Time perception is inherently subjective and malleable. We experience a wide range of time scales, from less than a second to decades. In addition, our perception of time can be affected by our attentional and emotional states. Previous psychological and neuroimaging studies have used several paradigms and methods to probe factors that influence time perception. Considering these factors facilitates approaches to improve time management and to enhance sensory experiences. This Collection of time perception studies includes reports that focus on stimulus property, physiological state, cross-modal interaction, attention, learning, age, and environment. These findings help to illuminate the complex mechanisms of time perception.
Humans have no absolute sense of time. Time perception is fundamentally subjective and depends on one's experiences and circumstances. Moments of excitement and joy can seem dizzyingly faster, whereas moments of boredom and stress can feel interminable1, illustrating how attentional and emotional states affect time perception. Also, time perception has critical effects on many cognitive abilities and motor skills. For instance, we can play the piano with quick movements. Accurate temporal and rhythmic performance are important not only for playing music, but also for multisensory perception, language, and motor planning2,3. In addition, we may vividly recall memories from years ago. We have a broad range of time scales4. These timings appear to be underpinned by different neural mechanisms5. The brain navigates and processes time ranges from subsecond to year, highlighting its remarkable adaptability and complexity.
The more often we pay attention to the passage of time, the longer we perceive time to be6. Our perception of the passage of time may vary as a function of age and education7 or mood state8. This is probably consistent with the contextual-change hypothesis that the perceived duration of an event is affected by the number of contextual changes9. Extending this idea may explain how different age groups perceive time differently. For boys and girls, holiday adventures are hard to come by. Adults have many routine activities and time seems to pass at an accelerated pace. Relative to adults, children may use heuristic methods for duration estimation10. However, it should be noted that feeling the passage of time and estimating duration may employ different mechanisms of time perception11.
A simple explanation for the perceived compression and expansion of time is the event-density hypothesis. This postulates that the number of events occurring during a certain period affects perception of time intervals12, assuming that the “internal clock” counts at a constant rhythm13. Directing attention to salient stimuli or engaging in complex tasks increases internal pulses, i.e., the density of events, resulting in the perception that time is passing quickly6. This hypothesis is consistent with the idea that cellular metabolism and the internal clock are intimately interconnected. An early study argued that as body temperature increases, the internal clock seems to advance faster, leading to the perception of shorter durations14. Cognitive components, such as working memory and attention, were incorporated into the pulse-generating pacemaker and developed into the scalar expectancy model15 and the attentional-gate model16.
Time perception depends not only on endogenous factors, such as attentional, motivational, and physiological levels17,18, but also on exogenous factors, such as speed of motion, stimulus complexity19, salience of visual stimulus features20, and spatial, temporal, social context21 or environment22. Previous studies have frequently employed experimental paradigms such as temporal order or duration judgements to assess time perception of short intervals. In such paradigms, a novel or “oddball” stimulus is perceived as longer in duration than repeated or “standard” stimuli23. The first visual stimulus in a train appears to be perceived as longer than successive stimuli24. However, such a phenomenon does not occur in relation to auditory stimuli. There is a consensus that timing of subsecond intervals is supported by distributed sensory-specific mechanisms25,26. An event-related potential (ERP) study demonstrated that people with normal hearing, but not deaf individuals, show a strong ERP response to visual stimuli in temporal areas during a time-bisection task, whereas the same response is not elicited during a space-bisection task27 (see also a study of developmental viewpoint28). Gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA) levels in the human visual cortex measured using magnetic resonance spectroscopy appear to correlate with perceived durations of visual intervals, suggesting that the GABAergic system contributes to individual differences in time perception29. However, time perception studies in this Collection have found that learning of temporal interval discrimination transfers between auditory and visual modalities30 and that cross-modal correspondence between auditory pitch and visual elevation selectively affects temporal recalibration31.
There is no single sensory organ responsible for time perception. Different brain regions are involved in temporal processing depending on time scales. Subsecond time intervals are mainly processed in the cerebellum32, whereas temporal processing in the range of seconds and minutes is supported by the prefrontal cortex and striatum33,34. In addition, time perception is impaired in disorders of the precuneus/posterior cingulate gyrus35 and supramarginal gyrus36. In particular, the precuneus may contribute to our sense of “presentness”, providing the “now” in the passage of time37,38.
The advent of digital technology has had an unprecedented impact on time perception. Ubiquitous access to the Internet facilitates instantaneous information retrieval and synchronous communication. A consequence of this persistent connectivity is the potential for information overload, such that the sense of time tends to become ambiguous. The widespread prevalence of social media notifications may contribute to the perceived acceleration of time. However, through flow and meditation states, alternative perceptions of time can be experienced. Specifically, a flow experience is a symbolic phenomenon of time distortion, in which one forgets the passage of time by immersing oneself in a certain activity. People in a flow state often report this state as being “in the zone”39. Although there are anecdotal reports of flow experiences by athletes, few studies have captured quantitative aspects of flow states40. However, some studies have identified flow states in terms of attentional fluctuations41,42. Using such methods, it may be possible to overcome methodological difficulties and to measure altered time perception.
Articles in this Collection show that the interplay of stimulus property, physiological state, attention, age, and environment fundamentally shapes individual temporal experiences. A deep understanding of these factors is undoubtedly crucial to the ongoing field of time perception research.
References
James, W. The Principles of Psychology (Henry Holt and Company, 1890).
Kaya, E. & Henry, M. J. Reliable estimation of internal oscillator properties from a novel, fast-paced tapping paradigm. Sci. Rep. 12, 20466. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-24453-6 (2022).
Li, L., Yotsumoto, Y. & Hayashi, M. J. Temporal perceptual learning distinguishes between empty and filled intervals. Sci. Rep. 12, 9824. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-13814-w (2022).
Eagleman, D. M. Human time perception and its illusions. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 18, 131–136. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2008.06.002 (2008).
Wittmann, M. & Paulus, M. P. Decision making, impulsivity and time perception. Trends Cogn. Sci. 12, 7–12. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2007.10.004 (2008).
Martinelli, N. & Droit-Volet, S. What factors underlie our experience of the passage of time? Theoretical consequences. Psychol. Res. 86, 522–530. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-021-01486-6 (2022).
Wittmann, M. & Lehnhoff, S. Age effects in perception of time. Psychol. Rep. 97, 921–935. https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.97.3.921-935 (2005).
Buzi, G., Eustache, F., D’Argembeau, A. & Hinault, T. The role of depressive symptoms in the interplay between aging and temporal processing. Sci. Rep. 13, 11375. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-023-38500-3 (2023).
Block, R. A. & Reed, M. A. Remembered duration: Evidence for a contextual-change hypothesis. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Learn. 4, 656–665. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.4.6.656 (1978).
Stojić, S., Topić, V. & Nadasdy, Z. Children and adults rely on different heuristics for estimation of durations. Sci. Rep. 13, 1077. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-023-27419-4 (2023).
Lamprou-Kokolaki, M., Nédélec, Y., Lhuillier, S. & van Wassenhove, V. Distinctive features of experiential time: Duration, speed and event density. Conscious. Cogn. 118, 103635. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2024.103635 (2024).
Poynter, D. In [Adavances in Psychology] Time and Human Cognition: A Life-Span Perspective Vol. 59 (eds Levin, I. & Zakay, D.) Ch. 8, 305–331 (Elsevier, 1989).
Treisman, M. Temporal discrimination and the indifference interval. Implications for a model of the “internal clock”. Psychol. Monogr. 77, 1–31. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0093864 (1963).
Hoagland, H. The physiological control of judgments of duration: Evidence for a chemical clock. J. Gen. Psychol. 9, 267–287. https://doi.org/10.1080/00221309.1933.9920937 (1933).
Gibbon, J., Church, R. M. & Meck, W. H. Scalar timing in memory. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 423, 52–77. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.1984.tb23417.x (1984).
Zakay, D. & Block, R. A. Temporal cognition. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 6, 12–16. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.ep11512604 (1997).
Sabat, M., Haładus, B., Klincewicz, M. & Nalepa, G. J. Cognitive load, fatigue and aversive simulator symptoms but not manipulated zeitgebers affect duration perception in virtual reality. Sci. Rep. 12, 15689. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-18520-1 (2022).
Polgári, P., Jovanovic, L., van Wassenhove, V. & Giersch, A. The processing of subthreshold visual temporal order is transitory and motivation-dependent. Sci. Rep. 13, 7699. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-023-34392-5 (2023).
Kovarski, K. et al. Movie editing influences spectators’ time perception. Sci. Rep. 12, 20084. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-23992-2 (2022).
Ziat, M., Saoud, W., Prychitko, S., Servos, P. & Grondin, S. Malleability of time through progress bars and throbbers. Sci. Rep. 12, 10400. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-14649-1 (2022).
Boned, J. & López-Moliner, J. Duration judgments are mediated by the similarity with the temporal context. Sci. Rep. 12, 22575. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-27168-w (2022).
Jording, M., Vogel, D. H. V., Viswanathan, S. & Vogeley, K. Dissociating passage and duration of time experiences through the intensity of ongoing visual change. Sci. Rep. 12, 8226. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-12063-1 (2022).
Eagleman, D. M. & Pariyadath, V. Is subjective duration a signature of coding efficiency? Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B. 364, 1841–1851. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2009.0026 (2009).
Rose, D. & Summers, J. Duration illusions in a train of visual stimuli. Perception 24, 1177–1187. https://doi.org/10.1068/p241177 (1995).
Grondin, S. Timing and time perception: A review of recent behavioral and neuroscience findings and theoretical directions. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 72, 561–582. https://doi.org/10.3758/app.72.3.561 (2010).
Mauk, M. D. & Buonomano, D. V. The neural basis of temporal processing. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 27, 307–340 (2004).
Gori, M., Amadeo, M. B., Pavani, F., Valzolgher, C. & Campus, C. Temporal visual representation elicits early auditory-like responses in hearing but not in deaf individuals. Sci. Rep. 12, 19036. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-22224-x (2022).
Polver, S., Háden, G. P., Bulf, H., Winkler, I. & Tóth, B. Early maturation of sound duration processing in the infant’s brain. Sci. Rep. 13, 10287. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-023-36794-x (2023).
Terhune, D. B., Russo, S., Near, J., Stagg, C. J. & Kadosh, R. C. GABA predicts time perception. J. Neurosci. 34, 4364–4370. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.3972-13.2014 (2014).
Xiong, Y. Z., Guan, S. C. & Yu, C. A supramodal and conceptual representation of subsecond time revealed with perceptual learning of temporal interval discrimination. Sci. Rep. 12, 10668. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-14698-6 (2022).
Uno, K. & Yokosawa, K. Cross-modal correspondence between auditory pitch and visual elevation modulates audiovisual temporal recalibration. Sci. Rep. 12, 21308. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-25614-3 (2022).
Koch, G., Oliveri, M. & Caltagirone, C. Neural networks engaged in milliseconds and seconds time processing: Evidence from transcranial magnetic stimulation and patients with cortical or subcortical dysfunction. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B. 364, 1907–1918. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2009.0018 (2009).
Lalonde, R. & Hannequin, D. The neurobiological basis of time estimation and temporal order. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 151–173. https://doi.org/10.1515/revneuro.1999.10.2.151 (1999).
Allman, M. J. & Meck, W. H. Pathophysiological distortions in time perception and timed performance. Brain 135, 656–677. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awr210 (2012).
Critchley, M. The Parietal Lobes (Hafner Press, 1953).
Pacella, V. et al. Temporal judgments of actions following unilateral brain damage. Sci. Rep. 12, 21668. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-26070-9 (2022).
Peer, M., Salomon, R., Goldberg, I., Blanke, O. & Arzy, S. Brain system for mental orientation in space, time, and person. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 112, 11072–11077. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1504242112 (2015).
Tang, L. et al. Neural correlates of temporal presentness in the precuneus: A cross-linguistic fMRI study based on speech stimuli. Cereb. Cortex 31, 1538–1552. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhaa307 (2021).
Csikszentmihalyi, M. Flow: The Psychology of Optimal Experience (HarperCollins, 1990).
Hancock, P. A. et al. A meta-analysis of flow effects and the perception of time. Acta Psychol. 198, 102836. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2019.04.007 (2019).
Esterman, M., Noonan, S. K., Rosenberg, M. & Degutis, J. In the zone or zoning out? Tracking behavioral and neural fluctuations during sustained attention. Cereb. Cortex 23, 2712–2723. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhs261 (2013).
Terashima, H., Kihara, K., Kawahara, J. I. & Kondo, H. M. Common principles underlie the fluctuation of auditory and visual sustained attention. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 74, 705–715. https://doi.org/10.1177/1747021820972255 (2021).
Funding
HMK was supported by the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science KAKENHI grants (nos. 20H01789 and 22K18659).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
HMK: writing–original draft, writing–review and editing; EG: writing–review and editing; APP: writing–review and editing. All authors gave final approval for publication.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Kondo, H.M., Gheorghiu, E. & Pinheiro, A.P. Malleability and fluidity of time perception. Sci Rep 14, 12244 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-62189-7
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-62189-7