Abstract
Hand stencils are a remarkable graphic expression in Prehistoric rock art, dating back to 42 ka BP. Although these stencils provide direct impressions of the artists’ hands, the characterization of their biological profile (i.e., biological sex and age) is very challenging. Previous studies have attempted this analysis with traditional morphometrics (TM), whereas little research has been undertaken using Geometric Morphometrics (GM), a method widely used in other disciplines but only tentatively employed in rock art studies. However, the large variation in relative finger position in archaeological hands poses the question of whether these representations can be examined through GM, or, in contrast, if this creates an unmanageable error in the results. To address this issue, a 2D hand scans sample of 70 living individuals (F = 35; M = 35) has been collected in three standardized positions (n = 210) and digitized with 32 2D conventional landmarks. Results show that the intra-individual distance (mean Procrustes distance between Pos. 1–2 = 0.132; 2–3 = 0.191; 1–3 = 0.292) is larger than the inter-individual distance (mean in 1 = 0.122; 2 = 0.142; 3 = 0.165). Finally, it has been demonstrated that the relative finger positions, as well as the inclusion of all hand parts in the analysis, have an overshadowing effect on other variables potentially involved in the morphometric variability of the hand, such as biological sex.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
Rock art is one of the earliest visual expressions in humankind, dating back (at least) to as early as 45.500 years BP, on the Indonesian island of Sulawesi1. The artistic corpus is varied, including the depiction of animals, hunting scenes, hand stencils, anthropomorphic figures and symbols. Altogether, such artistic expressions provide critical cultural, environmental, and subsistence insights into the past peoples that created them.
Hand stencils are a recurrent and ubiquitous rock art expression with the earliest examples found in the caves on Sulawesi with documented minimum U-Th dates of 37.2 ka BP for Lubang Jeriji Saléh2, and 32.29 ± 0.24/39.67 ± 0.32/32.60 ± 0.76 for Leang Balangajia 1, Leang Jarie and Leang Sampeang respectively3, among others. Further early examples are Castillo Cave (Spain) with a date of 37.63 ± 0.34 ka BP (minimum age)4 and Maltravieso (Spain) with 41.68 + 2.44/ − 2.29 ka cal BP and 70.08 + 3.82/ − 3.37 ka cal BP5.
In some cases, hand images are positive painted impressions of hands held directly against the cave surface. In other cases, pigment was blown over the hand and adhered to the supporting rock, thus resulting in a negative impression of the hand or ‘stencil’. In either case, hand images provide a proxy of the morphology of the hand of the “model”.
Modern human hand form and function have been thoroughly studied in different fields and with multiple aims, including work/ergonomics6,7,8,9,10,11, sports12,13, clinical14,15, forensic16,17 and personal identification18,19,20,21, biometry21,22,23, paleoanthropology, archaeology24,25,26,27,28,29,30,31 and other fields.
For biological sex30,32,33 and age 27,30,32,34 estimation from hand morphology, the most common approach has been conventional morphometrics, i.e. the study of form based on linear measurements, angles, ratios, etc6,8,11,35,36,37. Conventional morphometric approaches to archaeological hand stencils have consistently used different linear measurements and indexes (mostly ‘Manning Index’38,39, which is based on a sexually-based differentiation in the length of the 2nd and 4th digits due to fetal exposure to hormones) to estimate the biological sex31,40,41 of the individuals who left these images on the cave walls.
Despite the indisputable relevance of such studies, conventional morphometrics are affected by some limitations that led to the development of other morphometric approaches, such as Geometric Morphometrics (GM)42,43. GM is now a standard tool to examine morphology and its covariance with other underlying variables in multiple fields of research, such as biology, entomology, anatomy, biomechanics, palaeoanthropology, and bioanthropology44,45,46,47,48,49. In spite of its potential, few studies have actually used GM to infer the biological sex or/and age of living humans from their own hand morphology. The few studies that have employed this approach use a more or less standardized position50,51,52 and none of them has explored the actual archaeological record to examine the relevance of variations in relative finger positions on ensuing analyses.
A meticulous analysis of the archaeological record reveals a considerable variability in hand positions on the wall, ranging from fully extended hands to nearly closed ones, with all intermediate configurations represented (see Fig. 1).Such variation in relative finger position may obscure an assessment of morphological differences between individuals because GM uses the spatial relationship between Landmark (LM) coordinates44,53,54,55. Thus, changing the position of fingers may induce changes in LM configurations that are larger than differences between individuals. Indeed, the potential problems that this archaeological variability could pose have not been formally addressed before51.
This present study investigates the statistical effect of the variability in relative hand position when conducting a GM study as well as identifying and describing the associated factors, with the ultimate objective of characterizing archaeological blown hand stencils. For that, we test the null hypothesis (H0), which states there are no significant morphological differences between different hand positions and those between subjects.
Materials and methods
Materials
2D left-hand scans were collected from 70 living adults of known biological sex and age (balanced sex sample of 35 biological females and 35 biological males; > 20 years of age, following Bogin & Smith, 199656). The hands were digitized together with a scale using an HP Officejet Pro 8600 Plus contact scanner, which provides 300 dpi scans in jpeg format.
A scanning protocol established three different standardized hand positions (Fig. 2):
-
(1)
“Closed hand”, i.e., fingers fully extended but as close as possible (adducted) although not touching;
-
(2)
“Natural position”, i.e., fingers fully extended and semi-spread apart (abducted);
-
(3)
“Fully open”, fingers fully extended and abducted.
A scan of each position was obtained for all the participants, totalling three scans per individual. Thus, 210 (3 × 70) images were acquired.
None of the participants suffered from any pathologies affecting the limbs, amputations or deformities of the fingers and/or hands. Only left hands were scanned in the study, as this laterality is documented more in the archaeological record than right hands (L = ~ 70%; R = ~ 30%).
This experiment has been approved by the Ethics Committee of Cantabria University (Cod. CE Tesis 03/2021) following all the values and regulations for data protection and anonymity. In turn, we confirm that all steps of the experimental process have been carried out in accordance with the relevant guidelines and regulations. Finally, we confirm that informed consent was obtained from all subjects and/or their legal guardian(s) for the collection, processing and publication of the data, as well as that no other personal information has been collected but the strictly necessary for the study (biological sex and age).
Methods
Thirty-two 2D conventional landmarks (LM) were acquired from the digital images of each digitization. Data were collected directly from the hand scans using TPSdig software. The LMs were located in the main anatomical reference points of the hand and enable a detailed size and shape morphological analysis of the hand (Fig. 2, Table 1). To that end, each scan was loaded into TPSdig2®42 and the corresponding LM coordinates were collected from each stencil image. The 2D landmark system can show sufficiently strong differentiation to test the null hypothesis (H0) and serves as a basis for a future study on parietal art which, given its nature, precludes a 360-degree view. All LM coordinates were then submitted to a Generalized Procrustes Analysis to remove the effects of translation, rotation, and scaling from the raw coordinates and to produce size and shape variables55.
First, Shapiro–Wilk test was run to test the normality of the dataset. Resulting value concludes no normality data (p < 0.05), so subsequent tests have therefore been adapted to the nature of the data. Subsequently, Centroid Size (CS), defined as the square root of the sum of squared distances of all landmarks from their centroid55, was employed to assess the impact of hand position on object size. Subsequently, centroid size was measured and categorized based on hand position (as described above). To further explore the intricacies of size and its correlation with shape, an allometric study57,58 was conducted. A multivariate regression analysis was employed with shape (represented by Procrustean coordinates) as the dependent variable and size (Centroid Size) as the independent one with 100 permutations.
Afterwards, Procrustes distances between individuals were computed for each position, representing inter-individual distances. These were then compared to Procrustes distances of the same individuals but with different hand positions, representing intra-individual differences. The results were organized based on hand position and subsequently visualized using boxplots. If the observed differences between contrasting intra-individual hand positions are larger than those between individuals, it suggests that the former has a more substantial impact on the morphological analysis. This, in turn, hinders the comparison of hand stencils in different positions.
Additionally, we conducted two distinct Principal Component Analyses (PCAs) to scrutinize both the data and explanatory variables of shape, stratifying the samples by their biological sex. The initial PCA was based on the variance–covariance matrix after a Procrustes superimposition. Our examination focused on the first two Principal Components, guided by the scree plot to identify the components with the most substantial variance explanation (Supp. Fig. 1).
In turn, a second Principal Component Analysis was performed on the residual data from first PCA derived from a dummy regression between Procrustes coordinates and position of the covariate (covariate: Position 1 = -1; Position 2 = 0; Position 3 = + 1). This procedure aims to mitigate the influence of relative finger position as this factor has been demonstrated to contribute a significant percentage of variability, potentially obscuring other influential factors.
Due to the non-normal distribution of the data and the repetitive nature of cases, the Friedman test was employed to examine potential differences in both size (evaluated through centroid size) and shape (evaluated through PC scores) among groups, defined by finger position. This statistical analysis was conducted using Past59, with subsequent pairwise Wilcoxon post-hoc tests for detailed comparisons between groups.
Finally, it’s crucial to acknowledge that various variables such as age, biological sex, or musculature may potentially influence hand shape, and their impact might be obscured by the high percentage explained by hand movement. In-depth studies on biological sex and ontogeny variables have been extensively explored where each element of the hand was individually examined60. These studies revealed distinct results in biological sex differences observed from childhood onwards (+ 7 years) in terms of size and from youth onwards in terms of shape (+ 10/12 years). Consequently, we have chosen to briefly explore the variable of biological sex in this work to illustrate the trends that may be obscured by the dominant influence of hand movement. To test whether the difference between biological males and females is statistically significant, the Friedman two-sample test was run in all mentioned statistical procedures, both Centroid Size data and PCA Scores.
The intra-observer error study was performed by taking five measurements of the same scan61. The intra-observer error was accepted after testing that the largest Procrustes distance within the measurements of the same individual (0.013819) is smaller than the smallest Procrustes distance between different individuals (0.020328)61.
GM data collection and analyses in this study were performed in tpsDig, tpsRelw, and MorphoJ, which are freely available at the SUNY Stony Brook Morphometrics website (http://life.bio.sunysb.edu/morph/). Statistical testing was undertaken in MorphoJ and Past59.
Results
The results reveal a clear size distinction associated with hand position. Specifically, Centroid Size (CS) shows an incremental pattern from Position 1 to Position 3 (mean of Pos. 1 = 30.52, Pos. 2 = 32.98, and Pos. 3 = 36.93), correlating with finger abduction (refer to Fig. 3). The Friedman test indicates a statistically significant difference among their medians (p < 3.694E − 28). Additionally, post-hoc Wilcoxon pairwise tests demonstrate a significant size difference between all groups (P1-P2 p = 5.3188E − 12; P2-P3 p = 5.1041E − 12; P1-P3 p = 3.5595E − 13).
Regarding allometry, results indicate a predicted percentage of 40.46%, which implies that a significant portion of the shape is explained by size (Fig. 4). The regression scores exhibit a significant difference among the three groups according to the Friedman test and Wilcoxon pairwise comparisons. As supported by Fig. 4, individuals' shapes are strongly correlated with their size changes. The analysis reveals that hands with a smaller Centroid Size correspond to closed hands, while larger Centroid Sizes correspond to open hands. Individuals are therefore organized based on their degree of openness: 1, 2, or 3.
Figure 5 shows that inter-individual Procrustes distances are consistent across hand positions and lower than intra-individual differences. The latter are lowest between hand positions 1 and 2 (i.e., between adducted and semi-abducted fingers) and largest between positions 1 and 3 (i.e., between adducted and fully abducted fingers). This implies that the statistical difference within the same hand (intra-individual average Procrustes distance: Position 1–2 = 0.137; Position 2–3 = 0.191; Position 1–3 = 0.292) is greater than the difference existing between hands of different individuals (inter-subject average Procrustes distance: 1 = 0.122; 2 = 0.142; 3 = 0.165).
Additionally, we perform a PCA on the covariance matrix after a Procrustes superimposition. In this first PCA, we observe the individual arrangement of groups from left to right, associated with decreasing to increasing finger openness (i.e., abduction), as shown by the wireframes (Fig. 6). Notably, 82.86% of the variance is elucidated by the first two principal components. The Friedman test indicates a significant difference among the medians of the three positions in both PC1 and PC2 (p = 7.1694E-30 and 0.0015). Furthermore, Wilcoxon pairwise tests disclose significant differences between all groups in PC1 (p < 0.01). However, in PC2, no significant differences are discerned between Position 2 and Position 3 (p = 0.1056).
Then trough a second Principal Component Analysis (Fig. 7), run on resulted data from a dummy regression on the residuals data from the first principal component analysis, we determine that 58.13% of the variance in shape can be explained by movement. The Friedman test indicates significant differences among the medians of the three groups in PC1 (p = 1.5255E − 05). Finally, according to Wilcoxon pairwise tests, positions 1 and 3 are not distinguishable in PC1 (p = 0,692), and none of the three positions are distinguishable from each other PC2 (p = 0.88568). As it can be slightly noticed in the wireframes even after removing the effect of the finger extension category (1, 2, 3), PC1 and PC2 are not completely free from its influence.
Finally, in relation to biological sex, through Friedman two-sample test, statistical differentiation in size (Centroid Size) has been identified, both in the analysis of the overall sample and in each position. On the contrary, regarding the aspect of shape no statistical differences were observed between males and females in either of the two Principal Component Analyses (PC Scores in PC1 and PC2), whether examining all positions collectively or individually within each position.
Conclusion and discussion
This article explores the application of Geometric Morphometrics (GM) in the analysis of prehistoric hand stencils, highlighting its potential in a relatively unexplored field within rock art studies. Palaeolithic hand stencils corpus reveals significant variability in hand positions in rock art, ranging from fully extended hands to partially closed ones. This variability poses challenges for morphometrics and underscores the need to formally address it. Through this work it is demonstrated that relative finger positions variable affects significantly the results and should be specifically addressed. So that, the null hypothesis (H0) proposed at the beginning of the study is rejected as the results show that the difference between hands of the same individual in its three positions is, in fact, greater than that between the hands of different individuals in each of the positions.
On one hand, results reveal a significant size distinction associated with hand position, indicating a predictable relationship between finger abduction and centroid size. The regression of Procrustes Coordinates against Centroid Size reinforces the substantial percentage of shape explained by size, with hands of smaller centroid sizes corresponding to closed positions and larger ones to open positions. Thus, depending on the questions, centroid size may not represent the best option when studying the parameter of size under conditions comparable to those of this study. On the other, Procrustes distances and PCA analyses highlight the substantial influence of relative finger positions on morphology. PCA results emphasized the influence of hand movement on shape variation, concluding 58.13% of the shape variation could be explained by relative finger position.
Regarding biological sex, it has been documented a statistical differentiation in size (Centroid Size), both in the analysis of the overall sample and in each position, where on shape no statistical differences were observed between males and females in either of the two Principal Component Analyses (PC Scores in PC1 and PC2). These results contradict those obtained in Fernández-Navarro et al. 202460 where the difference in size between biological males and females is statistically discernible from ages 3–7 and in shape from ages 7–13.
On the other hand, the efficacy of the palm has been previously pointed out and discussed, arguing that it is the most appropriate element for the evaluation of sex through human hands62,50, especially since it is not as exposed to changes in hand posture as are the fingers52. This parameter, along with others related to sexual dimorphism of the hands, will be analysed in a dedicated article60.
We can conclude that the combined analysis of hand elements is masking other potential variables, such as sexual dimorphism. So, results in this paper do not imply a real absence of differentiation between the biological sexes. Instead, it suggests that, given the inclusive nature of the hand elements in the current sample, any such distinctions are not visual and statistical apparent.
This highlights the inaccuracies in classifying and characterizing an individual through hand scanning if the variable of movement/positioning is not homogenized beforehand or, alternatively, extracted through the independent analysis of its elements51,52. Thus, the establishment of a standardized system in data collection or the analysis of independent elements should be considered when examining previous studies in a wide variety of disciplines in which the object of study is composed of moveable structures60. In short, this article is a first step towards the correct use of GM in contexts related to archaeological materials, especially those containing postural information which, as we have shown, has a decisive influence on any biometric approach.
Our results showing the meaningful impact of hand/finger position are thus tracking first, are foremost, movement, and so are consistent with previous research using GM to examine motion of, e.g., fish feeding systems63,64, human gape cycle65, heart cycle66, posture and limbs67 or breathing68,69. In such studies, sequential postural changes are used to assess motion trajectories in multivariate shape space68.
Finally, we would like to highlight the experimental nature of this work. This data cannot be directly extrapolated to archaeological contexts and conditions; instead, it serves as an experimental study guiding the design of data collection and the corresponding analysis framework. This clarification will help ensure a comprehensive understanding of the study's scope and its applicability to real hand findings within the context of Paleolithic art.
Data availability
Corresponding raw-measurement dataset is not publicly available due to personal privacy reasons but are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request. Additionally, a comprehensive supplementary information file collecting summary statistical description/information generated by the sample analysis is included.
References
Aubert, M. et al. Pleistocene cave art from Sulawesi, Indonesia. Nature 514, 223–227 (2014).
Aubert, M. et al. Palaeolithic cave art in Borneo. Nature 564, 254–257 (2018).
Brumm, A. et al. Oldest cave art found in Sulawesi. Sci. Adv. https://doi.org/10.1126/sciadv.abd4648 (2021).
García-Díez, M. et al. The chronology of hand stencils in European Palaeolithic rock art: Implications of new U-series results from El Castillo Cave (Cantabria, Spain). J. Anthropol. Sci. 93, 1–18 (2015).
Hoffmann, D. L. et al. U-Th dating of carbonate crusts reveals Neandertal origin of Iberian cave art. Science 1979(359), 912–915 (2018).
Davies, B. T., Abada, A., Benson, K., Courtney, A. & Minto, I. A comparison of hand anthropometry of females in three ethnic groups. Ergonomics 23, 179–182 (1980).
Courtney, A. J. & Ng, M. K. Hong Kong female hand dimensions and machine guarding. Ergonomics 27, 187–193 (1984).
Imrhan, S. N., Nguyen, M.-T. & Nguyen, N.-N. Hand anthropometry of Americans of Vietnamese origin. Int. J. Ind. Ergon. 12, 281–287 (1993).
Bolstad, G., Benum, B. & Rokne, A. Anthropometry of Norwegian light industry and office workers. Appl. Ergon. 32, 239–246 (2001).
Nag, A., Nag, P. K. & Desai, H. Hand anthropometry of Indian women. Indian J. Med. Res. 117, 260–269 (2003).
Mandahawi, N., Imrhan, S., Al-Shobaki, S. & Sarder, B. Hand anthropometry survey for the Jordanian population. Int. J. Ind. Ergon. 38, 966–976 (2008).
Barut, C., Tan, Ü. & Dogan, A. Association of height and weight with second to fourth digit ratio (2D:4D) and sex differences. Percept. Mot. Skills 106, 627–632 (2008).
Fallahi, A. & Jadidian, A. The effect of hand dimensions, hand shape and some anthropometric characteristics on handgrip strength in male grip athletes and non-athletes. J. Hum. Kinet. 29, 151–159 (2011).
Kulaksiz, G. & Gözil, R. The effect of hand preference on hand anthropometric measurements in healthy individuals. Ann. Anat. Anatomischer Anzeiger 184, 257–265 (2002).
Boz, C., Ozmenoglu, M., Altunayoglu, V., Velioglu, S. & Alioglu, Z. Individual risk factors for carpal tunnel syndrome: An evaluation of body mass index, wrist index and hand anthropometric measurements. Clin. Neurol. Neurosurg. 106, 294–299 (2004).
Krogman, W. M. & Iscan, M. Y. The Human Skeleton in Forensic Medicine (Springfield, 1986).
Kanchan, T. & Krishan, K. Anthropometry of hand in sex determination of dismembered remains—A review of literature. J. Forensic Leg. Med. 18, 14–17 (2011).
Oden, C., Ercil, A. & Buke, B. Combining implicit polynomials and geometric features for hand recognition. Pattern Recognit. Lett. 24, 2145–2152 (2003).
Zhang, D., Kong, W.-K., You, J. & Wong, M. Online palmprint identification. IEEE Trans. Pattern Anal. Mach. Intell. 25, 1041–1050 (2003).
Bulatov, Y., Jambawalikar, S., Kumar, P. & Sethia, S. Hand Recognition Using Geometric Classifiers 753–759 (Springer, 2004). https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-540-25948-0_102.
Yoruk, E., Konukoglu, E., Sankur, B. & Darbon, J. Shape-based hand recognition. IEEE Trans. Image Process. 15, 1803–1815 (2006).
Sanchez-Reillo, R., Sanchez-Avila, C. & Gonzalez-Marcos, A. Biometric identification through hand geometry measurements. IEEE Trans. Pattern Anal. Mach. Intell. 22, 1168–1171 (2000).
Kondo, M. et al. Sexual dimorphism in the human hand proportion: A radiographic study. Bull. Natl. Museum Nat. Sci. 43, 1–6 (2017).
Sahly, A. Les Mains Mutilées Dans l’art Préhistorique. (Ministère des Affaires Culturelles, Uniersité de Toulouse, Toulouse, 1966).
Flood, J. Rock art of the Koolburra plateau. Rock Art Res. 4, 91–126 (1987).
Gunn, R. G. Aboriginal Rock Art of Victoria. (1987).
Gunn, R. G. Hand sizes in rock art: Interpreting the measurements of hand stencils and prints. Rock Art Res. 23, 97–112 (2006).
Groenen, M. Les représentationsd e mains négatives dans les grottes de Gargase t de Tibiran (Hautes-Pyrénée)s. Bull. Soc. roy. belge Anthrop. Préhist (1988).
Mc Donald, J. Looking for a woman’s touch: Indications of gender in shelter sites in the Sydney Basin. The second australian women in archaeology conference (1995).
Guthrie, R. D. The Nature of Paleolithic Art (University of Chicago Press, 2005).
Snow, D. R. Sexual dimorphism in Upper Palaeolithic hand stencils. Antiquity 80, 390–404 (2006).
Mackie, M. Determining the Age and Sex of Hand Spray Artists, Johnson County, Wyoming. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/323368502 (2018).
Carden, N. & Blanco, R. Measurements and replications of hand stencils. In Paleoart and Materiality (eds Bednarik, R. G. et al.) 129–146 (Archaeopress Publishing Ltd, 2016). https://doi.org/10.2307/j.ctvxrq0ks.12.
Ripoll López, S. & Muñoz Ibañez, F. J. Las representaciones infantiles de manos en el yacimiento de la Fuente del Trucho (Asque–Colungo, Huesca). in III Congrés Internacional d’Història dels Pirineus. Actes i comunicacions 693–798 (Institut d’Estudis Andorrans, Sant Julià de Lòria, 2019).
Agnihotri, A. K., Purwar, B., Jeebun, N. & Agnihotri, S. Determination of sex by hand dimensions. Internet J. Forensic Sci. 1, 12–24 (2005).
Agnihotri, A. K., Agnihotri, S., Jeebun, N. & Googoolye, K. Prediction of stature using hand dimensions. J. For. Leg. Med. 15, 479–482 (2008).
Ibrahim, M. A., Khalifa, A. M., Hagras, A. M. & Alwakid, N. I. Sex determination from hand dimensions and index/ring finger length ratio in North Saudi population: Medico-legal view. Egypt. J. For. Sci. 6, 435–444 (2016).
Baker, F. Anthropological notes on the human hand. Am. Anthropol. 1, 51–76 (1888).
Nelson, E., Manning, J. & Sinclair, A. News Using the length of the 2nd to 4th digit ratio (2D:4D) to sex cave art hand stencils: Factors to consider. Before Farm. 2006, 1–7 (2006).
Snow, D. Sexual dimorphism in European upper paleolithic cave art. Am. Antiq. 78, 746–761 (2013).
Rabazo-Rodríguez, A. M., Modesto-Mata, M., Bermejo, L. & García-Díez, M. New data on the sexual dimorphism of the hand stencils in El Castillo Cave (Cantabria, Spain). J. Archaeol. Sci. Rep. 14, 374–381 (2017).
Rohlf, F. J. tpsDig, Digitize Landmarks and Outlines. (2006).
Adams, D. C., Rohlf, F. J. & Slice, D. E. Geometric morphometrics: Ten years of progress following the ‘revolution’. Ital. J. Zool. 71, 5–16 (2004).
O’Higgins, P. The study of morphological variation in the hominid fossil record: Biology, landmarks and geometry. J. Anat. 197, 103–120 (2000).
Mitteroecker, P. & Gunz, P. Advances in geometric morphometrics. Evol. Biol. 36, 235–247 (2009).
O’Higgins, P., Fitton, L. C. & Godinho, R. M. Geometric morphometrics and finite elements analysis: Assessing the functional implications of differences in craniofacial form in the hominin fossil record. J. Archaeol. Sci. 101, 159–168 (2019).
Hugo, A. Sexual dimorphism using geometric morphometric approach. In Sexual Dimorphism (ed. Moriyama, H.) (InTech, 2013). https://doi.org/10.5772/55195.
Toro Ibacache, M. V., Manriquez Soto, G. & Suazo Galdames, I. Morfometría Geométrica y el Estudio de las Formas Biológicas: De la Morfología Descriptiva a la Morfología Cuantitativa. Int. J. Morphol. 28, 977–990 (2010).
García-Martínez, D. et al. Three-dimensional analysis of sexual dimorphism in ribcage kinematics of modern humans. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 169, 348–355 (2019).
Sanfilippo, P. G., Hewitt, A. W., Mountain, J. A. & Mackey, D. A. A geometric morphometric assessment of hand shape and comparison to the 2D:4D digit ratio as a marker of sexual dimorphism. Twin Res. Hum. Genet. 16, 590–600 (2013).
Králík, M., Katina, S. & Urbanová, P. Distal part of the human hand: Study of form variability and sexual dimorphism using geometric morphometrics. Anthropologia Integra 5, 7–25 (2014).
Nelson, E., Hall, J., Randolph-Quinney, P. & Sinclair, A. Beyond size: The potential of a geometric morphometric analysis of shape and form for the assessment of sex in hand stencils in rock art. J. Archaeol. Sci. 78, 202–213 (2017).
Editor, B., Fiore, D., Basile, M. & Kumar, G. Approach for the Estimation of the Individuals ’ Age and Sex Measurements and Replications of Hand Stencils: a Methodological Approach for the Estimation of the Individuals ’ Age and Sex Natalia Carden Rocío Blanco Mediciones y réplicas de improntas de m.
Dryden, I. L. & Mardia, K. V. Statistical Shape Analysis (Wiley, 1998).
Zelditch, M. L., Swiderski, D. L., Sheets, H. D. & Fink, W. L. Geometric Morphometric for Biologist: A Primer (Elsevier, 2012). https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-012778460-1/50003-X.
Bogin, B. & Smith, B. H. Evolution of the human life cycle. Am. J. Hum. Biol. 8, 703–716 (1996).
Mosimann, J. E. Size allometry: Size and shape variables with characterizations of the lognormal and generalized gamma distributions. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 65, 930–945 (1970).
Gould, S. J. Allometry and size in ontogeny and phylogeny. Biol. Rev. 41, 587–640 (1966).
Hammer, Ø., Harper, D. A. T. & Ryan, P. D. PAST: Paleontological statistics software package for education and data analysis. Palaeontologia Electronica 4, 9 (2001).
Fernandez-Navarro, V., Garate, D. & García Martínez, D. Ontogeny and sexual dimorphism in the human hands through a 2D geometric morphometrics approach. Under Rev. (2024).
Rosas, A. et al. Neandertal talus bones from El Sidrón site (Asturias, Spain): A 3D geometric morphometrics analysis. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 164, 394–415 (2017).
Kanchan, T. & Rastogi, P. Sex determination from hand dimensions of north and south Indians. J. For. Sci. 54, 546–550 (2009).
Martinez, C. M., McGee, M. D., Borstein, S. R. & Wainwright, P. C. Feeding ecology underlies the evolution of cichlid jaw mobility. Evolution (N. Y.) 72, 1645–1655 (2018).
Martinez, C. M. & Wainwright, P. C. Extending the geometric approach for studying biomechanical motions. Integr. Comp. Biol. 59, 684–695 (2019).
Laird, M. F., Ross, C. F. & O’Higgins, P. Jaw kinematics and mandibular morphology in humans. J. Hum. Evol. 139, 102639 (2020).
Piras, P. et al. 4D-analysis of left ventricular heart cycle using procrustes motion analysis. PLoS One 9, e86896 (2014).
Adams, D. C. & Cerney, M. M. Quantifying biomechanical motion using Procrustes motion analysis. J. Biomech. 40, 437–444 (2007).
Bastir, M. et al. The evolution, form and function of the human respiratory system. J. Anthropol. Sci. 100, 141–172 (2022).
Gómez-Recio, M. et al. Geometric morphometrics of 4D motion data: A proof of concept for applications in palaeoanthropology. Span. J. Palaeontol. https://doi.org/10.7203/sjp.25648 (2022).
Acknowledgements
The authors would like to express their gratitude to the Interdisciplinary Center for Archaeology and the Evolution of Human Behaviour (ICArEHB) of the University of Algarve, Portugal, for their participation, both of the members of the research team and of the students, putting their hand as an object of study for this analysis. This work is part of the PhD project of the author V. Fernández-Navarro titled “From the hand to the mind: demography and semiotics of Palaeolithic artists through the representation of hands”. This doctoral thesis aims to characterize the biological sex and age of Palaeolithic blown and printed archaeological hand representations through the classical and geometric morphometry of the hand and its comparison with current populations. The corresponding author is beneficiary of a “Concepción Arenal” 2020-21 pre-doctoral scholarship at the University of Cantabria for research towards the PhD. RM Godinho is funded by Fundação para a Ciência e a Tecnologia (FCT; contract reference 2020.00499.CEECIND; https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.54499/2020.00499.CEECIND/CP1613/CT0002). This research was also funded by: FCT R&D projects (project “ParaFunction—Are Neanderthals adapted to heavy masticatory and paramasticatory function?”, reference 2022.07737.PTDC; https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.54499/2022.07737.PTDC). The present study has been conducted as part of the research project "Before art: social investment in symbolic expressions during the Upper Palaeolithic (B-Art)” (PID2019-107262GB-I00), funded by the Ministry of Science, Innovation and Universities, PI: Diego Garate.
Funding
The author V. Fernandez-Navarro ´ is beneficiary of a “Concepcion ´ Arenal” 2020–21 pre-doctoral contract at the University of Cantabria for research toward the doctoral thesis titled “From the hand to the mind: demography and semiotics of Palaeolithic artists through the representation of hands”. RM Godinho is funded by Fundação para a Ciência e a Tecnologia (FCT; contract reference 2020.00499.CEECIND; https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.54499/2020.00499.CEECIND/CP1613/CT0002). This research was also funded by: FCT R&D projects (project “ParaFunction—Are Neanderthals adapted to heavy masticatory and paramasticatory function?”, reference 2022.07737.PTDC; https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.54499/2022.07737.PTDC). The present study has been conducted as part of the research project “Before Art: social investment in symbolic expressions during the Upper Palaeolithic (B-Art)” (PID 2019–107262 GB-I00), funded by the Ministry of Science, Innovation, and Universities (MCIN/ AEI /10.13039/ 501100011033), PI: Diego Garate.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
V.F.N., R.M. and D.G.M wrote the main manuscript text and run all the statistical analysis. V.F.N prepared Figs. 1-5 and Tab.1. D. Garate.M. coordinated the whole work and all authors reviewed the manuscript in several occasions.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Fernández Navarro, V., Godinho, R.M., García Martínez, D. et al. Exploring the utility of Geometric Morphometrics to analyse prehistoric hand stencils. Sci Rep 14, 6336 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-56889-3
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-56889-3
Comments
By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.