Abstract
Educational attainment is a key indicator of status and opportunity in meritocratic societies. However, it is unclear how educational expansion has affected the link between cognitive abilities and educational attainment. As a result, this study examines the correlation between cognitive ability and educational attainment across male birth cohorts in Norway. Utilizing administrative register data covering more than four decades, we investigate multiple measures of educational attainment and their connection to cognitive abilities. Our findings reveal a consistent decline in the correlation over time. These findings question the assumed shift towards meritocracy in educational systems and highlight a more complex relationship between cognitive ability and educational attainment.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
An established hypothesis holds that broadened societal access to educational opportunities reduces the influence of family background and increases intergenerational mobility by meritocratically lifting talented youths out of their origin class1. The educational system has been seen as a “stairway to opportunity” open to all, with educational credentials increasingly viewed as an indicator of skill sets and future productivity2.
According to these meritocratic narratives, educational opportunities are increasingly allocated on the basis of individual ability and effort rather than parental wealth and resources3,4. If this were so, we would expect strong predictors of educational performance, such as cognitive capabilities, to increasingly align with educational attainment. Cognitive ability does not strictly equate with merit, talent, or natural endowments, but it is a relatively stable individual-level trait that predicts academic potential, which implies that it should be increasingly aligned with educational attainment as social programs have helped level the socioeconomic playing field. This implication has, to our knowledge, not been examined systematically.
Intelligence or cognitive ability is predictive of socio-economic success later in life5,6,7. Within-individuals, cognitive ability seems to become more predictive across age for earnings and occupations, but not necessarily for education6. Evidence on temporal change in society at large is nevertheless sparse and hard to parse, with comparisons of samples collected at different time points indicating both stability, declining and increasing correlations6,7,8,9. A systematic and population-based comparison of this correlation across birth cohorts has not, to our knowledge, been conducted.
To remedy this, we examine the temporal trends in the cognitive ability-educational attainment link, calculating how cognitive ability correlates with educational attainment across four decades of Norwegian male birth cohorts (1950–1991). The Norwegian case is of particular interest, as our sample covers a period of rapid and substantial democratization of education as the Norwegian welfare state expanded after the second world war. Social differences were to be reduced through a publicly funded, broadly accessible educational system, which remains even today relatively free of privatization and a market orientation10. Compulsory schooling was increased from 7 to 9 years in 1969, universal rights to higher secondary education for those aged 16 and 19 came in 1994, and the school starting age was reduced from 7 to 6 in 199711. Moreover, the state offer stipends and loans to anyone who enrolls in tertiary education, and credits and degrees are easily transferable across tertiary institutions.
Our analyses use Norwegian administrative register data with full population coverage and link scores from a high-quality cognitive ability test used at military conscription for men to different measures of individual educational qualifications. Across multiple methods and measures we find strong negative trends, with weakening correlations between educational attainment and cognitive ability. Correlations nevertheless remain relatively strong, pointing to cognitive ability as an important factor related to educational attainment. The loosening connection between cognitive ability and education over time nevertheless has major implications for sorting and rewards in many social domains.
Results
Figure 1 displays the correlation between cognitive ability and four different educational attainment measures calculated for each birth cohort 1950–1991. The correlation for years of education at age of 30 (in green) starts out above 0.63 for the oldest birth cohorts, but declines over cohorts ending with a correlation under 0.50 for the cohorts born after 1980. The positive bivariate relationship between cognitive ability and schooling remains, but flattens out after the 1970’s and decreases over time.
The series of correlations between intelligence and constant educational earnings ranks (i.e., earnings rank within educational degrees; shown in red) show a decline, from more than 0.60 for individuals born in the 1950’s to roughly 0.35 after 1990. This indicates a near-halving of the strength of the relationship between cognitive ability and educational attainment over one generation.
We also allow the educational earnings rank to vary across birth cohorts (orange series). After 1970 a stronger downward trend is seen, which arrives at a correlation of around 0.40 for the youngest cohorts. This indicates that the correlation with cognitive ability declines even more when we adjust education ranks by their changing economic rewards over time.
Educational attainment is a discrete outcome variable that does not conform to a normal distribution, but it can be modeled as a discretized version of an unobserved (latent) educational liability that captures all factors influencing educational attainment. Similarly, we can view a stanine score of e.g., 8 as a discretization of an underlying continuous ability scale. Polychoric correlations estimate the correlation between these latent constructs and are shown in purple. These are consistently higher, but show the same pattern with a long term secular decline. While the correlation starts slightly under 0.75 for the cohort born in 1950, it declines steadily to just above 0.65 in more recent cohorts.
A series of supplementary analyses provide further robustness checks. One concern is reverse causality, as school attendance has well-documented—albeit modest—effects on cognitive ability test scores12,13. To avoid picking up reverse causality, Figure S3 (in the Supplement) displays the correlations of conscription test scores with post-test educational achievement—i.e., using only variation in educational achievements observed after age 19. The figure shows the same downward trend as the main analysis. A second concern is that high-SES families, recognizing the increasing importance of education, may be working harder than before to push lower-ability children into education, weakening the ability-attainment gradient. To assess this, we estimate the regression slope of cognitive ability on educational attainment while controlling for parental SES (here fathers’ earning ranks), plotting the ability coefficient for each birth cohort separately. While the estimates are noisier and the trends fluctuate more, the overall pattern of decline remains clear (see Figure S4 in the Supplement).
Discussion
Across multiple measures of educational attainment, we find a steady long-term decline in the correlation of cognitive ability scores from conscription testing and educational attainment for males in Norway. The correlation remains moderate to strong in recent cohorts and cognitive ability remains coupled to educational attainment, but the clear trend indicates that educational attainment is weakening over time as a signal of cognitive ability. This observation together with the finding that we already find a very high correlation between educational attainment and cognitive ability for birth cohorts prior to educational reforms and the democratization of education, goes counter to the hypothesis that educational attainment increasingly aligns with individual level ability as educational opportunities are broadened. Essentially, it questions the presumed evolution from ascription to achievement as the guiding principle of success in post-industrial societies14. Ability scores come from tests at age 18–19, which has been shown to increase the correlation between intelligence and education compared to when cognitive ability is measured at an earlier age6. Yet, as this age of measurement was consistent across years, it should not impact the trend in the correlations.
Norway is arguably one of the contemporary societies where it is easiest to obtain higher levels of education. From a less developed educational system, school reforms in the 1960s increased compulsory schooling for everyone in Norway12. Since 1994, everyone has a legal right to a place in a high school program irrespective of their past performance. The tertiary education system is relatively open and universal financial aid is provided by the government. The heritability of educational attainment has been found relatively high in Norway and similar countries1. Hence, this is a context where we would expect a strong correlation between cognitive ability and educational attainment to emerge, reflecting a shift towards meritocratic hierarchies following from the expansion of mass education.
The Norwegian data indicates the opposite of the expected pattern: educational attainment is over time becoming weaker as a signal of the cognitive abilities. Although this can mean different things, it has implications for the interpretation and valuation of educational attainment as an expression of an underlying trait. As cognitive ability does not equate with merit, educational attainment could still potentially be a strongly meritocratic signal of some broader set of traits, but this bundle may have become increasingly weighted towards non-cognitive factors such as motivation, perseverance, and emotional intelligence, perhaps as a result of changing demands in the labor market15,16,17,18. Others nevertheless argue that the role of non-cognitive skills, such as personality, in predicting socio-economic success has been overstated and does not remotely match the predictive power or cognitive capability19,20.
An alternative, potentially more plausible explanation for our findings, is the nature of the changing educational and labor market. Education might have become substantially less selective, as educational expansions may have made it generally easier to attain longer education, regardless of cognitive ability. This was coupled with a diversification of educational fields, whereby occupations that previously did not require higher education, and that may require other abilities than cognitive skills, might increasingly require degrees. The increased decoupling of educational attainment and cognitive ability could also happen if new educational tracks emphasize practical skills and less abstract curriculums that make cognitive ability less important. This is especially relevant in Norway, where vocationally oriented tertiary institutions are offering a wide range of educational programs and are acquiring an important status next to universities21. They are nevertheless still mainly attended by students with lower average grades and they have lower rejection rates, which could make the achievement of high educational attainment less dependent on cognitive ability22. Drawing on signaling theory23, educational attainment may also have become a noisier signal over time if the cost required to complete higher education has fallen sufficiently. Employers will to a lesser degree be able to screen candidates by using their educational credentials, and demand for other signals will increase.
Finally, there are some limitations to our study. We would note that the cognitive ability test used at conscription has remained essentially unchanged since the test was developed in the early 1950s, which means that test items may have become outdated and function differently due to changes in social context (e.g., the introduction of calculators in school would give pupils less training in pen-and-pencil calculations required for the conscription test). In addition, we only had cognitive ability scores available for men, which does not allow us to say anything about the correlation for women. Although research has shown that intelligence does not relate differently to school success for boys and girls24, and we would hence expect to observe similar patterns for women, this should be tested explicitly in future research.
Materials and methods
Data
We use information on male cognitive ability, educational attainment and taxable earnings in administrative register data from Statistics Norway. The data cover the male Norwegian birth cohorts 1950–1991, and contain a pseudonymous individual-level identifier that allows information to be linked across registers. The project was approved by the Norwegian Data Protection Authority (Datatilsynet). The Norwegian Center for Research Data and the University of Oslo's Data Protection Official have approved the Data Protection Impact Assessment for the project.
The cognitive ability data are in the form of stanine scores from compulsory military conscription testing, available for the large majority of all males who are typically scored around age 18/19. The stanine scores reflect performance on three timed tests covering arithmetic (30 items), word similarities (54 items) and figures (36 items). The test items were unchanged throughout the period, but the scoring norm was changed for the 1961 and later cohorts to accommodate the Flynn effect. Additionally, the mathematics part of the test was changed to a multiple-choice format around the 1980 test year.
Information on education comes from the educational registry. Educational attainment is defined in four separate ways, to assess robustness of results across measures: (1) years of schooling at the age of 30, (2) the “global” or constant educational-earnings rank (calculated from pooled birth cohorts), (3) the cohort-specific or variable educational-earnings rank, and (4) latent educational liability that treats observed education as binned ranges of an underlying continuous liability. The second and third measures explicitly consider the economic benefits of specific educational credentials, while the last measure adjusts for the “mechanical” measurement error in correlation measures when using an ordinal outcome variable.
Measurement of earnings rank draws on annual earnings, which are available since 1967. The algorithm calculating lifetime rank first computes the individual’s percentile position in the male birth cohort earnings distribution at each age between ages 30 and 60 (or the highest observed age for those born after 1961) and uses the average of age-specific percentiles to form the individual’s lifetime percentile rank. The constant educational-earnings rank measure is computed as the average lifetime rank in four-digit educational attainment cells, using the pooled 1930 through 1981 birth cohorts. For the time-varying educational-earnings rank measure, we compute the average rank in each four-digit attainment cell using individuals born 10 years earlier than those in our sample (i.e., matching attainment of those born 1950–1991 with earnings rank of those born 1940–1981).
Figures S1 and S2 (in the Supplement) show descriptive statistics for all but one measurement across birth cohorts (not feasible for educational liability measures) as well as the size of each of these cohorts. The statistics show fluctuations and trends in line with the extant literature, e.g. the emergence and reversal of the Flynn effect and general educational upgrading over cohorts. The correlations reported should not be affected by fluctuations in the variance and mean of our measures, as correlations are standardized within each birth cohort, ensuring comparability across time.
Statistical methods
Correlations between cognitive ability and educational attainment are calculated for each of the birth cohorts separately using standard Pearson product-moment correlations. In addition, as the years of education show substantial bunching and a decidedly non-normal distribution, we also calculate polychoric correlations that model years of education as reflecting binned ranges of an underlying latent educational liability—where the latent thresholds separating different attainment categories is allowed to vary across birth cohorts. 95% confidence intervals are calculated for all four series.
Data availability
Norwegian privacy regulations limit our ability to share our register data, but we can provide guidance on how to request access to these data. The leading author can be contacted for more information on how to apply for the data.
References
Engzell, P. & Tropf, F. C. Heritability of education rises with intergenerational mobility. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 116, 25386–25388 (2019).
Brown, P. & Tannock, S. Education, meritocracy and the global war for talent. J. Educ. Policy 24, 377–392 (2009).
Abdellaoui, A., Borcan, O., Chiappori, P. & Hugh-Jones, D. Trading social status for genetics in marriage markets: Evidence from UK biobank. School of Economics Working Paper 2022–04, 1–63 (2022).
Bond, R. & Saunders, P. Routes of success: influences on the occupational attainment of young British males. Brit. J. Sociol. 50, 217–249 (1999).
Ng, T. W., Eby, L. T., Sorensen, K. L. & Feldman, D. C. Predictors of objective and subjective career success: A meta-analysis. Personnel Psychol. 58, 367–408 (2005).
Strenze, T. Intelligence and socioeconomic success: A meta-analytic review of longitudinal research. Intelligence 35, 401–426 (2007).
Jencks, C. et al. Who Gets Ahead? The Determinants of Economic Success in America (Basic Books, 1979).
Marks, G. N. Cognitive ability has powerful, widespread and robust effects on social stratification: Evidence from the 1979 and 1997 US National Longitudinal Surveys of Youth. Intelligence 94, 1–19 (2022).
Hasl, A., Kretschmann, J., Richter, D., Voelkle, M. & Brunner, M. Investigating core assumptions of the “American Dream”: Historical changes in how adolescents’ socioeconomic status, IQ, and GPA are related to key life outcomes in adulthood. Psychol. Aging 34, 1055–1076 (2019).
Wiborg, S. Neo-liberalism and universal state education: the cases of Denmark, Norway and Sweden 1980–2011. Comp. Educ. 49, 407–423 (2013).
Thuen, H. & Volckmar, N. Postwar School Reforms in Norway. Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Education 1–28 (2020).
Brinch, C. N. & Galloway, T. A. Schooling in adolescence raises IQ scores. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 109, 425–430 (2012).
Ritchie, S. J. & Tucker-Drob, E. M. How much does education improve intelligence? A meta-analysis. Psychol. Sci. 29, 1358–1369 (2018).
Bell, D. Meritocracy and equality. Public Interest 29, 29 (1972).
Bowles, S., Gintis, H. & Osborne, M. The determinants of earnings: a behavioral approach. J. Econ. Lit. 39, 1137–1176 (2001).
Jokela, M., Pekkarinen, T., Sarvimäki, M., Terviö, M. & Uusitalo, R. Secular rise in economically valuable personality traits. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 114, 6527–6532 (2017).
Deming, D. J. The growing importance of social skills in the labor market*. Q. J. Econ. 132, 1593–1640 (2017).
Lindqvist, E. & Vestman, R. The labor market returns to cognitive and noncognitive ability: Evidence from the Swedish enlistment. Am. Econ. J. Appl. Econ. 3, 101–128 (2011).
Zisman, C. & Ganzach, Y. In a representative sample grit has a negligible effect on educational and economic success compared to intelligence. Soc. Psychol. Person. Sci. 12, 296–303 (2021).
Zisman, C. & Ganzach, Y. The claim that personality is more important than intelligence in predicting important life outcomes has been greatly exaggerated. Intelligence 92, 101631 (2022).
Kyvik, S. The non-university higher education sector in Norway. In Non-university Higher Education in Europe (eds Taylor, J. S. et al.) 169–189 (Springer, 2008).
Borgen, N. T. Changes in the economic returns to attending prestigious institutions in Norway. Eur. Soc. 17, 219–241 (2015).
Spence, M. Job market signaling. In Uncertainty in Economics (eds Diamond, P. & Rothschild, M.) 281–306 (Academic Press, 1978).
Spinath, B., Eckert, C. & Steinmayr, R. Gender differences in school success: what are the roles of students’ intelligence, personality and motivation?. Educ. Res. 56, 230–243 (2014).
Acknowledgements
The authors wish to thank Are Skeie Hermansen for his valuable feedback and Adrian Farner Rogne for his input. Torkild Hovde Lyngstad was supported by funding from the European Research Council under the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme (Grant agreement 818420). Arno Van Hootegem was supported by a convergence grant from UiO:Life Sciences. Ole Rogeberg, Bernt Bratsberg and Torkild Hovde Lyngstad also received project funding from the Norwegian Research Council, Grant 300917
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
A.V.H. wrote the majority of the text and created the visualizations. O.R. and B.B. conducted the analyses and wrote parts of the text. T.H.L. revised the text and the final version.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Van Hootegem, A., Røgeberg, O., Bratsberg, B. et al. Correlation between cognitive ability and educational attainment weakens over birth cohorts. Sci Rep 13, 17747 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-023-44605-6
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-023-44605-6
Comments
By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.