Introduction

Gene editing as a new breeding technology

Interest in gene editing in agricultural crops has increased remarkably. Countries such as the United States have taken a proactive stance on utilising this technique; however, the products and even the technology itself still need time in many countries for clear positioning beyond technical, normative, ethical, and political concerns Schultz-Bergin, 2018; Sprink et al., 2016a; Tachikawa, 2017). Japan, which has relatively strict regulations on genetically modified (GM) crops, also is at the final stage of decision-making process (Japan 224th Food Safety Group, 2019). Genetic modification and gene editing are indeed different technologies, and if there are differences in public perceptions between those two, empirically investigating such differences in a country with a lower acceptance of GM food, Japan in the case of the present study, should promote clearer understandings of determinants for perceptions of gene edited crops.

There are two different political standpoints related to gene editing (Sprink et al., 2016b). First, product-based policy regards gene editing as a technology that is closer to conventional breeding than to genetic modification. This view is seen in the case when no exogenous gene is introduced in gene editing; conventional breeding can also bring the same desired results after a much longer time. Thus, the safety of these two should be considered to be similar. The second idea, process-based policy, situates gene editing closer to genetic modification: as long as DNA manipulation exists, the outcome brings higher risks and uncertainties; therefore, the method should be considered unconventional.

In previous research investigating both expert and public perceptions of risk of biotechnology application on food, experts significantly perceived less risk compared with the lay public (Savadori et al., 2004). This result leads to the prediction that with great expectations about emerging technology, researchers who have domain-specific scientific knowledge in biotechnology would tend to adopt product-based policy. Meanwhile, according to a recent interview report by Hopp et al. (2017), the public tends to take the process-based policy. However, a salient shortage exists in quantitative evidence on peopleā€™s risk perceptions toward application of gene editing in agricultural crops in relation to different levels of scientific knowledge. Thus, we statistically examined this topic in contrast with other existing technologies in genetic modification and conventional breeding using samples categorised into three groups by level of domain-specific scientific knowledge (experts in molecular biology, experts in other fields, and the lay public). Observing how different levels of domain-specific knowledge affect the response to each technology, including the latest, would deepen the understanding of public perceptions toward new science and technology (S&T), advancing related research.

Scientific knowledge, provided information, and attitude change

Psychological literature offers comprehensive evidence of the relationship between provided information and attitudinal change. People use available information when they make judgements (Finucane et al., 2002; Slovic et al., 1981). Thus, information should play an important role also in public risk, benefit, and value perceptions concerning food. The literature also suggests that outcomes are complex (Forgas, Cooper and Crano, 2011; Hyland and Birrell, 1979). A vast body of literature has discussed this topic in relation to scientific knowledge. For example, Zhu and Xie (2015) investigated the role of scientific knowledge in the above relationship and showed the effects of knowledge of both benefit and risk on attitudinal change related to GM foods, attributed to information provided. However, Frewer et al. (1998) claimed that the most important determinant of attitude changes toward genetic engineering after information provision was prior attitude. These findings suggest that an analysis capable of verifying changes within subjects would be necessary by taking differences in scientific knowledge into consideration.

Therefore, we focus first on the role of domain-specific scientific knowledge in the relationship between provided information and attitudinal change concerning the application to food of three technologies. As researchers in public communication of science stress the need of promoting reflections on scientific communities and stimulating dialogue between experts and the lay public (Kato-Nitta et al., 2017), answering this question should provide better mutual understanding. For this purpose, single-factor within-subject design of experimental survey was employed. By testing differences in responses to identical items by the same respondent for four conditions (before the information is provided and after information on three respective technologies is provided), other conditions, including the individual factors of respondents, are assumed to be controlled. Thus, this method matches our research goal.

Our first research question is as follows. RQ 1: How do attitudinal changes on different breeding technologies differ in three groups stratified by domain-specific knowledge? Our empirical study of statistically observing this issue would yield essential information on public perceptions of gene editing as new S&T.

Boundary conditions of deficit model framework

Our second research question aims to shed light on how the general scientific knowledge or science literacy of the lay public influences attitudinal change concerning acceptance of science owning to the information provided. The relationship between science literacy and peopleā€™s acceptance toward emerging S&T such as GM crops has been studied extensively in connection with the deficit model frameworkā€”in other words, an increase in ā€˜properā€™ scientific knowledge among the public linearly improves their acceptance of new S&T. These studies were mainly associated with criticism for its oversimplified assumption, and there is also empirical evidence rejecting this. For instance, Bucchi and Neresini (2002) proved that better-informed people do not always show more positive attitudes toward GM food. Midden et al. (2002) reported that scientific knowledge or information may promote negative perceptions of the lay public.

Some researchers explain that scientistsā€™ preconception of the public and its affinity with easily implemented policy may explain why the deficit modelā€™s assumption continues to occupy a prominent position in science communication (Simis et al., 2016; Suldovsky, 2016). Nonetheless, as Slovic (1999) claimed, even if it is not the most dominant variable, scientific knowledge remains closely and importantly linked with public attitudes toward S&T. Ahteensuu (2012) attends to relevance by stating that scientific knowledge does explain, at least partially, public negative attitudes of new S&T. He further stressed the need of distinguishing between cases in which deficit-model explanations are warranted and cases in which they are not.

This discussion demonstrates that further studies must be conducted on the relationship between public science literacy and acceptance of food-related applications of new S&T. To explore this issue, the current study examines attitudinal changes in various facets of peopleā€™s perceptions, particularly of changes in risk, benefit, and value perceptions. It can be considered that peopleā€™s acceptance is promoted if decreases in risk perception and increases in benefit perceptions or value perceptions were observed. By investigating those three aspects of peopleā€™s perceptions, we multidimensionally provide insight into peopleā€™s acceptance of emerging S&T. Furthermore, this study introduces to the above discussion the elaboration likelihood model (Petty and Cacioppo, 1986) to explore the relationship among science literacy, provided information, and attitude change. The modelā€™s foundational assumption is that attitude change is achieved with not only individual knowledge but also individual relevance to the information.

Therefore, we address RQ 2: Does science literacy influence the attitudinal changes that result from information provision about the differences in applied breeding technologies on agricultural crops? A confirmation of the empirical results for this question by taking into account individual relevance and different cases of applied technologies should contribute to the discussion on the relationship between scientific knowledge and the lay publicā€™s acceptance toward emerging S&T.

Methods

Data

Two web-based surveys were conducted from December 2016 to February 2017. The surveys were entrusted with a survey company, Nippon Information Incorporated. Survey 1 used a quasi-representative sample from the companyā€™s large opt-in panel of approximately 1,100,000 volunteers from the online population in Japan. An initial screening was made to mitigate the potential bias in demographic distributions, and allocation was made in proportion to the size according to region, gender, and age based on the 2015 Japan national population census, and drew 3350 respondents aged between 20 and 69 years on a first-come, first-serve-basis. Then, the final participants of 3000 were selected by excluding respondents with extreme-shorter response time (from the shorter side of time to be about less than 1/10 of median total response time). This treatment was made to enhance reliability of the data based on the idea of ā€˜satisficingā€™ in quantitative survey methodology (Krosnick, 1991; Maniaci and Rogge, 2014; Tourangeau et al., 2013). This idea is based on behaviour which respondents do not pay enough amount of cognitive effort to provide the suitable answers in a survey.

The participants for Survey 2 were scientists with and without expertise in molecular biology. We adopted an opportunistic sampling method in addition to recruiting volunteers using academic societiesā€™ websites, such as the Molecular Biology Society of Japan and the Physical Society of Japan. We also utilised electronic mailing lists for recruiting participants, including TENNET (operated by the Japan Astronomical Society) and Jeconet (a Japanese academic mailing list related to ecology). Survey 2 participantsā€™ fields of specialty were distributed as follows: Micro-biology, such as molecular biology, 56.3%; macro-biology, such as ecology, 13.7%; physical sciences, informatics, chemistry, or geology, 11.6%; social sciences and humanities, 6.1%; medicine, nursing, or health, 4.6%; other disciplines, 7.6%. The final sample size for the second survey was 197. Table 1 shows the survey demographics.

Table 1 Demographic distributions of survey 1 and survey 2

The data in this study were used in previous studies (Kato-Nitta et al., 2017; Tachikawa et al., 2017), and the surveys utilised were conducted under Japanese Privacy Information Protection Law. Participation was completely voluntary, and participants could withdraw at any time. Informed consent of all participants was obtained by Nippon Information Incorporated.

The surveys provided fundamental information, starting with the definition of genome and to textual explanation of the basic genome research, as well as health and agricultural applications of genome research. At this point, the initial attitudes, as a baseline of within-subject experimental design, were measured with 11 items on benefit, risk, and value perceptions toward genome research applied to agricultural crops. Details of the fundamental information provided are shown in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Provided fundamental Information on genome research. Fundamental textual information provision about definition of genome, basic genome research, and health and agricultural applications of genome research

Then, figures with texts (Figs. 2 and 3) were used when explaining the differences among the three existing breeding technologies of conventional breeding, genetic modification, and gene editing. The tomato is considered as a fine model plant by biologists (Busch et al., 1991), so we used figures depicting tomatoes to explain the technological differences. Next, three measurementsā€”one of perceptions of applying conventional breeding, one of perceptions of applying genetic modification, and one of perceptions of applying gene editingā€”were made, each using 11 items (33 total items) spanning the oldest to the newest technologies. In order to answer these items, participants were allowed to refer to the figures (Fig. 2) and access the provided URL links to the website of the Ministry of Agriculture, Forestry, and Fisheries during the survey.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Information on technological differences. Information provision by illustrations with text to explain the differences among the three existing breeding technologies of conventional breeding, genetic modification, and gene editing

Fig. 3
figure 3

Information on technological differences in text. Information provision in text to explain the differences among the three existing breeding technologies of conventional breeding, genetic modification, and gene editing. This information is provided with the information described in Fig. 2

Measures and analyses

Relationship between domain-specific knowledge and attitudinal change

To answer the first research question, we assessed various facets of perceptions on applying genome research to agricultural crops with the 11-item scale. Nine of the eleven items were adopted from the JSPS KAKENHI (17019024) research project ā€˜Public attitudes toward genomic researches in Japan (g-elsi)ā€™, and two extra items were added for the current study. Four measurements were made using this scale: before the explanation of technical differences (1st, baseline) and after presentation of technical differences among the three breeding technologies (2nd, 3rd, and 4th). The details of the 11 items are provided below, as well as in Fig. 4 and Table 4. The participants were asked to answer using a five-point scale (1ā€‰=ā€‰disagree to 5ā€‰=ā€‰agree).

Table 2 Cronbachā€™s alpha for aggregated variables for calculation on dependent variables

What do you think of a genome research application for breeding agricultural crops using the technology of (conventional breeding/genetic modification/gene editing)?

  1. 1.

    Beneficial to stable food supply

  2. 2.

    Beneficial to human health care

  3. 3.

    Beneficial to economic development

  4. 4.

    Impacts plant and insect ecology

  5. 5.

    Insufficient safety confirmation

  6. 6.

    Fear of unexpected adverse effects

  7. 7.

    Possibility of misusing this technology

  8. 8.

    Bioethically questionable

  9. 9.

    Cannot understand well and feel somewhat fearful

  10. 10.

    Universally favoured to be promoted

  11. 11.

    Research considered insignificant

On the above 11 items, we statistically explored the differences in mean values of the four conditions of (1) before information on technologies, (2) conventional breeding, (3) genetic modification, and finally, (4) gene editing, using a single-factor repeated measures ANOVA. As the result of the statistical test depends on the sample size, we further calculated effect sizes to determine the extent of attitudinal changes and also conducted power analyses. We categorised participants into three groups by level of domain-specific knowledge of molecular biology: (A) lay public (nā€‰=ā€‰3000), (B) experts in the other fields (nā€‰=ā€‰86), (C) experts in molecular biology (nā€‰=ā€‰111). We also observed differences in attitudinal changes of the three groups from the data including the above three groups.

Influence of science literacy on attitudinal change

To answer the second research question, we tested the following hypothesis with regression analyses: peopleā€™s scientific knowledge influences their attitudinal change on agricultural crops owing to information provided that explains the differences among applied technologies. For these analyses, we exclusively used the data from Survey 1 (the lay public).

Dependent variables

We constructed two composite variables using nine items out of the above 11 items: benefit perception comprised three items and risk perception comprised six items. A Cronbachā€™s alpha coefficient for each of these constructs was calculated to confirm the internal consistency and reliability for each item. These values are shown in Table 2.

There were two items related to value perception: ā€˜Universally favoured to be promotedā€™ and ā€˜Research considered insignificantā€™. Since the former is assessing individual perception of other individualsā€™ value perception and the latter is assessing individual perception per se, the two items were considered to be unsuitable for aggregation; therefore, we used only the latter item to be included in regression models for the purpose of analysis in assessing individual attitude change on value perceptions.

After the above operationalisation, we calculated the sizes of the respective changes (difference scores) described as follows (Y1 to Y9) and then used them as dependent variable in regression analysis.

[Benefit perceptions]

Y1ā€‰=ā€‰Gene editing minus before information

Y2ā€‰=ā€‰Genetic modification minus before information

Y3ā€‰=ā€‰Conventional breeding minus before information

[Risk perceptions]

Y4ā€‰=ā€‰Gene editing minus before information

Y5ā€‰=ā€‰Genetic modification minus before information

Y6ā€‰=ā€‰Conventional breeding minus before information

[Value perceptions]

Y7ā€‰=ā€‰Gene editing minus before information

Y8ā€‰=ā€‰Genetic modification minus before information

Y9ā€‰=ā€‰Conventional breeding minus before information

We decided to use the difference score (gain score) after weighing both its pros (Cronbach and Furby, 1970) and cons (Edwards, 1970) and its intuitive appeal on interpretability of results. Our dependent variables represented the perceived superiority or inferiority of each technology to the baseline of ā€˜before the information on technological differencesā€™. R-squares of regression equations are generally lower compared with the analysis not using such operation, as is characteristic when the major source of variations (baseline) is subtracted from the dependent variable. When the size of Y1 to Y3 and Y7 to Y9 are positive, the change can be interpreted as positive, and the benefit or value perception is higher compared with the perception before information provision. When the size of Y4 to Y6 is positive, the change can be interpreted as positive, and the risk perception is higher compared with the perception before information provision.

Independent variables

Science literacy: We measured participantsā€™ general scientific knowledge with 11 items that have been repeatedly used in international comparative studies (European Union, 2001; National Science Board, 2016), as well as by the Japanese government (Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology, 2004). The scale consists of items such as ā€˜Antibiotics kill viruses, as well as bacteria: True or falseā€™. We calculated the total sum of the correct answers to the 11 questions. The current studyā€™s participants (the lay public) in Survey 1 recorded a correct answer rate of 53.6%, which was about a same correct answer rate of 54% reported in the previous survey conducted by the Japanese government (Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology, 2004).

Individual relevance to the information (benefit of general genome research, risk of general genome research, and value of general genome research): We measured participantsā€™ individual relevance to the information by measuring participantsā€™ benefit, risk, and value perceptions on general genome research. As this research focuses on peopleā€™s perceptions of genome research application on agricultural crops, we defined perceptions on general genome research as ā€˜perceptions on basic genome science and genome research as applied to medicineā€™. We adopted benefit of general genome research as control variables for models Y1 to Y3, risk of general genome research for Y4 to Y6, and value of general genome research for Y7 to Y9. Items for construct variables of individual relevance were measured before provision of information on technological differences (Figs. 2, 3) and the respective Cronbachā€™s alpha values for each of the aggregated independent variables are provided in Table 3. As is shown in Table 3, Cronbachā€™s alphas for the aggregated independent variables yielded adequate internal consistency and reliability.

Table 3 Cronbachā€™s alpha for aggregated independent variables

Trust in food governance: Previous studies noted that trust has been considered the essential variable to understanding peopleā€™s risk perceptions (Lobb, 2005; Slovic, 1999; Slovic et al., 1981). Recent empirical studies have also shown that trust was an important factor for both risk and benefit perceptions on the application of biotechnology to agricultural crops (RodrĆ­guez-Entrena, M. and Salazar-OrdĆ³Ć±ez, 2013; Slovic, 1999). We introduced this concept as controls to test the hypothesis associated with the second research question. Trust in Japanese food governance was assessed with items used in previous Japanese studies (Kato-Nitta et al., 2017). This scale consists of four items evaluating participantsā€™ trust in governmental food safety policy, as well as safety measures of food business companies. The participants were asked to answer using a seven-point scale (1ā€‰=ā€‰completely disagree to 7ā€‰=ā€‰completely agree). The Cronbachā€™s alpha value for this scale is shown in Table 3.

Risk-avoidance orientation: We utilised this concept for controlling the level of original disposition of the participants with respect to risk. The part of this scale was used in the previous studies (and two extra items were added for the purpose of the current study (Kato-Nitta et al., 2017). The scale consists of six items and was developed to assess if the participants have a ā€˜zero-riskā€™ orientation or whether they favourably evaluate products described as ā€˜additive-freeā€™ or ā€˜pesticide-freeā€™. The answers to this scale was made with a five-point scale (1ā€‰=ā€‰Do not praise at all to 5ā€‰=ā€‰Praise highly). The Cronbachā€™s alpha value for this scale is shown in Table 3.

Results

Evaluation of mean differences of four conditions

Figure 4 shows the mean values of the four measurements from left to right (1ā€‰=ā€‰before the information on technological differences is provided; after the information on technological differences is provided: 2ā€‰=ā€‰conventional breeding, 3ā€‰=ā€‰genetic modification, and 4ā€‰=ā€‰gene editing) of the three groups, categorised with domain-specific knowledge of molecular biology. Table 4 shows the result of single-factor repeated measures ANOVA in the mean value of the responses in each group.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Group mean differences of four conditions. Line charts of each item represent the mean values of the four measurements assessing benefit, risk, and value perceptions (1ā€‰=ā€‰before the information on technological differences is provided; after the information on technological differences is provided: 2ā€‰=ā€‰conventional breeding, 3ā€‰=ā€‰genetic modification, and 4ā€‰=ā€‰gene editing) of the three groups, categorised with domain-specific knowledge of molecular biology. Red coloured linesā€‰=ā€‰Lay public, Green coloured linesā€‰=ā€‰Experts in other fields, Blue coloured linesā€‰=ā€‰Experts in molecular biology. Note: Given that the variable on the x axis is not strictly continuous, bar charts are more appropriate. However, we used the line charts for readability

Table 4 Attitudinal change caused by information on differences among three breeding technologies

Considering the overall positions of the three curves shown in Fig. 4, the following trends were observed. Experts in molecular biology showed the highest benefit perceptions, the lay public showed the lowest, and experts in other fields were in the middle. For risk perceptions, experts in molecular biology were the lowest, the lay public was the highest, and experts in other fields were in the middle. By reading the comprehensive shapes of the three curves, it can be observed that for both benefit and risk perceptions, there is a similarity among the lay public and experts in other fields. In contrast, for the two items of value perceptions, experts in other fields showed a similar trend with that of experts in molecular biology. Thus, the domain-specific knowledge of genome literacy affects the relationship between the provision of information and attitudinal change concerning food application of three technologies.

Group A (the lay public)

The red curve in Fig. 4 and statistical tests in Table 4 revealed that the lay publicā€™s perception of food-related application changed after the information was provided. As for benefit perceptions, all statistical tests were significant. The results of multiple comparisons with Bonferroni correction shown in Table 4 also suggest that people consider benefit aspects of gene editing higher than those of genetic modification out of all three items assessing benefit perceptions. As for the two items of ā€˜beneficial to stable food supplyā€™ and ā€˜beneficial to economyā€™, the values of effect size f were smaller than 0.10, as shown in Table 4. According to the criteria proposed by Cohen (1988), the interpretations for effect size f include the following: fā€‰ā‰„ā€‰0.10 as ā€œsmallā€, fā€‰ā‰„ā€‰0.25 as ā€œmediumā€, and fā€‰ā‰„ā€‰0.40 as ā€œlargeā€. Therefore, although the results of statistical tests were significant for those two items, the degree of changes was small.

All statistical tests were significant for risk and value items, and the values of effect size f were larger than 0.10. The overall tendency of the red curves indicates that the lay public considers the risks of conventional breeding as the lowest and those of genetic modification as the highest. The results of multiple comparisons with Bonferroni correction, suggest that the risk perception for gene editing was lower than that for genetic modification; however, such difference was relatively small compared with the difference between conventional breeding and genetic modification.

Group B (experts in other fields)

The green coloured curve in Fig. 4 and statistical tests in Table 4 revealed that Group Bā€™s attitudes changed after the information was provided, except for one item of ā€˜beneficial to food supplyā€™. The results suggest that this group consider the benefit aspects of gene editing as similar to those of genetic modification out of all three items assessing benefit perceptions. This finding is based on the lack of statistical difference found between the two technologies. Because the mean of 4 (gene editing) is higher than that of 3 (genetic modification), and there is a statistical difference between 3 and 4 in Group B for the item ā€˜possibility of misusing this technologyā€™, risk perception of gene editing was marked higher than that of genetic modification. The value of effect size f for this item was 0.823, which was much larger than Cohenā€™s criterion: fā€‰ā‰„ā€‰0.40 as ā€œlargeā€.

Group C (experts in molecular biology)

The blue coloured curve in Fig. 4 and statistical tests in Table 4 revealed that Group Cā€™s perceived benefits did not change after the provision of information. The results of statistical tests in Table 4 suggest that risk perceptions of the experts in molecular biology changed before and after the information technological differences was provided, except for one item, ā€˜cannot understand well and feel somewhat fearfulā€™.

In this group, there were no statistically significant differences among the three technologies for the risk perception items, including ā€˜impacts plant and insect ecologyā€™, ā€˜insufficient safety confirmationā€™, and ā€˜fear of unexpected adverse effectsā€™. The shapes of the curves of these items are distinct from those of the other two groups. Given the statistical differences between 2 (conventional breeding) and 3 (genetic modification) and between 2 and 4 (gene editing) for the items of ā€˜possibility of misusing this technologyā€™, and ā€˜bio-ethically questionableā€™, the risk aspects of those items were considered as being ā€˜differently naturedā€™ in this group from those of ā€˜insufficient safety confirmationā€™ or ā€˜fear of unexpected adverse effectsā€™. The values of effect size f for those items were 0.551 and 0.398, which were higher than the Cohenā€™s criteria: fā€‰ā‰„ā€‰0.40 as ā€œlargeā€; fā€‰ā‰„ā€‰0.25 as ā€œmediumā€, respectively.

As the large sample size of lay public participants might have influenced the result, we further conducted power analyses. For example, concerning the item with the smallest effect size f of 0.045 in Table 4 (beneficial to stable food supply), a sample size capable of obtaining the Cohenā€™s proposed convention of power value of 0.8 (Ī±ā€‰=ā€‰0.05) was 271. Therefore, we would have obtained the same results even if the sample size of the lay public had been less than 1/10 of the current study. This result enabled us to conclude that the results of the statistical tests were not dependent on the sample size.

Evaluation of effects of science literacy on attitudinal change

To test the hypothesis based on RQ 2 that science literacy influences attitudinal change on agricultural crops caused by the provision of information, regression analyses were used. Table 5 shows the results of benefit perceptions, Table 6 shows those of risk perceptions, and Table 7 shows those of value perceptions.

Table 5 Influence of science literacy on attitudinal changes of benefit perceptions
Table 6 Influence of science literacy on attitudinal changes of risk perceptions
Table 7 Influence of science literacy on attitudinal changes of value perceptions

The results showed that the hypothesis above was confirmed for benefit perceptions of all three breeding technologies. It was also confirmed for risk perceptions and value perceptions of conventional breeding. However, it was not confirmed for risk perceptions and value perceptions of the two new technologies of genetic modification and gene editing. The details of regression analyses are as follows.

Benefit perceptions

All three models (Table 5) were statistically significant. Looking at the effects of science literacy, this variable has a positive coefficient and is statistically significant for all the three models of benefit perceptions. Thus, the results for the benefit perceptions supported the hypothesis by showing the positive effects of scientific knowledge in increasing benefit perceptions after the information was provided as for all three breeding technologies.

Risk perceptions

All three models (Table 6), were statistically significant. As to the effects of science literacy, this variable has a negative coefficient and is statistically significant for Model Y6. In contrast, this variable is not statistically significant for the models of new S&T (Models Y4 and Y5). Thus, the result for Model Y6 supported the hypothesis, but those for Models Y4 and Y5 did not. Therefore, the positive effects of scientific knowledge to reduce risk perceptions, after the information is provided, was observed only for the conventional breeding technique. There were no effects of scientific knowledge to reduce risk perceptions of the two new S&T of gene editing and genetic modification.

Value perceptions

Model Y9 was statistically significant whereas Models Y7 and Y8 were not statistically significant. As to the effects of science literacy, this variable has a positive coefficient and is statistically significant for Model Y9. Thus, the result for Model Y9 supported the hypothesis, but those for Models Y7 and Y8 did not. Therefore, the positive effects of scientific knowledge to increase value perceptions, after the information is provided, was observed only for the conventional breeding technique. There were no effects of scientific knowledge to increase value perceptions of the two new S&T of gene editing and genetic modification. Overall trends of the models of value perceptions were similar to those of risk perceptions.

Discussion

We investigated both expert and lay perceptions on food application of new S&T by statistically examining whether attitude changes toward applied technologies were observed in relation to peopleā€™s scientific knowledge. The approach of the current study, which empirically elucidated the perceptions of three groups (experts in molecular biology, experts in other fields, and the lay public) on gene editing compared with genetic modification and conventional breeding techniques, provides essential evidence for stakeholders concerning new breeding technologies.

Results of the current study demonstrated that domain-specific scientific knowledge affects peopleā€™s risk, benefit, or value perceptions, according to the stratification of participants into three groups of experts in molecular biology, experts in other fields, and the lay public. The experts in molecular biology showed the highest benefit and lowest risk perceptions compared to the other two groups. As stated by Sandin and Moula (2015), those who believe in the potential of new agricultural technologies tend to refer to benefit aspects, while those who criticise tend to raise concerns of risk aspects.

The lay public tended to consider gene editing as closer to genetic modification by clearly differentiating the conventional breeding technique from the two newer techniques. They showed a dramatic increase in benefit perceptions and a significant reduction in risk perceptions after being provided information of conventional breeding (Fig. 4 and Table 4). Thus, the lay public appraised this technique with respect to outcomes as the one with the lowest risk and the highest benefit. However, the results of the single-factor within-subject ANOVA among the lay public also suggested that their perceptions of gene editing slightly improved compared with genetic modification. As such, the lay public somewhat identified the technical differences between gene editing and genetic modification, perhaps through process-based thinking.

The current studyā€™s approach of differentiating experts into two groups according to domain-specific scientific knowledge empirically showed that the perceptions of a specific science or technology will be different if scientistsā€™ expertise is different. Similar to those of the lay public, experts in other fields differentiated the benefit outcomes of the three technologies, probably based on process-based thinking. Contrary to the other two groups, experts in molecular biology did not differentiate the benefit outcomes of the three technologies. This finding is noteworthy because it empirically supported our hypothesis, suggesting that experts in molecular biology assessed the benefit aspects of gene editing through product-based thinking, not through process-based thinking.

Our further investigation revealed that the levels of perceptions of experts in other fields fall between those of experts in molecular biology and of the lay public in many items. That said, it is important to note that there were some characteristics that the two groups of experts generally shared and some aspects they did not share. As to the value perception concerning the application and promotion of scientific research to food, experts in other fields showed similar responses to experts in molecular biology to all three breeding technologies. Nevertheless, they responded similarly to the lay public concerning most of the items assessing risk perceptions. In fact, they displayed higher risk perception toward gene editing than toward genetic modification for the item ā€˜possibility of misusing this technologyā€™, while the lay public showed higher risk perception toward genetic modification than toward gene editing for this item. As there was no statistical difference between the two technologies in the group of experts in molecular biology for this item, the relationship between domain-specific scientific knowledge and risk perceptions on new S&T remains complex; perhaps there is no linear situation in their association.

The regression analyses of Tables 5ā€“7 revealed that the influence of science literacy on the lay publicā€™s attitudinal change toward agricultural crops is confirmed all in benefit, value, and risk perceptions for the conventional breeding technique. For this technique, the effects after information provision were inverse: science literacy increased the benefit and value perceptions and reduced the risk perception. This hypothesis based on RQ2 was also confirmed in the benefit perceptions for the two new S&T of gene editing and genetic modification. Notably, however, such assumption was rejected by the analyses of risk perceptions and value perceptions for the two new S&T.

In interpreting the contradictory results presented above, one possible interpretation is that the assumption of the deficit model was valid only for conventional S&T, knowledge on which can be acquired through classroom education, but not valid for emerging S&T, knowledge on which may be acquired mainly through informal learning. An alternative interpretation is that the modelā€™s assumption on new S&T is valid only for increasing benefit perceptions but not for reducing risk perceptions or increasing value perceptions. We propose that the two interpretations above together constitute a new hypothesis about the relationship between scientific knowledge and public perceptions toward emerging S&T. In proposing the above hypotheses, we empirically demonstrated the deficit modelā€™s boundary conditions, and offered logic to explain previous empirical studiesā€™ inconsistency on deficit model type of explanations.

Limitations and future suggestions

Further empirical investigation should be conducted to confirm our new hypotheses about the deficit model. The assumption of the model was valid only for conventional S&T but not for emerging S&T; moreover, the modelā€™s assumption on new S&T is valid only for increasing benefit perceptions but not for reducing risk perceptions or increasing value perceptions, including all the possible other interpretations.

Because it was almost impossible for the current study to exclusively extract experts in molecular biology without a conflict of interest in this field, our participants who were experts in molecular biology may face this conflict, as their major earnings might be from gene editing or genetic modification. As a previous study investigating peopleā€™s attitudes toward climate change risks (Kahan et al., 2012) proposed that attitude stemmed more from conflict of interest than from scientific knowledge, future studies may be needed methodologically to exclude experts in molecular biology with conflicts of interest in this field.

The statistical analyses were focused on attitudinal changes before and after the provision of information on the technological differences among gene editing, genetic modification, and conventional breeding. Our interest was in the factors of technological differences among the three technologies; therefore, we could not cover the contextual-specific factors related to gene editing. This point was raised by Bauer, Heinz (2002): context-specific factors, such as knowledge gaps and information gaps related to specific S&T in different countries or in different cultures, would affect study results. Further examinations should be conducted in different national and cultural contexts by methodologically separating these two factors.

The analyses were conducted using datasets obtained through online surveys. This method was suitable for the current studyā€™s approach of within-subject experimental design, but further studies should be conducted including utilising different modes of measurement. Replications should also be made with different items to assess peopleā€™s benefit, risk, and value perceptions because number format and framing conditions of items may influence the obtained results (Peters et al., 2011). In addition, as we did not have a control group, we could not completely control the order effect or the effect from information provision. This is a limitation of the current study, and the accumulation of data for reproducibility shall ensure that the results can stand as foundation for future related work.

Conclusion

In this study, the lay public attitudes toward gene edited crops tended to be higher both in benefit and value perceptions and lower in risk perceptions than attitudes toward GM crops. Obtaining such results even in Japan, with its relatively lower acceptance and strict regulations on GM food, may further boost the potential of this emerging technology. We must note, however, the differences between gene editing and genetic modification were exceedingly small, compared to the differences between conventional breeding and genetic modification. Further, the effect of the lay publicā€™s science literacy on perceptions toward new S&T of gene editing or genetic modification after the information provision on applied technological differences was observed only for increased benefit perceptions and not for increased value perceptions or decreased risk perceptions.

We empirically demonstrated the deficit modelā€™s boundary conditions and offered logic to explain previous empirical studiesā€™ inconsistency in deficit model-type explanations. We demonstrated that (1) the modelā€™s assumption is valid for conventional science and technology (S&T) but not for emerging S&T, and (2) the modelā€™s assumption regarding emerging S&T was valid for increasing benefit perceptions but not for increasing value perceptions or reducing risk perceptions. These empirical results emphasise the uncertainty of peopleā€™s attitude toward emerging breeding technologies. The ability of scientists, as well as policy makers to face such a complex situation is put to the test.