Abstract
Muscle synergy analyses are used to enhance our understanding of motor control. Spatially fixed synergy weights coordinate multiple co-active muscles through activation commands, known as activation coefficients. To gain a more comprehensive understanding of motor learning, it is essential to understand how activation coefficients vary during a learning task and at different levels of movement proficiency. Participants walked on a line, a beam, and learned to walk on a tightrope—tasks that represent different levels of proficiency. Muscle synergies were extracted from electromyography signals across all conditions and the number of synergies was determined by the knee-point of the total variance accounted for (tVAF) curve. The results indicated that the tVAF of one synergy decreased with task proficiency, with the tightrope task resulting in the highest tVAF compared to the line and beam tasks. Furthermore, with increasing proficiency and after a learning process, trial-to-trial similarity increased and temporal overlap of synergy activation coefficients decreased. Consequently, we propose that precise adjustment and refinement of synergy activation coefficients play a pivotal role in motor learning.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
The underlying mechanisms by which the central nervous system governs movement control and adapts during skill acquisition are still not fully understood. Muscle synergy analyses are widely utilized in the field of motor control to model how the central nervous system coordinates movements1,2,3. In simple terms, muscle synergies refer to groups of co-active muscles, known as synergy weights, that are recruited by activation coefficients. While synergy weights are assumed to be spatially structured in the spinal cord, activation coefficients represent time-dependent control inputs from supraspinal areas1,3. In accordance with Bernstein’s levels of movement construction4,5, muscle synergies simplify the complex coordination of the numerous muscles in the human body by controlling the activation of a limited number of spatially fixed, and temporally independent synergy weights, rather than controlling each muscle individually.
Over the last two decades, researchers have studied muscle synergies extracted from electromyography recordings in both healthy and pathological populations performing various tasks. These studies have demonstrated that similar synergy weights are recruited in different movements. So-called shared synergies are likely to occur when different movement tasks involve common mechanical subtasks6. For instance, shared synergies have been found between walking and cycling7, walking and slipping6, walking and standing reactive balance tasks8, stepping and non-stepping postural behaviors9, seated and standing cycling10,11, and overground and treadmill running12. The identification of common synergies across different movements provides additional support for the concept of spatially fixed synergy weights. Furthermore, studies on the neuronal organization within the spinal cord in animals have provided evidence in support of the muscle synergy hypothesis13,14,15. To describe the complexity of motor control, the total variance accounted for (tVAF) by a given number of synergies and the number of needed synergies are widely used parameters. For example, fewer synergies and higher tVAF—indicating lower motor complexity—have been found in people with cerebral palsy16,17 and stroke18,19 compared to unimpaired populations, and in younger compared to older adults during walking20,21.
It is generally accepted that generating identical movements in successive attempts is impossible due to the inherently noisy nervous system22. This noise can originate from either the central nervous system, through movement planning, or from peripheral structures, such as force production by muscles. In 2017, Dhawale et al.23 conducted a review on movement variability in motor learning. They concluded that variability in the planning space is more likely a feature of the plasticity of the motor system that drives motor learning, rather than unwanted noise. Furthermore, trial-to-trial similarity of movements increases with increasing task proficiency23,24,25,26, aligning with the principles of reinforcement learning23. Based on reinforcement learning theory a system learns new behavior through a process of trial and error27. Motor commands that lead to favorable outcomes (i.e., successful execution of a movement task) are repeated, reinforced, and refined in subsequent attempts. Wu et al.24 conducted a study in which participants were trained to replicate a curve shape using hand trajectories in a reaching task. They found that individuals with higher kinematic variability prior to training showed faster rates of learning. Therefore, it appears that variability during the learning process increases the likelihood of finding the optimal motor command.
To date, only a few studies have examined the role of muscle synergies in movement learning. For instance, Sylos-Labini et al.28 compared synergy activation coefficients in walking across different age groups, from neonates to adults. They observed an increase in trial-to-trial similarity of activation coefficients during locomotor development. Consistent with a previous study on locomotor development29, the authors additionally showed that motor complexity and the number of synergies increased with age. Among young adults, an increase in trial-to-trial similarity of activation coefficients was highly correlated with improvements in bowling scores over three sessions30. Sawers et al.31 found that professional ballet dancers had higher trial-to-trial similarity and higher beam walking proficiency compared to individuals with no dance or gymnastics experience. Additionally, dancers exhibited higher consistency within synergy weights and higher spatial distinctness between synergy weights, with high spatial distinctness indicating that each synergy weight is composed of unique muscle contributions. Similarly, dance-based rehabilitation improved the consistency and spatial distinctness of synergy weights in individuals with Parkinson’s disease32. All of the studies mentioned were limited either by inter-participant comparisons28,33,34,35,36, which neglected individual differences in motor control, or inter-session comparisons35,36,37, which neglected variations in skin–electrode impedance and electrode position. A number of studies conducted by Berger and colleagues38,39,40 sought to overcome these limitations by employing a within-participant, within-session motor learning protocol. Briefly, participants were required to utilize isometric muscle contractions in order to move a mass within a virtual environment. Subsequently, muscle combinations to control the mass were altered by modifying the mapping between recorded muscle activity and the simulated force applied to the mass. If the mapping was altered in a way that participants could accurately move the mass only by modifying the temporal activation coefficients of existing synergy weights, the rate of adaptation was faster and the success rate was higher compared to a change of mapping that would require the acquisition of new synergy weights. Consequently, the authors proposed that adaptations of activation coefficients play a pivotal role in early learning, while actual changes of synergy weights or the formation of new synergies could potentially emerge after a longer time span. Although these findings provide comprehensive information regarding skill acquisition, the experiments were limited to isometric tasks and the learning of muscle combinations, rather than learning actual movements.
To the best of our knowledge, no study has yet explored changes in muscle synergies during movement learning using a within-participant, within-session protocol. Therefore, we collected motion capture data and electromyography signals from participants performing a learning task, i.e., learn to walk on a tightrope, which could be learned within one data collection session. Participants completed two additional walking tasks: walking on a line, which is an easy task with high movement proficiency, and walking on a beam, which was a less common but still manageable task for participants. We investigated whether there were differences in motor complexity (quantified with the tVAF of synergies), trial-to-trial similarity of activation coefficients, and temporal overlap of activation coefficient between (1) an early and a late stage of a learning process (i.e.: learning to walk on a tightrope), as well as between (2) common and less common movement tasks (i.e.: walking on a line vs a beam vs a tightrope)—addressing movement proficiency. Additionally, we investigated whether the contribution of synergy weights to a movement task changes during learning or with different proficiency levels. The study primarily focused on muscle synergies, but also analyzed trial-to-trial similarity of joint angles to gain a more comprehensive understanding of motor learning. We hypothesized that motor complexity, trial-to-trial similarity of synergy activation coefficients and joint angles would (1) increase during learning and (2) be higher for more common movements. In contrast, a decreased temporal overlap of activation coefficients was expected (1) after learning and (2) with higher task proficiency. Our study's findings will provide new insights into motor control adaptations during learning processes.
Materials and methods
Participants
Ten healthy participants (age: 25.2 ± 3.34 years; bodyweight: 69.9 ± 7.34 kg; height: 1.76 ± 0.09 m; body-mass-index: 22.63 ± 1.51 kg/m2; 6 men and 4 women) without neurological or orthopedic impairments were recruited for this study. All participants were recreational athletes from various disciplines who exercised at least twice a week and were not able to walk on a slackline or tightrope beforehand. The study was conducted in accordance with the relevant guidelines and regulations after receiving approval from the ethics committee of the University of Vienna (reference number: 00820). Written informed consent was obtained from all participants prior to data collection.
Experimental setup and data collection
Each participant performed three different walking tasks: (a) walking along a line taped on the ground (LINE; length: 310 cm; width: 1.4 cm); (b) walking on a beam (BEAM; length: 341.5 cm; width: 10 cm; height: 28.5 cm) and (c) walking over a tightrope (TIGHTROPE; length: 363 cm; diameter: 0.9 cm; height: 52 cm) spanned between two platforms (Fig. 1). The process of learning to walk on the tightrope was divided into two stages: TRfail and TRsucc. TRfail consisted of the first five attempts, during which participants were able to complete at least one full gait-cycle of the right leg but were unable to maintain balance over the entire tightrope. TRsucc included the attempts in which participants successfully walked over the tightrope in four out of five consecutive attempts. A successful attempt was defined as walking the entire length of the tightrope and maintaining balance on the second platform. If a participant successfully balanced over the entire tightrope in two out of the first five attempts, the task’s difficulty was increased with visual constraints. This was done by either placing an eye-patch over their left eye or, if they succeeded in two out of the first five attempts with an eye-patch, by closing both eyes. The conditions were recorded in the following order: (1) LINE-walking (startLINE), (2) BEAM-walking (startBEAM), (3) the start of the learning process on the TIGHTROPE (TRfail) until (4) the end of the learning process (TRsucc), (5) BEAM-walking (endBEAM), and (6) LINE-walking (endLINE). To ensure consistent visual constraints across tasks for later comparisons, five trials with opened eyes, an eye-patch over the left eye and both eyes closed were recorded each time (start and end) for LINE and BEAM. For participants who completed the learning task (i.e., walking over the tightrope) with visual constraints, the LINE and BEAM tasks with matching visual constraints were selected for further analyses to ensure that neural pathways were equally affected in each condition. As data from the first right stance phase was further analyzed, we aimed to minimize transient accelerations at the onset step17,20,41 by instructing participants to start each trial with their left leg. Only the stance phase was analyzed to neglect highly variable movement times between stance and swing phases across conditions36,42. No additional constraints were given for pause time, step cadence, step length, or hints for walking over the tightrope, in order to facilitate self-directed learning.
Prior to the data collection, thirteen surface electromyography (EMG) sensors (eleven PicoEMG and two Mini Wave Infinity, Wave Plus wireless EMG system, Cometa, Milan, Italy) were placed on the trunk and right limb according to the Seniam guidelines (Seniam.org), and recommendations from previous studies43,44,45: tibialis anterior (tib_abt), peroneus longus (per_long), soleus, gastrocnemius medialis (gast_med), vastus lateralis (vast_lat), rectus femoris (rect_fem), biceps femoris (bic_fem), semitendinosus (sem_tend), gluteus maximus (glut_max), rectus abdominis (rect_abd), extensor obliques (ext_obli), lumbar multifidus (multifid) and erector spinae iliocostalis (erec_spin). A baseline EMG signal of several seconds was recorded while participants lay in a supine and relaxed position on a massage table. The standard Vicon Plug-in-Gait marker set (Vicon, Oxford, UK), including 21 reflective markers, was placed on the legs, pelvis and trunk of each participant46. The heel and toe markers were placed on the participants’ shoes, similar to Paterson et al.47. A 12-camera 3D motion capture system (Vicon, Oxford, UK) was used to simultaneously record marker trajectories at a sampling rate of 200 Hz, EMG data at 1000 Hz and ground reaction forces from one force plate at 1000 Hz (Kistler Instrumente, Winterthur, Switzerland). In addition, participants wore in-shoe force sensor insoles (loadsol®, Novel, Munich, Germany), which were used to determine stance phases. Insoles data was captured at 200 Hz (loadsol-s android application version 1.7.63) on a mobile phone (Huawei P30 Lite, Huawei, Shenzhen, China) and brought to zero level every 5–10 trials to minimize errors caused by sensor drifts. Foot contact instances were determined by custom scripts based on vertical contact forces over 30 N. Time synchronization between the insole and Vicon data was achieved by participants stepping on the force plate at the beginning of each trial. The experimental data was captured and processed using Vicon Nexus 2.12 software (Vicon, Oxford, UK). Subsequent analyses were conducted using Gnu Octave version 6.2.048 and MATLAB (R2022a, Mathworks Inc., Natick, USA).
EMG processing
Raw EMG signals were high-pass filtered at 25 Hz (4th-order Butterworth zero lag-filter) to remove movement artefacts17,49,50,51, demeaned, full-wave rectified and low-pass filtered at 7 Hz (4th-order Butterworth zero-lag filter), similar to previous gait studies52,53,54,55,56. The choice of a 7 Hz low-pass cutoff frequency was a compromise due to different stance phase duration times between the three walking tasks (Supplementary Fig. S1). After filtering, baseline noise was removed by subtracting the root-mean-square of the filtered baseline EMG signal to improve the signal-to-noise ratio57,58,59,60, and any resulting negative values were set to zero. Based on a visual inspection of raw and filtered EMG envelopes, trials with artefacts (e.g., movement artefacts) were removed, resulting in 4–5 remaining trials per condition. Afterwards, signals were time-normalized to 101 data points (100% of stance phase) and amplitude normalized to values between 0 and 1, where an amplitude of 1 was equal to the maximum activation amplitude of a muscle among all trials19,21,41,42,54,61,62.
Synergy extraction and determining the number of synergies
For each participant, processed EMG signals of trials from all conditions were concatenated10,63 and muscle synergies were extracted according to the spatial/synchronous synergy model58. This model describes motor control of muscle activations (EMG signals) as a linear combination of fixed spatial synergy weights and time-varying activation coefficients4,42,58. Non-negative-matrix-factorization has been shown to be the most appropriate method for extracting muscle synergies in walking56. Therefore, we used the “nmf_bpas” octave function64, an advanced algorithm of classical non-negative-matrix-factorization65,66,67, to extract 1–12 (number of muscles − 1) muscle synergies. Instead of random inputs, the algorithm was initialized with outputs of the nonnegative single-value-decompensation with low-rank correction algorithm68 to improve the factorization58,68,69,70. Extracted synergy weights were normalized to 1 based on their maximum values, and activation coefficients were multiplied by the same normalization values to keep their product constant71,72. Detailed information on the synergy extraction procedure is provided in the Supplementary material.
The overall total variance accounted for (tVAF) among all conditions was calculated for each number of extracted synergies (1–12). It quantifies the reconstruction accuracy after the factorization, and is defined as the uncentered Pearson’s correlation in percentage55. To determine the number of synergies that represents motor control across all conditions (NoS), knee point analysis was employed42,50,55,58,73 on the overall tVAF curve. The knee-point (v) was defined as the point on the tVAF curve that exhibits the smallest angle among three adjacent points (v − 1, v, v + 1). This approach assumes that beyond the knee-point, only unstructured data or noise is explained by additional synergies73. The knee-point method was preferred over threshold-based methods due to its superior performance55 and independence from varying low-pass filter cutoff frequencies50. We further constrained our analysis by only determining the knee-point for synergies with a tVAF exceeding 95%. This threshold has been widely used52,55,60,63,74,75,76 and was added based the visual observation of sharp jumps in some tVAF curves, likely caused by the splitting of a synergy due to salient features77.
Synergy analyses
Overall synergy analyses
We calculated tVAF using the EMG signals, synergy weights and activation coefficients of each condition. Then, the tVAF of one synergy (tVAF1) and the tVAF at NoS (tVAFNoS) were compared across conditions to assess movement complexity (tVAF1) and the goodness of reconstruction (tVAFNoS). Overlap and trial-to-trial similarity of activation coefficients were quantified using the Pearson correlation coefficient (r), the maximum value of the normalized cross-correlation coefficient (rmax) and the lag time at which rmax occurred, indicating the time shift between two curves (lag% in % of the stance phase). These parameters are widely employed in biomechanical studies to analyze waveforms7,10,11,71,78,79,80. In our study, we used all three parameters as they evaluate slightly different aspects (Fig. 2). The overlap of activation coefficients was determined by calculating the average value of all pairwise combinations of activation coefficients from different synergies within each trial for each condition. High values of r and rmax, along with small time-shifts (lag%), indicate temporal similarity and therefore a substantial amount of overlap in synergy activation coefficients19,21,69. The overall trial-to-trial similarity of activation coefficients was determined by calculating the average value of all pairwise combinations of activation coefficients from different trials within each synergy for each condition. High values of r and rmax, along with small time-shifts (lag%), indicate high trial-to-trial similarity.
Individual synergy analyses
In addition to evaluating the overall trial-to-trial similarity of each walking condition, we aimed to determine whether any observed differences in trial-to-trial similarity were specific to particular synergies. This necessitated the identification of similar synergy weights among participants. To classify similar synergy weights, we used k-means clustering (kmeans function in Octave—see Supplementary material), similar to recent synergy studies28,30,34,54,81. We computed the k-means clustering solution for a range of two to twelve clusters and repeated the process 100 times. For each repetition and each number of clusters, we calculated the average silhouette value82. The optimal number of clusters was then determined at the point where the maximum silhouette values plateaued, indicating small within- and high between-cluster distances30. Trial-to-trial similarity parameters (r, rmax, lag%) were calculated for synergies within the same cluster for each condition. For instance, if a cluster consisted of eight synergy weights, the trial-to-trial similarity of that cluster was determined by the trial-to-trial similarity values of the eight synergies. To examine the task-specific relevance of individual synergy weights, the tVAF of each synergy was calculated for every trial. These tVAF values were then averaged across trials of the same condition for each synergy within the same cluster.
Kinematic analyses
Joint angles were computed with OpenSim83 using the recently introduced addBiomechanics.org application84. This application uses bilevel optimization and enables to personalize musculoskeletal models and calculate joint angles in an easy and efficient way. We used the default option with the Rajagopal2015 model for human gait85. The joint angles were computed and then smoothened using a 6 Hz low-pass filter (4th-order Butterworth zero-lag filter) and time normalized to 101 datapoints of the stance phases. The study examined the following joint angles of the right leg and trunk: ankle plantar-/dorsiflexion, knee flexion/extension, hip flexion/extension, hip ab-/adduction, hip internal/external rotation, lumbar flexion/extension, lumbar medial/lateral bending, and lumbar internal/external rotation. The trial-to-trial similarity of each joint angle was determined by calculating the r, rmax and lag% values of all pairwise combinations of trials for each condition. The overall trial-to-trial similarity of joint angles was then defined as the average of these values. Additionally, correlation values were also averaged for each joint separately to assess whether similarity differs in each joint (results provided in Supplementary material).
Statistics
We employed a two-way repeated measures ANOVA with TASK (line, beam, tightrope) and TIME as factors on all the variables described above. The first time point (START) consisted of startLINE, startBEAM, and TRfail, while the second time point (END) included endLINE, endBEAM, and TRsucc. To address our first research question regarding changes during the learning process, we calculated contrasts between TRfail and TRsucc. Furthermore, contrasts between startLINE and endLINE were examined as a baseline to assess the stability of the variable analyzed, as no differences were expected between the two LINE conditions. Contrasts between startBEAM and endBEAM were analyzed to investigate possible transfer effects of learning from one balancing task (tightrope) to another (beam). Contrasts were only conducted if a significant difference was observed in any of the ANOVA outcomes (TASK, TIME, TASK*TIME). To address the second research question, which concerns the differences between less and more common movements, we analyzed the factor TASK and conducted post hoc pairwise comparisons with Bonferroni correction. Prior, sphericity was checked using the Mauchly test (if necessary, Greenhouse–Geisser correction was applied), and normal distribution was verified using the Shapiro–Wilk test. If the normal distribution requirement was violated, an aligned-rank-transformation was performed. This transformation allowed us to conduct factorial ANOVAs on nonparametric data86,87,88 and was implemented using ARTool 2.1.2 software (Washington, USA). Statistical analyses were performed with JASP 0.17.2 (Amsterdam, Netherlands). The alpha level was set at 0.05 and the results were reported at three levels of significance: p < 0.05, p < 0.01, and p < 0.001.
Results
Participants required 2.6 ± 1.4 attempts (range: 1–5) to perform their first complete gait-cycle with the right leg on the tightrope (TRfail) and 49.4 ± 22.8 attempts (range: 12–101) to complete the learning task (TRsucc). Two participants walked on the tightrope with visual constraints (one participant wore an eye-patch, while the other participant walked with closed eyes). All participants successfully completed the task of walking over the entire line and beam on their first attempts.
Muscle synergy analyses
Overall synergy analyses
An average of 5.9 ± 1.1 NoS was determined among participants. A significant effect of TASK (p < 0.001) was observed for tVAF1. Post hoc comparisons revealed that tVAF1 was higher in BEAM compared to LINE (p < 0.01), and TIGHTROPE was higher than both LINE and BEAM tasks (p < 0.001). There were no significant differences in tVAFNoS. Regarding the overlap of activation coefficients, the ANOVA revealed a significant effect of TASK for r (p < 0.05), with activation coefficients being more correlated to each other in TIGHTROPE compared to LINE and BEAM tasks (p < 0.05). There was no significant difference for rmax, but a significant effect of TASK (p < 0.001) and TIME (p < 0.05) for lag%. LINE had a higher lag% than BEAM (p < 0.01), and TIGHTROPE had the lowest lag% (p < 0.001) (Fig. 3).
The trial-to-trial similarity, as measured by r and rmax, was affected by TASK (p < 0.001) and TIME (p < 0.01). The highest values for r and rmax were observed in LINE, followed by BEAM (rmax: p < 0.01; r < 0.001), and the lowest values were observed in TIGHTROPE (both: p < 0.001). Contrasts revealed an increase in similarity from startBEAM to endBEAM (both: p < 0.05) and from TRfail to TRsucc (rmax: p < 0.05; r: p < 0.001). The ANOVA showed no significant differences for lag% for any of the comparisons (Figs. 4, 5).
Individual synergy analyses
Silhouette analyses identified six clusters (Figs. 6, 7), labelled Cl_1 to Cl_6 in the following paragraphs. Low tVAF values indicate a low contribution of synergy weights to the task. TASK significantly affected the tVAF of all clusters (p < 0.05–p < 0.01). In cluster 4, tVAF of BEAM was lower than LINE (p < 0.05) and the lowest in TIGHTROPE (p < 0.001). For the other clusters, tVAF of TIGHTROPE was higher than BEAM (CL_2, CL_5 and CL_6 p < 0.05–p < 0.001) and LINE (CL_1, CL_2, CL_3, CL_5 and CL_6 p < 0.01–p < 0.001). In BEAM, tVAF was higher than LINE for cluster 2 (p < 0.01). For cluster 2, ANOVA also showed a significant effect of TIME (p < 0.05), with lower tVAF in START compared to END, and the interaction TASK × TIME (p < 0.01). In cluster 6, contrasts revealed a decrease (p < 0.05) of tVAF over time in BEAM.
The trial-to-trial similarity (quantified with r and rmax) of clusters 1, 2, 4, and 5 was significantly affected by TASK (rmax: CL_5 p < 0.05; r: CL_5 p < 0.01; others p < 0.001), with higher r (p < 0.001) and rmax (CL_5 p < 0.05; others p < 0.001) values for LINE than TIGHTROPE, and higher r values (CL_5 p < 0.01; others p < 0.001) for BEAM than TIGHTROPE. In clusters 1, 2 and 4, rmax was higher (p < 0.01–p < 0.001) in BEAM compared to TIGHTROPE. Correlations were higher in LINE than BEAM in clusters 1, 2 and 4 (r and rmax: p < 0.05–p < 0.001). The lag% showed a significant effect (p < 0.05–p < 0.001) of TASK for clusters 1, 3, 4, and 5. LINE had a lower lag% than BEAM (CL_1 p < 0.05) and TIGHTROPE (CL_1, CL_4 and CL_5 p < 0.01–p < 0.001). BEAM had a lower lag% compared to TIGHTROPE (CL_1 and CL_5 p < 0.01). In contrast, cluster 3 had the lowest lag% in TIGHTROPE compared to the other two tasks (p < 0.05).
Significant effects of TIME were found for r in cluster 1 (p < 0.05), for rmax in clusters 4 and 6 (p < 0.05) and for lag% in cluster 4 (p < 0.05), with lower correlations and higher lag% in START compared to END. A significant effect of TASK × TIME was only found for r in cluster 4 (p < 0.05). Contrasts revealed a significant increase of r or rmax from startLINE to endLINE in cluster 6 (rmax: p < 0.05), from startBEAM to endBEAM in cluster 2 (r: p < 0.01), and from TRfail to TRsucc in cluster 1 (r: p < 0.05).
Kinematic analysis
The overall trial-to-trial similarity of joint angles, quantified by r, rmax and lag%, was significantly affected by TASK (p < 0.001). LINE had the highest correlations and the lowest lag% between trials, followed by BEAM, and TIGHTROPE (rmax LINE vs BEAM: p < 0.01; lag% LINE vs BEAM: p < 0.05; others: p < 0.001). There was a significant effect of TIME on r (p < 0.05), with lower r in START compared to END, and a significant interaction effect of TASK × TIME (p < 0.05). For rmax, TIME had a significant effect (p < 0.01), with an increase observed between START and END. All contrasts were significantly different (p < 0.05). Likewise, lag% was significantly affected by TIME (p < 0.01). Contrasts revealed higher lag% (p < 0.05) in startLINE and TRfail compared to endLINE and TRsucc, respectively (Figs. 4, 8).
Discussion
The study aimed to enhance our understanding of motor learning through synergy analysis, employing a within-participant, within-session study design. We observed reduced overlap and increased trial-to-trial similarity of activation coefficients after learning and with increasing movement proficiency. Additionally, our analyses revealed that the contribution of specific synergy weights to movements vary across tasks.
Over half a century ago, Bernstein5 proposed that in the early stages of learning, the number of possible movement executions is minimized to the essential elements required to master the fundamental aspects of a skill, thereby simplifying control. This concept, called “freezing the degrees of freedom”, has primarily been studied at the kinematic level. Guimarães et al.89 conducted a review focusing on joint kinematics and showed that during the initial learning phase, the joint range of motion is reduced and there is a higher temporal coupling of joint angles (e.g., adjacent joints extend/flex simultaneously) compared to a state of sufficient proficiency. However, to our knowledge, the mechanisms by which the central nervous system simplifies motor control in early learning remain unclear. Steele et al.74 found greater overlap of synergy activation coefficients when biomechanical and task constraints were present. Our study showed higher tVAF1 and greater overlapping of synergy activation coefficients—both indicating higher co-activation of synergies—in movements with low (i.e., walking over a tightrope) compared to movements with high (i.e., walking on a line) proficiency. In light of these findings, we hypothesize that in movements with low proficiency and during early learning, the temporal co-activation of synergies represents a simplification strategy of the central nervous system, which leads to the freezing of the degrees of freedom. While this hypothesis calls for further research, we are confident that a high temporal overlap of activation coefficients represents a compensation strategy of the central nervous system to promote robust motor control. Similarly, previous studies have demonstrated that activation coefficients were activated over a longer time period during and following gait perturbations compared with unperturbed walking90,91,92, and in patients with neurological disorders compared to healthy individuals93,94,95. The authors concluded that the longer recruitment of activation coefficients, corresponding to a greater temporal overlap, was a strategy to account for the higher demands during challenging locomotion tasks. Our study supports this theory and implies a modulation of the temporal overlap due to proficiency and during early learning.
Motor complexity has been assessed previously by the tVAF and NoS16,17,18,20,21. However, high tVAF values may arise from overlapping activation coefficients, representing a simplification strategy of the central nervous system to minimize the number of possible movement solutions, rather than a simpler motor control due to a decreased number of synergies necessary for the task per se. This idea is supported by previous studies on both impaired and unimpaired populations. Clark et al.21 found similar synergy weights in locomotion for stroke survivors and unimpaired individuals, when the same number of synergies were extracted, rather than the number determined by a tVAF threshold. The authors concluded that the spatial synergy weights did not differ, but that they were computationally merged by the factorization algorithm due to their overlapping recruitment profiles. Similarly, merging of synergy weights has been observed in post-stroke survivors96, individuals with Parkinson’s disease42 and people after cortical lesions97. In summary, we propose that one simplification strategy of the central nervous system to promote robust motor control is to co-activate muscle synergies. This can be measured by the tVAF and the temporal overlap of synergy activation coefficients.
Our findings indicate that the temporal overlap of activation coefficients can have a big impact on muscle synergy analyses, including the number of shared synergies. Factorization algorithms extract synergies based on muscle co-activation. Therefore, computational merging of synergies due to overlapping activation coefficients may result in synergies that do not accurately reflect the underlying mechanisms in the central nervous system. Previous studies on real and simulated datasets69,73,74 have observed this phenomenon. In these studies, increased temporal overlap of activation coefficients led to merging of synergy weights due to the underlying assumptions of the factorization algorithms. In the current study, we found a small number of shared synergies when calculated separately for each condition (see Supplementary material), but similar synergies when calculated over all conditions. Additionally, our results showed a great temporal overlap of activation coefficients in the low compared to the high proficiency movement tasks. These findings suggest that similar synergies were used for the tasks, but high overlap of activation coefficients during the low proficiency tasks may have resulted in the low number of shared synergies. In previous studies, a lower amount of shared synergies between overground walking and balancing tasks has been found in individuals with no dance experience compared to expert dancers8,31, in stroke survivors compared to unimpaired individuals98, and prior to a dance-based rehabilitation in individuals with Parkinson’s disease32. Two of these studies also found lower spatial distinctness of synergy weights in individuals with no dance experience31, and before the dance-based rehabilitation32. The lower distinctness of the computed synergy weights may be a result of a greater overlap of activation coefficients, which can compromise the extracted synergy weights. Consequently, merged synergy weights could be a reason for the lower number of shared synergies in previous studies. Additionally, Santuz et al.62 demonstrated that the amount of merged synergy weights could vary depending on the factorization algorithm employed and called for greater awareness in attributing a neurophysiological nature to highly merged synergies. These findings emphasize the need for caution when interpreting muscle synergy results, especially in relation to shared synergies, tVAF and NoS. To reduce the impact of computational merging, we calculated synergies over the entire dataset for each participant rather than extracting each condition separately. This approach holds potential for advancing synergy research and is discussed in more detail in the Supplementary material.
The current study showed an increase in trial-to-trial similarity of activation coefficients during learning and with higher movement proficiency (Fig. 4). These results support previous research on trial-to-trial similarity as outlined in the introduction23,24,25,26,28,30,31. In terms of the overall trial-to-trial similarity of activation coefficients, we observed a transfer effect of a balance training from the tightrope to the beam task. However, there were no differences between startLINE and endLINE, suggesting that differences did not occur due to movement-artefacts or sensor-noise. Through cluster analysis, we were able to detect changes in similarity in certain synergies (Fig. 7). Cluster 6—mainly formed by tib_ant, vast_lat and rec_fem—did not show any changes in similarity due to proficiency or learning. Additionally, cluster 3—mainly formed by per_long—had a lower lag% in tightrope walking compared to the other two walking tasks. We speculate that the recruitment of synergies belonging to these two clusters is not crucial for different levels of proficiency or learning. The other clusters showed that trial-to-trial similarity increases with movement proficiency. While we observed an increase of trial-to-trial similarity from startBEAM to endBEAM and from TRfail to TRsucc in certain clusters, other clusters showed no changes throughout the learning process. This suggests that early learning is driven by an increase in the consistency of certain synergies, while other synergies increase their consistency in a later learning stage, i.e., at higher proficiency levels.
The duration of analyzed movements can affect muscle synergy analyses due to differences in smoothing of EMG envelopes36. In our study, stance phase duration differed between tasks and between TRsucc and TRfail. Specifically, stance phases were shorter in TRsucc than TRfail (Supplementary material). This explains the smoother synergy activation coefficients in LINE and BEAM tasks compared to the TIGHTROPE task36 (Fig. 5). One could assume less trial-to-trail similarity in TIGHTROPE compared to the other two tasks due to less smoothed activation coefficients. However, this would not explain the differences in similarity between LINE and BEAM tasks, as stance duration did not differ between these two tasks. To further evaluate whether our findings were affected by the different task durations, we adjusted the low-pass cutoff filter frequency for each trial based on its duration and repeated our main analyses on synergies. Detailed information and results for these additional analyses are provided in the Supplementary material. Briefly, these analyses showed similar results in terms of trial-to-trial similarity and overlap of activation coefficient between the tasks. Moreover, the temporal overlap of activation coefficients decreased from TRfail to TRsucc. In summary, the additional and main analyses yielded the same conclusions. Namely, fine tuning of synergy recruitment, i.e. increasing trial-to-trial similarity and decreasing temporal overlap of activation coefficients, is important for motor learning. This viewpoint is corroborated by studies conducted by Berger et al.38,39,40, which demonstrated the significance of activation coefficient adaptation in early motor learning. We hypothesize that upon a completion of the learning process, indicated by the successful completion of all attempts at TIGHTROPE walking (rather than just 4 out of 5), the trial-to-trial similarity and the overlap of activation coefficients will be comparable to those observed in LINE and BEAM tasks.
To gain a more comprehensive understanding of changes in trial-to-trial similarity, we also examined changes in similarity of joint angles (Figs. 4, 8). Surprisingly, the overall trial-to-trial similarity of kinematic data was not only higher with proficiency and after learning, but also in endLINE compared to startLINE. Therefore, we hypothesize that synergies reflect motor planning by the central nervous system, whereas joint kinematics is more influenced by peripheral noise in the movement execution22,23. Further analysis of the trial-to-trial similarity of EMG envelopes yielded comparable results to those observed in the overall synergy analyses with regard to task proficiency (see Supplementary material).
In the field of motor learning and development, three theories are widely discussed: the strict nativist view, the learning hypothesis, and the combined approach28. The strict nativist view proposes that locomotor patterns remain robustly conserved into adulthood, supported by the spatial synergy model42,58 and studies analyzing stepping patterns in newborns99. The learning hypothesis proposes that unstructured movement patterns are transformed into structured solutions during development through the interaction between the body and the environment, supported by studies showing low trial-to-trial similarity of both kinematics and synergies in early learning23,24,25,26,28,30,31. The combined approach posits the existence of conserved movement patterns that are enriched with new patterns to represent a wider range of tasks. The latter concept has recently been supported by analyses of muscle synergies in the development of walking28 and running100. In our study, we found similar synergy weights across tasks (see Supplementary material for a more detailed discussion). Besides similar synergy weights, we observed lower consistency in activation coefficients at low compared to high proficiency levels. Moreover, some synergy weights showed a minimal contribution to the line and beam tasks but were important for tightrope walking (Fig. 7), hinting towards an enrichment of the motor control repertoire. These findings therefore provide support for the combined theory for motor learning.
Our study included three notable limitations. Firstly, due to the intra-session design, we captured a limited number of gait cycles per condition. Oliveira et al.41 suggested extracting muscle synergies over a minimum of 20 concatenated steps to account for trial-to-trial variability in movement execution. To address this, we performed our main analysis on concatenated data of all conditions, including 24–30 stance phases per participant. Secondly, we defined the learning process as complete when participants successfully walked across the entire tightrope in four out of five consecutive attempts, which may not reflect a high level of proficiency. Nonetheless, despite this limitation, we observed significant changes in most of the parameters analyzed between TRfail and TRsucc. Thirdly, the study population was relatively small, consisting of ten young recreational athletes. Although this sample size aligns with most synergy studies10,30,32,78, future research should include a larger and more diverse participant pool. The study findings should be interpreted with consideration to the potential differences in learning rate and process between different populations, such as young and elderly individuals30,101,102,103.
In summary, we found that increasing movement proficiency leads to less overlap and higher trial-to-trial similarity of synergy activation coefficients. We propose that the higher temporal overlap represents a strategy employed by the central nervous system to promote robust motor control in challenging tasks with low proficiency and early learning. Furthermore, we suggest that fine-tuning of synergy activation coefficients is essential for motor learning.
Data availability
The raw and processed EMG signals, joint angles, and events (start and end of stance phases) are available as anonymized data on https://osf.io/x52je/?view_only=7f051720558a421e957a316bad1c0992. For c3d files and Octave codes, contact Paul Kaufmann at paul.kaufmann@univie.ac.at or Hans Kainz at hans.kainz@univie.ac.at. The authors will assist with any reasonable replication attempts for 2 years following publication.
References
Bizzi, E., Tresch, M. C. & Saltiel, P. The construction of movement by the spinal cord. Nat. Neurosci. 2, 167. https://doi.org/10.1038/5721 (1999).
d’Avella, A. & Bizzi, E. Shared and specific muscle synergies in natural motor behaviors. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 102, 3081. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0500199102 (2005).
Bizzi, E. & Cheung, V. C. K. The neural origin of muscle synergies. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 7, 6. https://doi.org/10.3389/fncom.2013.00051 (2013).
Profeta, V. L. S. & Turvey, M. T. Bernstein’s levels of movement construction: A contemporary perspective. Hum. Mov. Sci. 57, 133. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2017.11.013 (2018).
Bernstein, N. The Coordination and Regulation of Movements (Perganmon Press, 1967).
Nazifi, M. M., Yoon, H. U., Beschorner, K. & Hur, P. Shared and task-specific muscle synergies during normal walking and slipping. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11, 40. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2017.00040 (2017).
Barroso, F. O. et al. Shared muscle synergies in human walking and cycling. J. Neurophysiol. 112, 1998. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00220.2014 (2014).
Allen, J. L., Carey, H. D., Ting, L. H. & Sawers, A. Generalization of motor module recruitment across standing reactive balance and walking is associated with beam walking performance in young adults. Gait Posture 82, 247. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2020.09.016 (2020).
Chvatal, S. A., Torres-Oviedo, G., Safavynia, S. A. & Ting, L. H. Common muscle synergies for control of center of mass and force in nonstepping and stepping postural behaviors. J. Neurophysiol. 106, 1015. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00549.2010 (2011).
Hug, F., Turpin, N. A., Couturier, A. & Dorel, S. “Consistency of muscle synergies during pedaling across different mechanical constraints,” (in eng). J. Neurophysiol. 106(1), 91–103. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.01096.2010 (2011).
Turpin, N. A., Costes, A., Moretto, P. & Watier, B. Can muscle coordination explain the advantage of using the standing position during intense cycling?. J. Sci. Med. Sport 20, 616. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsams.2016.10.019 (2017).
Oliveira, A. S., Gizzi, L., Ketabi, S., Farina, D. & Kersting, U. G. Modular control of treadmill vs overground running. PLoS One 11, e0153307. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0153307 (2016).
Hart, C. B. & Giszter, S. F. A neural basis for motor primitives in the spinal cord. J. Neurosci. 30(4), 1322–1336. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.5894-08.2010 (2010).
Takei, T., Confais, J., Tomatsu, S., Oya, T. & Seki, K. Neural basis for hand muscle synergies in the primate spinal cord. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 114(32), 8643–8648. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1704328114 (2017).
Saltiel, P., Wyler-Duda, K., D’Avella, A., Tresch, M. C. & Bizzi, E. “Muscle synergies encoded within the spinal cord: evidence from focal intraspinal NMDA iontophoresis in the frog,” (in eng). J. Neurophysiol. 85(2), 605–619. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.2001.85.2.605 (2001).
Steele, K. M., Rozumalski, A. & Schwartz, M. H. Muscle synergies and complexity of neuromuscular control during gait in cerebral palsy. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 57, 1182. https://doi.org/10.1111/dmcn.12826 (2015).
Shuman, B. R., Schwartz, M. H. & Steele, K. M. “Electromyography data processing impacts muscle synergies during gait for unimpaired children and children with cerebral palsy,” (in English). Front. Comput. Neurosci. https://doi.org/10.3389/fncom.2017.00050 (2017).
Van Criekinge, T. et al. Lower limb muscle synergies during walking after stroke: A systematic review. Disability Rehabilit. 42(20), 2836–2845. https://doi.org/10.1080/09638288.2019.1578421 (2020).
Clark, D. J., Ting, L. H., Zajac, F. E., Neptune, R. R. & Kautz, S. A. A. Merging of healthy motor modules predicts reduced locomotor performance and muscle coordination complexity post-stroke. J. Neurophysiol. 103, 857. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00825.2009 (2010).
da Silva Costa, A. A., Moraes, R., Hortobágyi, T. & Sawers, A. Older adults reduce the complexity and efficiency of neuromuscular control to preserve walking balance. Exp. Gerontol. 140, 111050. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.exger.2020.111050 (2020).
Clark, D. J., Ting, L. H., Zajac, F. E., Neptune, R. R. & Kautz, S. A. Merging of healthy motor modules predicts reduced locomotor performance and muscle coordination complexity post-stroke. J. Neurophysiol. 103, 857. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00825.2009 (2010).
Faisal, A. A., Selen, L. P. J. & Wolpert, D. M. Noise in the nervous system. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 303. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2258 (2008).
Dhawale, A. K., Smith, M. A. & Ölveczky, B. P. The role of variability in motor learning. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 40, 498. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-072116-031548 (2017).
Wu, H. G., Miyamoto, Y. R., Castro, L. N. G., Ölveczky, B. P. & Smith, M. A. Temporal structure of motor variability is dynamically regulated and predicts motor learning ability. Nat. Neurosci. 17, 321. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3616 (2014).
Cardis, M., Casadio, M. & Ranganathan, R. High variability impairs motor learning regardless of whether it affects task performance. J. Neurophysiol. 119, 48. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00158.2017 (2018).
Levy-Tzedek, S. Motor errors lead to enhanced performance in older adults. Sci. Rep. 7, 8. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-03430-4 (2017).
L. P. Kaelbling, M. L. Littman, and A. W. Moore. Reinforcement learning: a survey," arXiv.org, 1996.
Sylos-Labini, F. et al. Complexity of modular neuromuscular control increases and variability decreases during human locomotor development. Commun. Biol. 5(1), 1256. https://doi.org/10.1038/s42003-022-04225-8 (2022).
Dominici, N. et al. “Locomotor primitives in newborn babies and their development,” (in eng). Science 334(6058), 997–999. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1210617 (2011).
Cheung, V. C. K., Zheng, X.-C., Cheung, R. T. H. & Chan, R. H. M. Modulating the structure of motor variability for skill learning through specific muscle synergies in elderlies and young adults. IEEE Open. J. Eng. Med. Biol. 1, 40. https://doi.org/10.1109/OJEMB.2019.2963666 (2020).
Sawers, A., Allen, J. L. & Ting, L. H. Long-term training modifies the modular structure and organization of walking balance control. J. Neurophysiol. 114, 3373. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00758.2015 (2015).
Allen, J. L., McKay, J. L., Sawers, A., Hackney, M. E. & Ting, L. H. Increased neuromuscular consistency in gait and balance after partnered, dance-based rehabilitation in parkinson’s disease. J. Neurophysiol. 118, 373. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00813.2016 (2017).
Zhao, K., Zhang, Z., Wen, H. & Scano, A. Intra-subject and inter-subject movement variability quantified with muscle synergies in upper-limb reaching movements. Biomimetics (Basel, Switzerland). https://doi.org/10.3390/biomimetics6040063 (2021).
Scano, A. et al. A Comprehensive spatial mapping of muscle synergies in highly variable upper-limb movements of healthy subjects. Front. Physiol. https://doi.org/10.3389/fphys.2019.01231 (2019).
Pale, U., Atzori, M., Müller, H. & Scano, A. Variability of muscle synergies in hand grasps: Analysis of intra-and inter-session data. Sensors (Basel). https://doi.org/10.3390/s20154297 (2020).
Hug, F. Can muscle coordination be precisely studied by surface electromyography?. J. Electromyogr. Kinesiol. 21, 12. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jelekin.2010.08.009 (2011).
Kristiansen, M., Samani, A., Madeleine, P. & Hansen, E. A. Muscle synergies during bench press are reliable across days. J. Electromyogr. Kinesiol. 30, 88. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jelekin.2016.06.004 (2016).
Berger, D. J., Gentner, R., Edmunds, T., Pai, D. K. & d’Avella, A. “Differences in adaptation rates after virtual surgeries provide direct evidence for modularity,” (in eng). J. Neurosci. 33(30), 12384–12394. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.0122-13.2013 (2013).
Berger, D. J., Borzelli, D. & d’Avella, A. Task space exploration improves adaptation after incompatible virtual surgeries. J. Neurophysiol. 127(4), 1127–1146. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00356.2021 (2022).
Berger, D. J. & d’Avella, A. “Persistent changes in motor adaptation strategies after perturbations that require exploration of novel muscle activation patterns,” (in eng). J. Neurophysiol. 130(5), 1194–1199. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00154.2023 (2023).
Oliveira, A. S., Gizzi, L., Farina, D. & Kersting, U. G. “Motor modules of human locomotion: Influence of EMG averaging, concatenation, and number of step cycles,” (in English). Front. Hum. Neurosci. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00335 (2014).
Ghislieri, M., Lanotte, M., Knaflitz, M., Rizzi, L. & Agostini, V. Muscle synergies in Parkinson’s disease before and after the deep brain stimulation of the bilateral subthalamic nucleus. Sci. Rep. 13, 6997. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-023-34151-6 (2023).
Huebner, A., Faenger, B., Scholle, H.-C. & Anders, C. Re-evaluation of the amplitude–force relationship of trunk muscles. J. Biomech. 48, 1205. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbiomech.2015.02.016 (2015).
Vera-Garcia, F. J., Moreside, J. M. & McGill, S. M. MVC techniques to normalize trunk muscle EMG in healthy women. J. Electromyogr. Kinesiol. 20, 16. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jelekin.2009.03.010 (2009).
Oshikawa, T., Adachi, G., Akuzawa, H., Okubo, Y. & Kaneoka, K. Electromyographic analysis of abdominal muscles during abdominal bracing and hollowing among six different positions. JPFSM 9, 163. https://doi.org/10.7600/jpfsm.9.157 (2020).
Kadaba, M. P., Ramakrishnan, H. K. & Wootten, M. E. “Measurement of lower extremity kinematics during level walking,” (in eng). J. Orthop. Res. 8(3), 383–392. https://doi.org/10.1002/jor.1100080310 (1990).
Paterson, K. L., Hinman, R. S., Metcalf, B. R., Bennell, K. L. & Wrigley, T. V. Plug-in-Gait calculation of the knee adduction moment in people with knee osteoarthritis during shod walking: Comparison of two different foot marker models. J. Foot Ankle Res. 10, 8. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13047-017-0187-4 (2017).
J. W. Eaton, D. Bateman, S. Hauberg, and R. Wehbring. "GNU Octave version 6.2.0 manual: a high-level interactive language for numerical computations." https://www.gnu.org/software/octave/doc/v6.2.0/
Potvin, J. R. & Brown, S. H. M. Less is more: high pass filtering, to remove up to 99% of the surface EMG signal power, improves EMG-based biceps brachii muscle force estimates. J. Electromyogr. Kinesiol. 14, 399. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jelekin.2003.10.005 (2004).
Hug, F., Turpin, N. A., Dorel, S. & Guével, A. Smoothing of electromyographic signals can influence the number of extracted muscle synergie. Clin. Neurophysiol. 123, 1896. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2012.01.015 (2012).
van der Krogt, M. et al. The effect of EMG processing choices on muscle synergies before and after BoNT-A treatment in cerebral palsy. Gait Posture 49, 31. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2016.07.095 (2016).
van den Hoorn, W., van Dieen, J. H., Hodges, P. W. & Hug, F. Effect of acute noxious stimulation to the leg or back on muscle synergies during walking. J. Neurophysiol. 113, 54. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00557.2014 (2015).
Boccia, G., Zoppirolli, C., Bortolan, L., Schena, F. & Pellegrini, B. Shared and task-specific muscle synergies of Nordic walking and conventional walking. Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports 28, 918. https://doi.org/10.1111/sms.12992 (2018).
Kim, Y., Bulea, T. C. & Damiano, D. L. Novel methods to enhance precision and reliability in muscle synergy identification during walking. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10, 455. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00455 (2016).
Ballarini, R., Ghislieri, M., Knaflitz, M. & Agostini, V. An algorithm for choosing the optimal number of muscle synergies during walking. Sensors (Basel, Switzerland). https://doi.org/10.3390/s21103311 (2021).
Rabbi, M. F. et al. Non-negative matrix factorisation is the most appropriate method for extraction of muscle synergies in walking and running. Sci. Rep. 10, 8266. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-65257-w (2020).
Roh, J., Rymer, W. Z., Perreault, E. J., Yoo, S. B. & Beer, R. F. Alterations in upper limb muscle synergy structure in chronic stroke survivors. J. Neurophysiol. 109, 781. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00670.2012 (2013).
Turpin, N. A., Uriac, S. & Dalleau, G. How to improve the muscle synergy analysis methodology?. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 121, 1025. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00421-021-04604-9 (2021).
L. Frey Law, C. Krishnan, and K. Avin. Modeling nonlinear errors in surface electromyography due to baseline noise: A new methodology. J. Biomech. 44, 205 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbiomech.2010.09.008.
S. Hiep Vu Nguyen, I. Nambu, and Y. Wada, "The adjustment of muscle synergy recruitment by controlling muscle contraction during the reaching movement," in 2016 IEEE International Conference on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics (SMC), 9–12 Oct. 2016 2016, pp. 000756–000761, https://doi.org/10.1109/SMC.2016.7844332.
Bianco, N. A., Patten, C. & Fregly, B. J. Can measured synergy excitations accurately construct unmeasured muscle excitations? (in eng). J. Biomech. Eng. https://doi.org/10.1115/1.4038199 (2018).
Santuz, A., Ekizos, A., Janshen, L., Baltzopoulos, V. & Arampatzis, A. “On the methodological implications of extracting muscle synergies from human locomotion,” (in eng). Int. J. Neural Syst. 27(5), 1750007. https://doi.org/10.1142/S0129065717500071 (2017).
Hagio, S., Fukuda, M. & Kouzaki, M. Identification of muscle synergies associated with gait transition in humans. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9, 48. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00048 (2015).
J. Kim and H. Park. Toward Faster Nonnegative Matrix Factorization: A New Algorithm and Comparisons. in presented at the Proceedings of the 2008 Eighth IEEE International Conference on Data Mining, 2008. [Online]. https://doi.org/10.1109/ICDM.2008.149.
Seung, H. S. & Lee, D. D. Learning the parts of objects by non-negative matrix factorization. Nature 401, 791. https://doi.org/10.1038/44565 (1999).
D. Lee and H. Seung. Algorithms for non-negative matrix factorization. Adv. Neural Inform. Process. Syst. 13 (2001).
Paatero, P. & Tapper, U. Positive matrix factorization: A non-negative factor model with optimal utilization of error estimates of data value. Environmetrics 5, 111–126 (1994).
Atif, S. M., Qazi, S. & Gillis, N. Improved SVD-based initialization for nonnegative matrix factorization using low-rank correction. Pattern Recognit. Lett. 122, 59. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.patrec.2019.02.018 (2019).
Soomro, M. H., Conforto, S., Giunta, G., Ranaldi, S. & De Marchis, C. Comparison of initialization techniques for the accurate extraction of muscle synergies from myoelectric signals via nonnegative matrix factorization. Appl. Bionics Biomech. 2018, 10. https://doi.org/10.1155/2018/3629347 (2018).
Boutsidis, C. & Gallopoulos, E. SVD based initialization: A head start for nonnegative matrix factorization. Pattern Recognit. 41, 1362. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.patcog.2007.09.010 (2008).
Banks, C. L., Pai, M. M., McGuirk, T. E., Fregly, B. J. & Patten, C. Methodological choices in muscle synergy analysis impact differentiation of physiological characteristics following stroke. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 11, 78. https://doi.org/10.3389/fncom.2017.00078 (2017).
Safavynia, S. A. & Ting, L. H. Task-level feedback can explain temporal recruitment of spatially fixed muscle synergies throughout postural perturbations. J. Neurophysiol. 107, 177. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00653.2011 (2012).
Tresch, M. C., Cheung, V. C. K. & d’Avella, A. Matrix factorization algorithms for the identification of muscle synergies: evaluation on simulated and experimental data sets. J. Neurophysiol. 95, 2212. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00222.2005 (2006).
Steele, K. M., Tresch, M. C. & Perreault, E. J. Consequences of biomechanically constrained tasks in the design and interpretation of synergy analyses. J. Neurophysiol. 113, 2113. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00769.2013 (2015).
Meyer, A. J. et al. Muscle synergies facilitate computational prediction of subject-specific walking motions. Front. Bioeng. Biotechnol. 4, 77. https://doi.org/10.3389/fbioe.2016.00077 (2016).
Rodriguez, K. L., Roemmich, R. T., Cam, B., Fregly, B. J. & Hass, C. J. Persons with Parkinson’s disease exhibit decreased neuromuscular complexity during gait. Clin. Neurophysiol. 124, 1397. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2013.02.006 (2013).
L. Ting and S. Chvatal. Decomposing Muscle Activity in Motor TasksMethods and Interpretation," 2010, pp. 102–138.
Frère, J. & Hug, F. “Between-subject variability of muscle synergies during a complex motor skill,” (in English). Front. Comput. Neurosci. https://doi.org/10.3389/fncom.2012.00099 (2012).
Ivanenko, Y. P., Poppele, R. E. & Lacquaniti, F. Five basic muscle activation patterns account for muscle activity during human locomotion. J. Physiol. 556, 282. https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.2003.057174 (2004).
Ogihara, H. et al. Kinematic gait asymmetry assessment using joint angle data in patients with chronic stroke—A normalized cross-correlation approach. Gait Posture 80, 173. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2020.05.042 (2020).
Kim, Y., Bulea, T. C. & Damiano, D. L. Children with cerebral palsy have greater stride-to-stride variability of muscle synergies during gait than typically developing children: Implications for motor control complexity. Neurorehabil. Neural. Repair. 32, 844. https://doi.org/10.1177/1545968318796333 (2018).
Rousseeuw, P. J. Silhouettes: A graphical aid to the interpretation and validation of cluster analysis. J. Comput. Appl. Math. 20, 53–65. https://doi.org/10.1016/0377-0427(87)90125-7 (1987).
Delp, S. L. et al. “OpenSim: Open-source software to create and analyze dynamic simulations of movement,” (in eng). IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. 54(11), 1940–1950. https://doi.org/10.1109/tbme.2007.901024 (2007).
W. Keenon et al., "Rapid bilevel optimization to concurrently solve musculoskeletal scaling, marker registration, and inverse kinematic problems for human motion reconstruction. bioRxiv. 2022.08.22.504896, 2022, https://doi.org/10.1101/2022.08.22.504896.
Rajagopal, A. et al. Full-body musculoskeletal model for muscle-driven simulation of human gait. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. 63, 2079. https://doi.org/10.1109/TBME.2016.2586891 (2016).
J. Higgins, R. Blair, and S. Tashtoush. The aligned rank transform procedure. in Conference on Applied Statistics in Agriculture, 04/26 1990, https://doi.org/10.4148/2475-7772.1443.
J. Wobbrock, L. Findlater, D. Gergle, and J. Higgins. The Aligned Rank Transform for Nonparametric Factorial Analyses Using Only ANOVA Procedures. in Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems, p. 146, 2011, https://doi.org/10.1145/1978942.1978963.
L. A. Elkin, M. Kay, J. J. Higgins, and J. O. Wobbrock, "An Aligned Rank Transform Procedure for Multifactor Contrast Tests," p. 768, 2021, https://doi.org/10.1145/3472749.3474784.
Guimarães, A. N., Ugrinowitsch, H., Dascal, J. B., Porto, A. B. & Okazaki, V. H. A. “Freezing degrees of freedom during motor learning: a systematic review,” (in eng). Motor Control 24(3), 457–471. https://doi.org/10.1123/mc.2019-0060 (2020).
Brüll, L. et al. Spatiotemporal modulation of a common set of muscle synergies during unpredictable and predictable gait perturbations in older adults. J. Exp. Biol. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.247271 (2024).
Santuz, A., Laflamme, O. D. & Akay, T. “The brain integrates proprioceptive information to ensure robust locomotion,” (in eng). J. Physiol. 600(24), 5267–5294. https://doi.org/10.1113/JP283181 (2022).
Santuz, A., Ekizos, A., Eckardt, N., Kibele, A. & Arampatzis, A. “Challenging human locomotion: stability and modular organisation in unsteady conditions,” (in eng). Sci. Rep. 8(1), 2740. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-21018-4 (2018).
Martino, G. et al. “Neuromuscular adjustments of gait associated with unstable conditions,” (in eng). J. Neurophysiol. 114(5), 2867–2882. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00029.2015 (2015).
Janshen, L., Santuz, A., Ekizos, A. & Arampatzis, A. Fuzziness of muscle synergies in patients with multiple sclerosis indicates increased robustness of motor control during walking," (in eng). Sci. Rep. 10(1), 7249. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-63788-w (2020).
Janshen, L., Santuz, A. & Arampatzis, A. “Muscle synergies in patients with multiple sclerosis reveal demand-specific alterations in the modular organization of locomotion,” (in eng). Front. Hum. Neurosci. 14, 593365. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2020.593365 (2020).
Mizuta, N. et al. Merged swing-muscle synergies and their relation to walking characteristics in subacute post-stroke patients: An observational study. PLoS One 17, e0263613. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0263613 (2022).
Cheung, V. C. K. et al. Muscle synergy patterns as physiological markers of motor cortical damage. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 109, 14656. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1212056109 (2012).
Allen, J. L., Kesar, T. M. & Ting, L. H. Motor module generalization across balance and walking is impaired after stroke. J. Neurophysiol. 122, 289. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00561.2018 (2019).
Ivanenko, Y. P. et al. Changes in the spinal segmental motor output for stepping during development from infant to adult. J. Neurosci. 33, 36a. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2722-12.2013 (2013).
Cheung, V. C. K. et al. Plasticity of muscle synergies through fractionation and merging during development and training of human runners. Nat. Commun. 11(1), 4356. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-18210-4 (2020).
Wu, T. & Hallett, M. The influence of normal human ageing on automatic movements. J. Physiol. 562, 615. https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.2004.076042 (2005).
Lin, C.-H. et al. Age related differences in the neural substrates of motor sequence learning after interleaved and repetitive practice. Neuroimage 62, 2020. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.05.015 (2012).
Daselaar, S. M., Rombouts, S. A. R. B., Veltman, D. J., Raaijmakers, J. G. W. & Jonker, C. Similar network activated by young and old adults during the acquisition of a motor sequence. Neurobiol. Aging 24, 1019. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0197-4580(03)00030-7 (2003).
Sammon, J. W. A nonlinear mapping for data structure analysis. IEEE Trans. Comput. C–18, 409. https://doi.org/10.1109/T-C.1969.222678 (1969).
Acknowledgements
We thank Francois Hug for the fruitful discussion related to our motor control projects and his feedback on our data analysis. Open access funding provided by University of Vienna.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
P. K.: Conceived and designed research, performed experiments, Analyzed data, Interpreted results of experiments, Prepared figures, Drafted manuscript, Approved final version of manuscript. W. K.: Conceived and designed research, Edited and revised manuscript, Approved final version of manuscript. B. G.: performed experiments, Interpreted results of experiments, Approved final version of manuscript. E. W.: Conceived and designed research, Performed experiments, Approved final version of manuscript. A. B.: Conceived and designed research, Approved final version of manuscript. H. K.: Conceived and designed research, Performed experiments, Interpreted results of experiments, Edited and revised manuscript, Approved final version of manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
We declare that the authors have no competing interests as defined by Nature Research, or other interests that might be perceived to influence the results and/or discussion reported in this paper.
Additional information
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Kaufmann, P., Koller, W., Wallnöfer, E. et al. Increased trial-to-trial similarity and reduced temporal overlap of muscle synergy activation coefficients manifest during learning and with increasing movement proficiency. Sci Rep 14, 17638 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-68515-3
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-68515-3