 2016 Kaikōura earthquakes), or had practiced an exercise such as the ShakeOut drill (McBride et al., 2019; Vinnell et al., 2020). There was a particularly strong sense of responsibility for others, as seen in other earthquake contexts (Turner et al., 1986; Dooley et al., 1992; Edwards, 1993; Russell et al., 1995; McIvor and Paton, 2007; Becker et al., 2014), with participants wanting to warn others about the EEW and take action to protect other people, particularly children but also other family members, pets, friends, and vulnerable persons (e.g., disabled, elderly, sick).

The figures of intended physical reactions are at odds with actual numbers of people who undertook such responses in warnings in Japan. Less than 10% of survey participants from Gunma and Chiba undertook any physical actions in response to the earthquake warnings (Nakayachi et al., 2019). A large body of literature has demonstrated that intentions strongly but do not completely predict behaviour (Webb and Sheeran, 2006), although some evidence supports that people’s intentions are acceptable proxies for behaviour (e.g., hurricane evacuation (Kang et al., 2007)).

Only 22% of participants thought it likely or extremely likely that they would do nothing in response to an EEW, a lower proportion than was seen in Japan (43% Gunma, 34% Chiba (Nakayachi et al., 2019)). There was an element of optimism from participants in that they thought the place they were in would be safe (30%), which was also seen in Japan (25% Gunma, 17% Chiba). However, our respondents were more likely to believe that they would not have enough time to undertake actions before the shaking began (30%) compared with Japan (5% for both Gunma and Chiba). Japanese survey respondents were more likely to do nothing in response to an EEW because they expected the impending shaking to be of small intensity (Gunma 28%; Chiba 38%), despite alerts only being sent out when strong shaking is anticipated. This is in contrast with New Zealand participants where only 11% thought this might influence their decision to do nothing. This speaks to the Japanese experience of early warnings, where perhaps repeated experience of warnings has helped them understand that they can take action, but they often do not because of their optimism that the shaking will not be severe and they will be safe (Nakayachi et al., 2019). This effect of experience has implications for EEW education and training; countries developing new EEW systems might need more of a focus on helping people understand what actions can be taken in response to a warning within a short timeframe, while countries with an advanced system may need to focus on reducing optimism and normalisation biases (Weinstein, 1980; Mileti and O’Brien, 1992; Spittal et al., 2005; Mclvor et al., 2009).

Participants provided suggestions about other actions they might take instead of the listed options. Actions that differed from those already listed included check or save pets (n = 48) and assess tsunami risk/evacuate from a tsunami (n = 35). These comments speak to two issues not picked up in the current EEW literature. The first is that pets comprise part of the decision-making process for EEW action. This has been seen in events with longer fuse warnings such as evacuation for the 2017 Port Hill (New Zealand) fires (Squance et al., 2018) and is reinforced in the EEW context.

The second is that people may consider evacuating for a tsunami threat on seeing an EEW alert. For example, one participant who suggested it was extremely likely they would move to safety and go outside on receipt of a warning said they would do so, “Because of the danger and tsunami risk of where we are”. As articulated in this instance, such evacuation may commence before shaking begins and people may move into an area that is dangerous during shaking, rather than protect themselves from shaking.

Warning time versus actions

Participants were invited to freely articulate what actions they thought they would take if they knew they had (a) 10 s or less of warning before earthquake shaking occurred, (b) 11–30 s of warning, (c) 31–60 s of warning, or (d) more than 60 s warning. Participants often provided multiple actions, and these were coded for each timeframe as ‘First action’, ‘Second action’, ‘Third action’, and ‘Fourth action’. Actions were then graphed for the different timeframes (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1: Anticipated actions for different warning timeframes.
figure 1

Showing actions that participants said they would take if they knew they had a 10s or less of warning before earthquake shaking occurred, b 11–30s of warning, c 31–60s of warning, or d more than 60s warning.

Participants were more likely to take no action or feel like they could not do anything for the shortest timeframe of <10 s. Similarly, for motionless reactions, people suggested that they were more likely to stay still and mentally prepare themselves if they thought they had <10 s of warning than if they had a longer warning timeframe. Mental preparedness also occurred for some people as a primary action for up to 30 s of warning, and as a secondary action for timeframes of up to 60 s and over. Looking for further information was suggested as a first action if people felt they had 30 s or more available to them before shaking started.

In terms of physical reactions, protecting oneself was seen more of a first action to take and was more likely to be taken in the first 10 s. The number of people suggesting that they would protect themselves as a primary action decreased the greater the warning time available and became predominantly a second, third, or fourth action. Undertaking safety actions had the opposite relationship, whereby these actions were more likely to be identified as a first action with 31–60+ s warning and less likely at 10 s or less. Telling other people that the earthquake was coming increased over warning timeframes; it was predominantly a first/second action within a warning timeframe of 0–30 s and a second/third action with a warning timeframe of 30 s and over. Many people selected “move nearby to safety” as an action across all timeframes, as a first action up to 30 s and a second and third action from 31 to 60 s and over. Only a few participants selected the option of specifically going outside/evacuating for the warning timeframe of <10 s, but the desire to undertake this action increased with available time, as highlighted also by previous research in a California setting (Goltz, 2001).

Analysis of the data highlights that perceptions of the time available between a warning and shaking will likely influence people’s responses to EEW. This has implications for decisions about whether information such as countdown timers, which show how many seconds there are before shaking begins in the receiver’s location, should be included in alerts. People may be more likely to stop, mentally prepare, and take protective action if they believe they only have a short time before shaking starts. This was reinforced by many of the respondents’ comments who suggested that for short warning timeframes they were extremely likely to undertake protective action because such actions were quick, easy, and provided the greatest benefit.

“Given [the] short window of opportunity, [you] need to take the steps that are most likely to have biggest impact”;

“They are incredibly simple tasks that require very little time to do—I would be able to do these (send a message, move under a table, etc.) without knowing how imminent the shaking is.”.

A belief that shaking will not start for many seconds more may increase people’s desire to move around increasing potential for injury should the shaking arrive (Johnston et al., 2014). However, knowledge of a longer timeframe before shaking begins may also have a benefit of being able to take other safety actions, for example, securing furniture or dangerous equipment, turning off equipment, or evacuating earthquake-prone buildings (Santos-Reyes, 2019). It is possible that for longer warning timeframes there may be a lower intensity of shaking, thus reducing risk and perhaps the imperative to undertake safety actions, but this is not a given and is dependent on the location and nature of the fault rupture (Minson et al., 2018). Lower levels of shaking may not always occur with longer warning timeframes. Consequently, EEW developers and emergency managers need to consider what creates the most benefit: providing messages about the time available before shaking so people can take safety actions or a simple message about imminent strong shaking which prompts immediate protective action.

Additionally, given that participants potentially intended to take up to four actions on receipt of a warning, actions cannot be considered mutually exclusive but part of a sequence of actions. This is also seen in other studies such as analysis of human behaviour in CCTV footage for the 2011 Christchurch earthquake (Lambie et al., 2016).

Conclusions

We undertook a public survey in Aotearoa New Zealand to better understand whether EEW was considered useful and acceptable by the public, before New Zealand makes an investment in such technology. Overall, the public strongly supported the concept of EEW and intended to take action before shaking to protect themselves and others. However, while intended actions are often a good predictor of real action (e.g., hurricane evacuation (Kang et al., 2007)) one wonders whether this would hold true given intention–behaviour gaps in other contexts (e.g. in the health risk context or for pro-environmental behaviours). The low rates of physical protective actions undertaken in Japan for real EEWs suggest that the results presented here should be interpreted with caution. In addition to protective action, the majority of participants indicated that they would use early warning as an opportunity to mentally prepare themselves for imminent shaking. This finding aligns with mental preparedness for actual warning instances in Japan (Nakayachi et al., 2019), suggesting that the value of this aspect may be transferable across contexts.

Participants shared their perspectives on preferred system attributes which included having MM intensity thresholds of 5–6 (moderate–strong shaking); receipt of warnings via mobile phones (supported by other channels); and a diversity of messages. In addition to receiving a warning message about “strong shaking expected soon” participants wanted supporting information about (a) the earthquake that had occurred, (b) additional geohazards such as tsunami, and (c) what actions to take.

Given the timeframes available for EEW response, initial messages should prioritise information that informs effective action (Potter, 2018; Wood, 2018). For example, an initial alert might provide a short message such as “Earthquake early warning, strong shaking expected soon, drop cover hold” or “Earthquake early warning, strong shaking expected soon, take protective action” (the latter of which could be useful across different contexts). More work is needed around how best to convey and support this information (Sutton et al., 2020).

After shaking has occurred, another short message could be delivered to coastal areas who received the EEW, stating that if the earthquake shaking felt ‘long or strong’ then evacuate immediately in case of tsunami (as per official guidance from emergency management (National Emergency Management Agency, n.d.b)). Subsequent messages could provide links to further information about the earthquake that has just occurred, the impacts, and what to do. This information could come from the same source or channel, or from separate sources or channels, as long as this integration is identified and addressed, as exemplified in the JMA system (Japan Meteorological Agency, 2019).

People’s anticipated actions on receipt of a warning differed over varying timeframes with people more likely to intend undertaking quicker and easier actions for shorter timeframes of <10 s (e.g., stop, mentally prepare, take protective action), and more likely to move to a nearby safe area, help others, look for more information, or take safety actions as timeframes increased. Mapping these actions out over time provides interesting insight into behaviours for short-fuse warnings and support for the idea that people likely need time to process the warning message and mentally prepare before acting (Wood et al., 2017).

Given that anticipated actions differed over varying timeframes, those developing EEW messaging should consider whether a priority is to focus on getting people to undertake quick protective actions or whether additional information like a countdown timer should be included to allow people to move around and undertake further safety actions. Given many alerts will only be received up to 10 s before shaking begins (Minson et al., 2018), it may be more realistic to focus public education on responses that are appropriate for that timeframe.

Finally, alert messages and educational and exercise initiatives should be aligned, to allow for consistency in understanding and response. Education and exercising comprises an important part of success in responding to an EEW. Educating people on how to respond to EEWs is an important component of the work being carried out for ShakeAlert in the USA (Cochran et al., 2018). Further, this suggestion is supported by differences between the Japan and New Zealand data; countries new to EEW might need to focus on training people how to respond to an EEW in the first instance, while countries with a mature system for which they have had personal experience receiving many EEWs, may need to focus on reducing optimism and normalisation biases.

In summary, our survey of the New Zealand public found support for having an EEW system, and optimism that such a system could be used for taking actions to assist safety and mental preparedness for earthquake shaking. We did not undertake a benefit–cost analysis or investigate benefits at a life-safety level; this could be an avenue for further research and has been undertaken by other countries (Hsiao et al., 2009; Cauzzi et al., 2016; Porter and Jones, 2018; Strauss et al., 2019; Bouta et al., 2020). Our recommendations for countries who are in the early stages of thinking about EEW, is to first take a step back and understand the social context within which the system is being developed. Our survey has contributed to this understanding for Aotearoa New Zealand. A better comprehension of social context will help develop warning system attributes, sources, channels and messages in ways that promote effective action, increasing the likelihood of such a warning reducing injuries and, potentially, fatalities.