Abstract
Motion-onset visual evoked potentials (MO VEPs) are robust to dioptric blur when low contrast and low spatial frequency patterns are used for stimulation. To reveal mechanisms of MO VEPs robustness, we studied whether the resistance to defocus persists even when using a high-contrast checkerboard using digital defocus in the emmetropic eyes of 13 subjects (males 20–60 years). We compared the dominant components of MO VEPs to pattern-reversal VEPs (PR VEP), which are sensitive to the blur. For stimulation, we used checkerboard patterns with 15´ and 60´ checks. To defocus the checkerboard, we rendered it with a second-order Zernike polynomial (\({Z}_{2}^{0}\)) with an equivalent defocus of 0, 2, or 4 D. For PR VEP, the checkerboards were reversed in terms of their contrast. To evoke MO VEP, the checkerboard of 60´ checks moved for 200 ms with a speed of 5 or 10 deg/s in the cardinal directions. The MO VEP did not change in peak time (P ≥ 0.0747) or interpeak amplitude (P > 0.0772) with digital blur. In contrast, for PR VEP, the results showed a decrease in interpeak amplitude (P ≤ 6.65ˑ10-4) and an increase in peak time (P ≤ 0.0385). Thus, we demonstrated that MO VEPs evoked by checkerboard, structure containing high spatial content, can be robust to defocus.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
Visual evoked potentials (VEPs) allow testing of the integrity of the visual system objectively, non-invasively and with minimal material cost and thus they contribute to the eventual diagnosis of retrobulbar pathological processes. The basic diagnostic parameters of VEP are influenced by the luminance, and spatial and temporal properties of the stimulus. For optimal inter-laboratory interpretability and high diagnostic yield, it is a good idea to follow ISCEV standards1. When a stimulus is projected onto the retina, light passes through the optical media of the eye, so the image may be modified even at this early level and cause erroneous conclusions about retrobulbar pathology. For this reason, ISCEV standards recommend optimally correcting the refractive error of the eye for the stimulus distance and recording the refractive error in the examination report.
In some patients, correcting the eye's refractive error is difficult. For this reason, knowing how the VEP parameters change in response to refractive error, tested using artificially blurred stimuli in healthy subjects, can be helpful. Such knowledge might allow differentiation between normal or pathological states of the visual system in a patient with an uncorrected refractive error2,3,4.
Many studies have looked specifically at the effect of artificially blurred pattern stimuli on the fundamental characteristics of VEP (VEP specifically induced by checkerboard reversal—PR VEP). In these studies, a high degree of dependence of key VEP parameters on the spatial frequency of the stimuli has been demonstrated. Specifically, it has been shown that with increased blurring of the images (due to refractive errors), there is a significant increase in the peak time of the P100 component and a decrease in its amplitude5,6,7. In their early work, White and Harter (1968) demonstrated that blur systematically alters VEPs and patented VEP-based method to assess visual refractive error8 (for review see9).
The low specificity of the PR VEP limits the ability to separate the decrease in visual acuity caused by refractive error from retrobulbar impairment. For these reasons, VEP variants that have low sensitivity for refractive errors would be useful in patients with reduced visual acuity. Examples of such VEP examinations are motion-onset VEPs (MO VEPs), which can also be elicited in the periphery of the visual field10,11,12 by stimulating patterns with low spatial frequencies. The dependence of the characteristics of MO VEPs on artificially (dioptrically) induced refractive error was investigated by Kordek et al. 20227. It was observed that dioptric blur significantly prolongs peak times and reduces interpeak amplitudes of PR VEPs, but not of MO VEPs. The experiment used clinically employed patterns that were optimized for activation of different visual channels. While a high-contrast checkerboard pattern with a significant representation of higher spatial frequencies was used for reversal stimulation, motion stimulation was elicited by a pattern with low content of higher spatial frequencies and low luminance contrast. The dissimilarity of the two patterns could also be a factor affecting the sensitivity of VEP to blur. For this reason, we performed another experiment in which we kept the structure identical for reversal and motion stimulation. At the same time, we used a digital blur, which allows for precise and consistent control of the blur parameters homogeneously throughout the stimulation area. It is also less prone to potential confounding factors related to lens-induced magnification or minification effects, vertex distance variation, and an imperfect optical axis alignment compared to dioptric blur13,14.
The aim of our study was to describe and compare the behaviour of the basic parameters of the PR VEP and MO VEP as a function of digitally induced second-order aberration (“defocus”) in the emmetropic eye and to answer the question of whether the robustness of the motion stimulus to blur persists when using a high-contrast checkerboard.
Methods
Subjects
A total of 13 men between the ages of 20 and 60 years (mean age 39 years) were examined. The subjects reported no neurological or ophthalmological problems at the time of examination. All subjects signed an informed consent and GDPR consent before the examination. All procedures performed in our study were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. The study was approved by the ethics committee of the University Hospital in Hradec Kralove (no. 201411S19P).
Stimulation
VEP examination was performed in the neurophysiology laboratory of the Department of Pathological Physiology, Faculty of Medicine in Hradec Kralove, on a CRT monitor (Vision Master Pro 510, Iiyama, Japan) with a resolution of 1024 × 768 pixels and an angular size of 37° × 28°, from a viewing distance of 60 cm. The visual stimuli were generated using PsychoPy v2022.2.5 (Open Science Tools Ltd., UK). For the examination of PR VEP and MO VEP, checkerboards of 15´ and 60´ square angular size were used as patterns according to ISCEV standards1. Each stimulation included the introductory text “Get ready, the stimulation is about to begin”.
During the PR VEP (simple checkerboard reversal) examination, the Michelson contrast between the white and black squares was 96%. The mean luminance was 17 cd/m2, constant throughout the experiment. The checkerboard was displayed in the full monitor area and reversed twice within one second. A red fixation mark was displayed in the centre of the monitor throughout. Each PR VEP examination (PR 15´, PR 60´) lasted approximately 25 s and consisted of 50 stimuli.
The MO VEP examination in this study was performed with the checkerboard of 60´ square angular size, with two temporal frequency values of 2 Hz (MO TF2) and 4 Hz (MO TF4). We choose to move the larger checkerboard only to keep stimulus parameters optimal for MO VEPs (size of the structure between 0.2 and 1.0 c/deg, and speed from 5 to 25 deg/s)10. For the checkerboard of 15´ square angular size, we would need a slow motion of 1 deg/s to keep the same temporal frequency (2 Hz) as for the larger checks. For such speed, it is necessary to move the pattern by 0.5 pixel/frame, which interrupts the smoothness of the motion in our setup.
Michelson contrast between white and black squares was 96%. Mean luminance was 17 cd/m2, constant throughout the experiment. A red fixation mark was displayed in the centre of the monitor throughout. The motion and reversal pattern schemes are shown in Fig. 1.
VEPs were recorded using unipolar electrodes (OZ, PZ, CZ, FZ and OL, OR—5 cm to the left and right of OZ). The reference electrode was placed in position A2, the ground electrode was on the left wrist. EEG recording was performed on a TruTrace device (Deymed Diagnostic s.r.o., Czech Republic) with a sampling frequency of 6250 Hz and a frequency bandwidth of 1—1000 Hz. The signal was then digitally filtered in the band 1—40 Hz using the pop_eegfiltnew function15. For display, the signal was smoothed with a Savitzky-Golay filter with a window width of 50 ms and 1st order interpolation (sgolayfilt.m, Matlab rel. 2023a, Mathworks, Inc., USA).
Digital blur
In our study, the simulation of second-order aberration (“defocus”—\({Z}_{2}^{0}\)) was induced as described in a study by Kordek et al. 202116. Thus, the resulting defocused checkerboards (of both angular sizes) were produced by computer convolution of the defocused checkerboards and the point spread function (PSF) described in14,17,18, which for the case of \({Z}_{2}^{0}\) aberration is given by Eq. (1)16,
The PSF is the squared modulus of the Fourier transform of the complex aperture function. The function \(p\left(r,\theta \right)\) represents the aperture’s amplitude function and has in our case is circular with a value of 1 inside the pupil and a value of 0 outside the pupil. The expression \(\sqrt{3}\cdot \left(2\cdot {\rho }^{2}-1\right)\) corresponds to the second order Zernike polynomial \({Z}_{2}^{0}\)19,where ρ is the radial distance in the pupil. This is scaled by \({\raise0.7ex\hbox{${2\pi }$} \!\mathord{\left/ {\vphantom {{2\pi } \lambda }}\right.\kern-0pt} \!\lower0.7ex\hbox{$\lambda $}}^{\prime}\), where λ is the wavelength of light (570 nm), and by the RMS Zernike coefficient to give the phase of the wavefront. The RMS Zernike coefficient is an optical metric that determines the degree of blurring of the checkerboard, defined as the root-mean-square of the wavefront 17. In our case, the RMS takes values of 0, 1.8 and 3.6 µm, which corresponds to equivalent defocus values, Me, of 0, 2 and 4 D in turn, based on the Eq. (2)20
where R is the pupil radius. For the correct construction of the transformed image, the pixel scale of the PSF (sPSF) must be equal to the pixel scale of the source image (sobj), i.e. sPSF = sobj. We started from the Fraunhofer diffraction theory for a rectangular aperture, where18 sPSF = λ / D∙α.
where D is the aperture diameter of the aperture and α is the oversampling factor18. The pixel size of the source image is given by sobj = vobj / Nobj, where Nobj is the number of pixels in the field of view of the source image and vobj is the field of view of the source image. Comparing the two scales, we obtain the following equality ”Eq. (3)”18,
Based on this theory, custom code for blurring the checkerboard was created, adapted from Young & Smithson (2014)18, in the Python programming language using the NumPy library21. The parameters of the source code were designed for a viewing distance of 60 cm and an oversampling factor of 2 was used. The resulting blurred checkerboards were generated at a resolution of 1024 × 768 pixels. To allow a motion animation, the checkerboards were extended to fill a canvas size of 2017 × 2015 pixels.
The checkerboards for each examination were digitally blurred with equivalent defocus values of 0, 2 and 4 D (denoted “Blur 0”, “Blur 2” and “Blur 4”, respectively). The transfer functions for each blur value and the frequency spectra of the checkerboards are shown in Fig. 2, where only symbolic representations of the patterns before blurring are shown. Patterns at the stimulus screen resolution for all blur levels are available in the supplementary material “digital_blur_stimuli.zip”.
VEP recording
First, we measured the refractive state of both eyes of the subject using a Nidek Ark1a auto refractometer and the better eye was selected for the following examination. In the case of equal refractive status of both eyes, we selected the so-called dominant eye using the Hole-in-Card Test (Dolman's method)22,23. Then, in the first step, visual acuity was measured monocularly using Landolt C optotypes24 (viewing distance 4 m), and in the second step, contrast sensitivity was examined using CSV-1000 E25,26 (viewing distance 2.5 m, total luminance 85 cd/m2, VectorVision, Guardion Health Sciences Inc., USA).
The VEP examination itself was also performed monocularly. For each subject, we performed the four aforementioned VEP stimulations (PR 15´, PR 60´, MO TF2, MO TF4). For each subject, the examination was performed twice, giving two repeated measurements for each equivalent defocus value and for each VEP stimulation. For each stimulation and blur, the resulting record was constructed as an average of both repetitions. Thus, 12 VEP records were obtained for each subject. For each person, we randomised the order of the stimulations and the blurring levels to eliminate the effect of adaptation and fatigue27,28. For all subjects, to remove additional effects from their refractive error we always added a visual correction measured with an auto refractometer using the dioptric test set Art. 51-BL, M.S.D. in front of the eye under examination, analogous to the methodology used in our previous study16.
Statistical analysis
For the PR VEPs, the peak time and amplitude values of N75, N145, and P100 components were determined from the OZ-FZ lead. For the MO VEPs, the peak time and amplitude values for P1, N2, and P2 were read from the PZ-A2 lead. Data evaluation was performed by authors DK and JK.
Peak times P100 and N2 and interpeak amplitudes PR-Am (A_P100 – (A_N75 + A_N145)/2) and, MO-Am ((A_P1 + A_P2)/2 – A_N2) were used for subsequent analysis, a methodology analogous to our previous study7.
The relationship between the aforementioned parameters and the blur (i.e., the equivalent defocus value), was described for each subject and VEP stimulus using a linear regression model. This resulted in regression line slopes for all subjects for the 4 stimulations and two parameters (peak time and interpeak amplitude). The normality of each set was tested by the Shapiro–Wilk test. We tested whether the slopes differed from zero by Student's t-test or Wilcoxon test (when normality was not met). Data processing was performed with the Jamovi program (The jamovi project (2023). Jamovi (Version 2.3) [Computer Software]. Retrieved from https://www.jamovi.org). Use of a different regression model, such as a logarithmic regression model, might generally not yield statistically significantly different results. Evidence for this claim is, in our study, the very small difference in the coefficient of determination for the two regression models. In this case, it is customary to choose the simpler, i.e., linear model. This model was also used by Bobak et al. (1987)29.
Results
The measured visual acuity of the subjects ranged from 0.8 to 1.6 (nine out of thirteen subjects have a VA value of 1) in decimal expression. Each subject was assigned their necessary dioptric correction in front of the eye and these ranged from − 0.5 D to 0.5 D (median = 0.25 D, Q1 = − 0.25 D, Q3 = 0.25 D).
Basic descriptive statistics of peak times and interpeak amplitudes for VEP stimulations and blur conditions are shown in Table 1 and Fig. 3. The VEP grand averages are shown in Fig. 4. Source values from all subjects used to calculate the values in Table 1 are available in the supplementary material (Table S1 and Table S2).
The basic descriptive characteristics of the linear trends and the error probability of rejecting the true null hypothesis in the regression analysis are summarized in Table 2 and Fig. 5.
This analysis shows that in the case of the PR VEP, there is a significant (p ≤ 6.65ˑ10–4) decrease in the PR-Am interpeak amplitude and a significant increase (p ≤ 0.0385) in the P100 peak time with increasing digital blur. For the PR 15´ (PR 60´) examination, the interpeak amplitude decreased by − 1.2 (− 0.5) µV and the dominant peak lengthened by 6 (1) ms with each dioptre of equivalent defocus of digital blur.
In the case of MO VEP, there was no significant (p ≥ 0.36, p ≥ 0.077) change in MO-Am interpeak amplitude or N2 peak time (p ≥ 0.075, p ≥ 0.33) with increasing digital blur in either the MO TF2 or MO TF4 condition.
The results of the regression analysis for all subjects, stimuli and parameters are presented in the supplementary material (Tables S3 and S4).
Discussion
As early as the 1960s, image blur was recognized as an essential factor influencing VEPs. Responses to blurred patterns have been investigated, for example, with flashes illuminating a cardboard structure30, the presentation of slides31, or viewing on television or computer screens. These experiments were the basis for the objective selection of refractive correction13,32,33 and are now followed by an objective assessment of visual acuity34. Investigating the effect of defocus on VEPs is also an approach that can elucidate the genesis of VEPs in both normal and pathological situations and, because of the objective nature of the results, can be a helpful tool for verifying and calibrating models of visual perception.
In our study, we examined VEPs in response to two partially independent modalities of stimulation: pattern-reversal and motion onset. With the same spatial characteristics of the pattern used, the stimulations differed in the speed of the change required to elicit a VEP. For the local instantaneous contrast change in a pattern-reversal stimulus, as the digital defocus value increased, the interpeak amplitude of PR-Am decreased, and the peak time of P100 was prolonged. However, under motion onset stimulation where local contrast changed more slowly, the interpeak amplitude MO-Am and the peak time of N2 remained without statistically significant changes.
Similar published experiments for PR VEPs showed that changes in both parameters are more marked at the blur of a small structure in agreement with our results. However, they differ in the actual degree of amplitude decrease and peak time prolongation7,35,36,37,38. The variability in the results of these studies can be attributed to factors not controlled between studies, in particular, the method of image blurring, the magnitude of brightness and contrast of the stimulating structure used, or the population of subjects examined.
Only one study has been published for MO VEP, which is from our laboratory7. The current results agree with our prior findings, which is an important finding because the two studies have a significant methodological difference. In the first study, the stimulus pattern for motion-onset was a set of low-contrast (Michelson contrast 10%) concentric circles with reduced higher spatial frequencies (less than 1 c/deg). During the review process, a question arose as to whether a high-contrast structure, particularly one that is used for pattern-reversal VEP, could also be used for vision with refractive errors testing similar way as the low-contrast low spatial frequency structure. The presence of high spatial frequencies or high contrast may interfere with the resistance of the MO VEP to dioptric blur7. In the current study, we used a high-contrast checkerboard pattern (Michelson contrast 96%) with spatial frequencies exceeding 10 c/deg. However, the MO VEPs continue to show resistance to blur (despite the PR VEP defiance). We can conclude that neither the high contrast nor high spatial content of the stimulus pattern is critical in the resistance of the MO VEP to blur.
Considering the properties of the pattern-reversal and motion onset stimuli, we may conclude that the remaining and responsible difference for the blur resistance is the more gradual stimulus change (200 ms of relatively slow motion). The temporal tuning of the stimuli results in different neural processing of the motion onset and pattern-reversal39,40. While the dominant component of MO VEP peaks above the parietal area at about 160 ms (N2 peak), the P100 of the PR VEP culminates above the occipital cortex at about 100 ms. We showed that these neural processes exhibit different resistance to the blur.
Our results correspond to an expected effect of the second order aberration (“defocus” with a low pass transfer function; see Fig. 2) on the checkerboard. The low spatial frequencies perceived even after blurring are sufficient to evoke MO VEP but not PR VEP responses. Considering the properties of the pattern-reversal and motion onset stimuli, we may infer that the remaining and responsible difference for the blur resistance is the temporal change of the pattern. For MO VEP, the pattern changes position gradually for about 200 ms, allowing detection even of low contrast and low spatial frequency patterns. For PR VEP, the fast stimulus change (less than 14 ms – e.g. a video frame), makes the detection of degraded stimulus more difficult.
Limitations
Our conclusion regarding the robustness of MO VEPs to defocus is based on the non-significant effects of blur observed in MO VEPs, in contrast to the significant effects seen in PR VEPs. It is important to consider that the lack of significant blur effects in MO VEPs may be due to higher noise levels or greater variability in MO VEP measurements compared to PR VEPs. This inherent noise could obscure the potential effects of blur on MO VEPs, leading to a non-significant result. From the variability listed in Table 1 and Fig. 3, such a possibility appears unlikely.
It should be noted that the different temporal properties of the stimuli result in different neural processing of the motion onset and pattern reversal stimuli39,40. While the dominant component of MO VEP peaks above the parietal area at about 160 ms (N2 peak), the P100 of the PR VEP culminates above the occipital cortex at about 100 ms. We showed that these neural processes exhibit different resistance to the blur. Our study only investigated specific velocities and spatial frequencies. Generalizability to other contexts might require further studies.
Conclusion
We have shown that digital defocus of checkerboard stimuli up to an equivalent defocus value of 4 D does not cause a significant prolongation or reduction of the dominant component of motion onset VEPs, in contrast to pattern-reversal VEPs.
We conclude that for the temporal frequencies used, neither the high contrast nor high spatial content of the stimulus pattern interferes with the motion onset evoked potential’s resistance to blur. Therefore, if there is a need to study visual function in refractive error, response to the motion onset of a checkerboard can be a reliable method.
Data availability
The data we worked with in the analysis is available in the supplementary material of this paper.
Code availability
The original code for rendering stimuli with Zernike defocus was released in the supplementary materials of Young & Smithson (2014)16.
References
Odom, J. V. et al. ISCEV standard for clinical visual evoked potentials: (2016 update). Doc. Ophthalmol. 133, 1–9 (2016).
Bach, M., Maurer, J. P. & Wolf, M. E. Visual evoked potential-based acuity assessment in normal vision, artificially degraded vision, and in patients. Br. J. Ophthalmol. 92, 396–403 (2008).
Hoffmann, M. B., Brands, J., Behrens-Baumann, W. & Bach, M. VEP-based acuity assessment in low vision. Doc. Ophthalmol. 135, 209–218 (2017).
Heinrich, S. P. & Strübin, I. Use of diffusing filters for artificially reducing visual acuity when testing equipment and procedures. Doc. Ophthalmol. 140, 83–93 (2020).
Hassankarimi, H., Jafarzadehpur, E., Mohammadi, A. & Noori, S. M. Low-contrast Pattern-reversal Visual Evoked Potential in Different Spatial Frequencies. J. Ophthalmic Vis. Res. https://doi.org/10.1850/jovr.v15i3.7455 (2020).
Kenemans, J., Baas, J. M., Mangun, G., Lijffijt, M. & Verbaten, M. On the processing of spatial frequencies as revealed by evoked-potential source modeling. Clin. Neurophysiol. 111, 1113–1123 (2000).
Kordek, D., Voda, P., Young, L. K. & Kremlacek, J. Effect of Dioptric Blur on Pattern-Reversal and Motion-Onset VEPs as Used in Clinical Research. Transl. Vis. Sci. Technol. 11, 7 (2022).
Harter, M. R. & White, C. T. Effects of contour sharpness and check-size on visually evoked cortical potentials. Vision Res. 8, 701–711 (1968).
Kothari, R., Bokariya, P., Singh, S., Narang, P. & Singh, R. Refractive errors and their effects on visual evoked potentials. J. Clin. Ophthalmol. Res. 2, 3 (2014).
Kuba, M., Kubová, Z., Kremláček, J. & Langrová, J. Motion-onset VEPs: Characteristics, methods, and diagnostic use. Vision Res. 47, 189–202 (2007).
Heinrich, S. P. A primer on motion visual evoked potentials. Doc. Ophthalmol. 114, 83–105 (2007).
Kremláček, J., Kuba, M., Chlubnová, J. & Kubová, Z. Effect of stimulus localisation on motion-onset VEP. Vision Res. 44, 2989–3000 (2004).
Millodot, M. Refraction Determined Electrophysiologically. Arch. Ophthalmol. 84, 272 (1970).
Dehnert, A., Bach, M. & Heinrich, S. P. Subjective visual acuity with simulated defocus. Ophthalmic Physiol. Opt. 31, 625–631 (2011).
Delorme, A. & Makeig, S. EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J. Neurosci. Methods 134, 9–21 (2004).
Kordek, D., Young, L. K. & Kremláček, J. Comparison between optical and digital blur using near visual acuity. Sci. Rep. 11, 3437 (2021).
Dai, G. Wavefront Optics for Vision Correction (SPIE press, 2008).
Young, L. K. & Smithson, H. E. Critical band masking reveals the effects of optical distortions on the channel mediating letter identification. Front. Psychol. 5, 1060 (2014).
Thibos, L. N., Applegate, R. A., Schwiegerling, J. T. & Webb, R. Standards for Reporting the Optical Aberrations of Eyes. J. Refract. Surg. 18, S652–S660 (2002).
Benjamin, W. Borish’s Clinical Refraction (Butterworth-Heinemann, 2006).
Harris, C. R. et al. Array programming with NumPy. Nature 585, 357–362 (2020).
Li, J. et al. Quantifying sensory eye dominance in the normal visual system: A new technique and insights into variation across traditional tests. Investig. Opthalmology Vis. Sci. 51, 6875 (2010).
Cheng, C.-Y., Yen, M.-Y., Lin, H.-Y., Hsia, W.-W. & Hsu, W.-M. Association of Ocular Dominance and Anisometropic Myopia. Investig. Opthalmology Vis. Sci. 45, 2856 (2004).
ISO/TC 172/SC 7 Ophthalmic optics and instruments. ISO 8596:2017 Ophthalmic optics — Visual acuity testing — Standard and clinical optotypes and their presentation. 1–10 (2017).
Li, X. et al. Influence of lenslet configuration on short-term visual performance in myopia control spectacle lenses. Front. Neurosci. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2021.667329 (2021).
Pomerance, G. N. & Evans, D. W. Test-retest reliability of the CSV-1000 contrast test and its relationship to glaucoma therapy. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 35, 3357–3361 (1994).
Kremláček, J. et al. Within-session reproducibility of motion-onset VEPs: Effect of adaptation/habituation or fatigue on N2 peak amplitude and latency. Doc. Ophthalmol. 115, 95–103 (2007).
Kremláček, J. et al. Role of latency jittering correction in motion-onset VEP amplitude decay during prolonged visual stimulation. Doc. Ophthalmol. 124, 211–223 (2012).
Bobak, P., Bodis-Wollner, I. & Guillory, S. The effect of blur and contrast of VEP latency: Comparison between check and sinusoidal grating patterns. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. Potentials Sect. 68, 247–255 (1987).
Spehlmann, R. The averaged electrical responses to diffuse and to patterned light in the human. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 19, 560–569 (1965).
Lifshitz, K. The averaged evoked cortical response to complex visual stimuli. Psychophysiology 3, 55–68 (1966).
Ludlam, W. M. & Meyers, R. R. The use of visual evoked responses in objective refraction*. Trans. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 34, 154–170 (1972).
Regan, D. Rapid objective refraction using evoked brain potentials. Invest. Ophthalmol. 12, 669–679 (1973).
Hamilton, R. et al. ISCEV extended protocol for VEP methods of estimation of visual acuity. Doc. Ophthalmol. 142, 17–24 (2021).
Tumas, V. & Sakamoto, A. C. Comparison of the mechanisms of latency shift in pattern reversal visual evoked potential induced by blurring and contrast reduction. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. Potentials Sect. 104, 96–100 (1997).
Sokol, S. & Moskowitz, A. Effect of retinal blur on the peak latency of the pattern evoked potential. Vision Res. 21, 1279–1286 (1981).
Cobb, W. A., Morton, H. B. & Ettlinger, G. Cerebral potentials evoked by pattern reversal and their suppression in visual rivalry. Nature 216, 1123–1125 (1967).
Collins, D. W., Carroll, W. M., Black, J. L. & Walsh, M. Effect of refractive error on the visual evoked response. BMJ 1, 231–232 (1979).
Di Russo, F. et al. Identification of the neural sources of the pattern-reversal VEP. Neuroimage 24, 874–886 (2005).
Pitzalis, S., Strappini, F., De Gasperis, M., Bultrini, A. & Di Russo, F. Spatio-temporal brain mapping of motion-onset veps combined with fmri and Retinotopic maps. PLoS One 7, e35771 (2012).
Acknowledgements
The authors thank Ladislava Kolková for help with electrophysiological examination. Supported by Ministry of Health of the Czech Republic, Grant no. AZV NV18-06-00484 and ERDF-Project „BraDy“ No. CZ.02.01.01/00/22_008/0004643. L.Y. is supported by a UKRI Future Leader’s Fellowship (MR/T042192/1).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
All authors contributed to the study conception and design and material preparation. Data collection was performed by D.K. and P.V. Python code was done by L.Y. Analysis was done by J.K. and D.K. The first draft of the manuscript was written by D.K. and all authors commented on subsequent versions of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Kordek, D., Young, L., Voda, P. et al. Motion onset VEPs can see through the blur. Sci Rep 14, 21296 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-72483-z
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-72483-z
Keywords
Comments
By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.