Abstract
Previous longitudinal studies found significant associations between screen time and increase in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) symptoms, but the mechanisms mediating this association remain understudied. Thus, we used data from a 5-year population-based longitudinal cohort of nearly 4000 Canadian high school students, modeled using multivariate multilevel mediation, to investigate the association of screen time (i.e., social media, television, video games, computer use) with ADHD symptoms via different potential behavioral and neuropsychological mediators (i.e. impulsivity, response inhibition, working memory). We studied direct and indirect between-person, concurrent within-person, and lagged-within-person effects of screens on ADHD symptoms. Results showed that increases in screen time in a given year were associated with an exacerbation of ADHD symptoms within that same year (within-person association), over and above potential common vulnerability (between-person association). Impulsivity proved to be the most robust mediator in the association of screen time with ADHD symptoms at both between and within-person levels. Only social media use displayed a significant lagged-within-person association with ADHD symptoms mediated by impulsivity, indicating an enduring influence on behavior, which was further shown to be mediated by chained changes in response inhibition on a Go/No-Go task. These findings provide clinical implications of screen time and should be an important focus in the management and prevention of ADHD symptoms among adolescents.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a behavioral disorder characterized by symptoms of inattention and/or impulsivity and hyperactivity, which can affect many aspects of behavior and performance among youth, both at school and at home1. ADHD during adolescence has been associated not only with neuropsychological functions, but also with significant academic, psychosocial, emotional, and cognitive impairment. For instance, it has been linked to poor social relationships, low self-esteem, deviant behavior, and substance use and abuse2.
In 2016, an estimated 6.1 million North-American children 2–17 years of age (9.4%) had received an ADHD diagnosis, of which the highest rates of ADHD were found among adolescents aged 12–17 years3,4. Furthermore, the U.S. national prevalence of ADHD was shown to increase by 12.6% over the past decade (from 8.47 to 9.54% from 2009–2011 to 2015–2017)4. It has been estimated that U.S. healthcare expenditures for ADHD total $23 billion5 and that the annual costs including healthcare, education, and reduced family productivity associated with youth ADHD in the U.S. have been estimated to range from $38 to $72 billion6.
Considering that ADHD risk is linked to both genetic and non-genetic environmental risk factors, identifying potential environmental factors that might explain epidemiologic shifts in ADHD severity or prevalence might help to inform more comprehensive treatment and public health strategies to promote youth mental health7. One of these environmental risk factors might be the rise of continuous expansion of the digital media landscape, which can be considered one of the most dramatic cultural changes that has occurred in the past decade. The ubiquity of internet access, social media platforms, online video gaming, and of the wide variety of mobile digital devices result in near-constant exposure to virtual information, which may interfere with adolescent neurodevelopment processes8,9 and make them increasingly digitally dependent showing behavior disorders10,11,12,13,14.
It has been suggested that North-American youth spend an average of 6–9 h interacting with digital devices, and it must be pointed out that many young people often use more than one screen medium simultaneously, increasing their total exposure to digital devices15. Moreover, the recent outbreak of the COVID-19 pandemic has resulted in a substantial increase in screen time among adolescents16,17,18. Finally, it is noteworthy that many digital platforms are designed to tap natural human attentional, impulsive, and reward processes, which in turn leads to habit formation and repeated use of the platform13,19,20,21,22,23.
Research on the impact of digital screen time on adolescent health is still in its infancy and the neurodevelopmental consequences of exposure to such digital platforms are not yet fully understood. This is particularly relevant to ADHD symptoms, as impulsivity, reward processing, and attention/concentration are key features that drive ADHD behavioral impairments24,25. In particular, deficits in working memory are typical among individuals with ADHD26, and a meta-analysis found significantly more and more severe verbal and visual-spatial working memory impairments in adolescents with ADHD compared to non-ADHD adolescents27. Previous works also suggested that those diagnosed with ADHD have been shown to differ from those without a diagnosis on a variety of tests of inhibitory control, such as, go/no-go tasks28,29,30,31 and continuous performance tasks32,33.
Findings from emerging research suggest that excessive usage of digital media in adolescents may affect brain functioning and cognitive development, including impaired attention and impaired memory processing, as well as impairment in impulse regulation and reward processing13,34,35. More specifically for social media, heavy Facebook users had worse short-term memory than light users36 and problematic use of social networking sites has been associated with attention problems37. Moreover, previous studies have shown that total television and video game exposure were related to future attention problems, controlling for earlier attention38,39. Longitudinal studies also showed significant, though modest, associations between modern digital media use (e.g. checking social media, texting, posting, etc.) and symptoms of ADHD40, as well as long-term effects of media multitasking on attention problems for adolescents aged 11 and 13 years old41.
Although the above-presented findings of previous works suggest an important relationship between digital media use and ADHD symptom severity, little research directly tested mediation hypotheses42, and even fewer have attempted to address these hypotheses by investigating potential pathways through the effect of digital media use on neuropsychological development in adolescence. As previously suggested42, mediating variables could shine a light on the underlying mechanism between screen time and its consequences, thereby providing further evidence of a causal relationship43. Furthermore, from a methodological perspective, ignoring mediating variables could lead to an underestimation of effect sizes in empirical research, and subsequent meta-analyses44. Hence, to obtain a true understanding of the association of digital media use and ADHD symptoms through direct and indirect effects, there is a need for studies that measure adolescents’ responses to different digital media over time, while taking into account mediating factors. The empirical findings and insights derived from such studies may prove pivotal in designing remedial and preventive interventions to assist in the management of ADHD-related behaviors among adolescents. To address this gap in the literature, potential mediating factors of interest could be impulsivity and neuropsychological functions, which are not only identified as core behavioral and cognitive features of ADHD25,45, but also seem to be affected by screen time, as reviewed above. Demonstrating chained mediation of cognitive processes in the link between screentime and mental health would contribute to a growing literature on potential causal effects of screentime on child brain health.
In this study, we used multivariate multilevel linear models (MLMs) to analyze longitudinal data to test causal hypotheses on the role of screen time (i.e., social media use, television viewing, video gaming, and computer use) in the growth of ADHD symptoms across adolescence, by: (i) first modeling, and controlling for potential common vulnerability to high levels of digital media use, impulsivity, neuropsychological functioning, and ADHD symptoms (between-person effects); (ii) modeling how changes in impulsivity and neuropsychological functioning co-vary directly with an increase in digital media use in the same year and the following year (concurrent within-person effects and lagged within-person effects, respectively); (iii) modeling how impulsivity and neuropsychological functions mediate indirect associations of digital media and ADHD symptoms at between-, within- and lagged-within person levels; (iv) and also compared the four types of screen time as predictors of ADHD symptoms and neuropsychological functioning to explore if certain types of digital media are particularly linked to cognitive and mental health outcomes.
We designed a multilevel analytic strategy in order to be able to differentiate between-person variation from within-person variation. The former reflects the time-invariant degree to which individuals differ in their average scores from others in the sample, in other words, the set point of an individual compared with their peers. The latter effect represents the time-varying degree to which individuals deviate from their own average scores when tested in successive years through repeated measures, representing the displacement from the set point at a given time point. Therefore, our longitudinal study allowed disaggregating inter-individual and intra-individual differences in the association of screen time and ADHD symptoms.
This study also applies the same strategy to investigating neurocognitive and behavioral mediators of this relationship to confirm its validity, by including objectively measured (task-based) response inhibition and working memory performance, as well as self-reported impulsivity, annually for five consecutive years. We will uniquely investigate temporal precedence of neurocognitive mediators in this relationship, in accordance with current etiologic theories of ADHD placing response inhibition, working memory and trait impulsivity at the core of this complex brain disorder25,45.
Results
Descriptive statistics
The sample included 3779 adolescents (1858 girls [49%]; mean [SD] age, 12.8 [0.5] years) who consented and completed the first survey of this 5-year longitudinal study.
The descriptive statistics of each variable and the frequency distribution for the different types of screen time over 5 years are available in Tables S1 and S2, respectively, as Supplementary Information.
As shown by the frequency distribution for the different types of screen time, we reported an increase in missing data over the course of the study (0.30% of missing data relative to all data at year 1; 32% of missing data relative to all data at year 5).
Over the 5-year period, there were reported substantial increases in minimum time spent using social media (year 1 mean[SD], 42[63] minutes; year 5 mean[SD], 70[66] minutes) and slight increases in time spent viewing television (year 1 mean[SD], 66[63] minutes; year 5 mean[SD], 72[66] minutes) and using the computer (year 1 mean[SD], 25[50] minutes; year 5 mean[SD], 26[50] minutes), while there were reported decreases in video gaming (year 1 mean[SD], 59[69] minutes; year 5 mean[SD], 55[72] minutes).
Scores related to ADHD symptoms increased across the 5-year study (year 1 mean[SD], 3.93[2.31]; year 5 mean[SD], 4.06[2.36]). Impulsivity reported stable scores across the five survey waves (year 1 mean[SD], 11.88[2.84] points; year 5 mean[SD], 11.31[2.84] pints). Concerning neuropsychological function variables, both response inhibition errors (year 1 mean[SD], 31.28[17.62]; year 5 mean[SD], 16.16[15.02]) and working memory errors (year 1 mean[SD], 16.05[10.19]; year 5 mean[SD], 8.89[8.40]) decreased from year-to-year.
Participants reported a mean[SD] socio-economic status score of 2.94[1.08].
The direct associations of screen time and ADHD symptoms
All results of MLM analysis indicating the direct associations of screen time and ADHD symptoms among adolescents (according to the model schematized in Fig. 1a) are shown in Table 1. The results showed a statistically significant between-person effect of social media use, television viewing, and video gaming on ADHD symptoms. Results also indicated a statistically significant concurrent within-person effect of social media use, television viewing, and video gaming on ADHD symptoms. Computer use showed no statistical significant association with ADHD symptoms at between and within levels. Since computer use did not show significant direct associations with ADHD symptoms, we did not include it in further statistical analysis. No further statistically significant lagged-within-person effect was found for screen time on ADHD symptoms.
The indirect associations of screen time and ADHD symptoms through impulsivity and neuropsychological functions
Results of MLM analysis assessing the association of social media use, television viewing and video gaming on ADHD symptoms in adolescents through impulsivity and neuropsychological functions (according to the model schematized in Fig. 1b) are shown in Tables 2, 3 and 4, respectively.
Findings showed a statistically significant association of social media use to ADHD symptoms mediated by impulsivity at both between-person and concurrent within-person levels, as well as at lagged-within-person level (which translates to a mediated path over 3 years, with the predictor, mediator, and the outcome consecutively measured 1 year apart). With regards to the association of social media to ADHD symptoms mediated by neuropsychological functions, ADHD symptoms were predicted by social media use only through response inhibition at between-person level.
We found a statistically significant impulsivity-mediated association of television viewing on ADHD symptoms at both between-person and concurrent within-person levels, while it showed a statistically significant association with ADHD symptoms mediated by both response inhibition and working memory only at between-person level.
Video gaming revealed a statistically significant impulsivity-mediated association with ADHD at between- and concurrent within-person levels, while response inhibition and working memory revealed a statistically significant mediation role only at between-person level.
Four-level chained mediation analyses
Considering that impulsivity proved to be a robust mediator of the longitudinal relationship between screen time and ADHD symptoms, MLM models investigated potential chained mediation processes whereby neuropsychological functions were considered as potential intermediate mediators between screen time and impulsivity, which in turn predicted ADHD symptoms 1 year later.
As first step, two different mediation MLMs were performed where impulsivity was predicted by screen time through each of the neuropsychological functions (see the model schematized in Fig. 1c). Results are shown in Table 5. Social media use showed a significant association with impulsivity mediated by response inhibition at between-person and concurrent within-person levels, and by working memory at between-person level. Television viewing and video gaming showed significant associations with impulsivity through response inhibition and working memory only at between-person level.
Two final analyses applying a 4-variable 4-year chained mediation MLMs with multiple mediators of interest were conducted (see the model schematized in Fig. 1d). In these models, ADHD symptoms were predicted by screen time through two chained mediators, i.e., each neuropsychological function and impulsivity. Results are shown in Table 6. Over and above significant between-person relationships, social media use showed significant concurrent within-person effects on ADHD symptoms through response inhibition on impulsivity, while television viewing and video gaming showed only significant between-person effects through response inhibition and working memory.
Discussion
Using a large population-based sample of nearly 4000 Canadian adolescents to investigate longitudinal direct and indirect associations of different types of screen time (i.e., social media use, television viewing, video gaming, and computer use) with ADHD symptoms, this study distinguished between different time-varying factors: between-person effect, concurrent within-person effect, and lagged-within-person effect. Different potential mediators of the association of screen time and ADHD (i.e., impulsivity, response inhibition, and working memory) were evaluated, and several important conclusions can be drawn.
First, our findings indicated that social media use, television viewing and video gaming were shown to be directly associated with ADHD symptoms during adolescence at both between- and concurrent within-person levels. In other words, the results demonstrated significant common vulnerability between higher levels of these types of screen time and general vulnerability to ADHD symptoms. Over and above this potential common vulnerability, increases in screen time on a given year predicted concurrent increases in ADHD symptoms relative to a person’s mean level of ADHD symptoms throughout adolescence, further suggesting a direct short-term negative impact of screen time on ADHD symptoms.
The study did not reveal a significant mediating role of both neuropsychological functions in the longitudinal association of screen time and ADHD symptoms (indirect lagged within-person effect), but did demonstrate significant concurrent and lagged relationships of social media with ADHD symptoms through impulsivity at within-person level, implying that any further increases in a given year of social media use were associated with more severe ADHD symptoms that same year and from 1 year to the next through higher self-reported impulsivity. This means that social media heavy users showed the strongest effect on ADHD symptoms through impulsive behavior compared with their peers, showing also a mediated effect on their ADHD symptoms on any given year comparing with their average set point, and a small-mediated effect on their ADHD symptoms in the following year.
Trait impulsivity, which moderately correlates with individual neuropsychological measures, like response inhibition and working memory, is considered a multifaceted construct and a key proximal indicator of risk for externalising mental health problems25,45. Accordingly, we further investigated the association of screen time and ADHD symptoms, testing the mediational role of neurophysiological functions in the relationship between screen time and this important proximal mediator of risk in a MLM with multiple chained mediators. Results showed that the common vulnerability to greater social media use, television viewing, and video gaming and ADHD behaviors was mediated by neurophysiological functions and impulsivity at between-person levels. However, only social media was shown to be longitudinally linked to increased risk for ADHD symptoms through a mediation path that involved disrupted neuropsychological functioning (inhibitory control) and impulsive temperament.
Altogether, our findings are in line with previous longitudinal studies showing significant associations between digital media use and ADHD symptom severity40, and stress the importance of monitoring screen time during adolescence in order to protect those at risk for heavy screen time and to reduce the likelihood of ADHD symptoms being exacerbated through disrupted neurocognitive functioning and behavioural regulation.
According to our results, it appears that there is strong evidence for a common vulnerability to heavy screen time use and adolescents’ neurocognitive risk for ADHD symptoms. It might be argued that adolescents vulnerable to ADHD or those with higher levels of ADHD symptoms are particularly drawn to digital media because they offer activities presenting them with brief, superficial and stimulating parcels of content, without much need to exert cognitive control and allowing for constant attentional switching46. Indeed, according to the American Psychiatric Association47, adolescents who live with ADHD often have difficulties in finishing cognitive tasks requiring extended periods of sustained attention and show a preference for immediate as opposed to delayed rewards, because of their impulsive attitude. This symptom profile is, in fact, characterized by decreased activation of brain regions implicated in cognitive control and inhibitory control systems48. Children with higher polygenic risk score for ADHD present alterations in white matter tracts related to visual functions, resulting in impaired executive control of visual functions, and making subjects with ADHD symptoms more sensitive to external visual stimuli and more easily distracted by digital media49. Based on our findings, we posit that social media, television and video games exposure, which showed between-effects on ADHD symptoms mediated by impulsivity and neuropsychological functions, might appeal to adolescents with higher levels of ADHD symptoms.
Over and above this common vulnerability and focusing on the longitudinal risk for ADHD highlighted by this study, our findings support an empirical model showing neuropsychological consequences of digital media use on cognition and temperament during adolescence, where social media proved to be the most robust longitudinal predictor for ADHD symptoms and this relationship was explained by the effects of social media on response inhibition and impulsivity through a chained pathway. Interestingly, in this chained pathway of effects, temporal precedence was demonstrated between social media and impulsivity and between impulsivity and ADHD symptoms, but neuropsychological outcomes of social media only proved to be concurrent. As shown in Fig. 2, increased time spent in front of social media leads to neurocognitive inhibitory control difficulties, which in turn result in a concurrent increase in self-reported impulsive behaviors and a consequent increase in ADHD symptoms in the same year, as well as the next year. These findings were shown to be robust even after accounting for the potential common vulnerability between these behaviors and neurocognitive difficulties, in a manner that is consistent with a causal hypothesis.
The mechanisms by which social media have a unique relationship to impaired response inhibition, are not well understood, but might be related to the nature of the brief and unnarrated content that youth are exposed to on social media. Indeed, social media involves rapidly scrolling through brief, diverse segments of visual information with sound bites, which requires limited cognitive control (e.g., images, brief posts or status updates). Interacting with this content can be characterized by instant rewards and constant stimulation that could raise the threshold for adolescent sustained attention on other less stimulating situations and tasks, exacerbating inattention and distractibility41,50. Compared to video game use, social media users’ responses are very limited, defined by a simple gesture in the form of short phrases, likes and/or emoji and are not goal oriented. Similarly, compared to content viewed on television, social media also rarely contains a sustained narrative, involving plot or character development, or a series of facts linked to an argument or a thesis. Finally, social media involves viewing content shared by peers, which, more strongly promotes social norms in young people, for better or worse51.
While this study did not focus on the interaction between risk factors and consequences of digital media on mental health, it is noteworthy that youth with ADHD often experience difficulties with behavior, self-regulation, and task focus making them vulnerable to social isolation and other disabilities52,53,54, and high exposure to screen time appears to contribute to some of these symptoms and exacerbate them in those who are already vulnerable39. A major strength of our study is the use of state-of-the-art multilevel mediation modeling to investigate the underlying mechanism of this association, confirming the role of impulsivity and executive functions in linking screen time to ADHD symptoms in adolescence. This model provides an overall picture of the onset and development of ADHD symptoms by highlighting specific longitudinal direct and indirect between- and within-person associations, considering concurrent and lagged time-varying effects. Another strength of the study is the use of a large sample size of adolescents.
However, the findings need to be interpreted in light of some limitations. It remains unclear what specific types of social media, digital media content, and what kind of video games or computer-related activities youth participated in. Additionally, even though ADHD symptoms were evaluated with the five items of the Hyperactivity/Inattention scale of the validated and reliable Strength and Difficulties Questionnaire55, no clinical diagnostic confirmation was provided and participants may have under-reported their ADHD symptoms due to social desirability or lack of insight into impairment.
In conclusion, in the present study, social media was found to be the type of screen time most robustly associated with symptoms of ADHD in adolescence, while impulsivity seemed to be the strongest mediator between screen time and ADHD symptoms at between- and concurrent within-person levels, as well as at lagged-within-person level. Because of the implications of these findings for interventions, clinicians may want to examine the degree to which adolescents spend time in front of various digital screens and target social media use during therapy as a main potential means of reducing cognitive, behavioral and clinical manifestations of these patterns of difficulties. Furthermore, since the association of all studied types of screen time and ADHD symptoms were mediated by impulsivity, interventions targeting impulsive temperament and behavior and the role of screen time in both managing and exacerbating impulsivity could help young people and their families to better manage ADHD symptoms to promote well-being. Finally, further longitudinal studies are needed to understand the impact of specific contents, and presentation formats on ADHD-related behaviors among adolescents to potentially inform how social media platforms could provide services to families without these potential unintended cognitive and behavioral consequences. Such research will require close collaborations with social media companies to be able to better investigate how the algorithmic nature of content presentation contributes to the potential impact on child brain and mental health.
Methods
Participants
This study used data from the Co-Venture cohort, which has an embedded cluster randomized trial for a subset of high-risk youth (for further details see56). From September 2012, 3826 adolescents (1798 girls [47%]; mean [SD] age, 12.7 [0.5] years) from 7th grade were recruited with no exclusion criteria from 31 representative schools in the Greater Montreal Area (QC, Canada) with respect to size, socioeconomic indicators, and district. This cohort represents a unique research opportunity in the history of digital media, when teen access to smartphones and social media platforms exploded and adolescent-targeted products and platforms became more and more available57. In total, 3779 participants (1858 girls [49%]; mean [SD] age: 12.8 [0.5] years) consented and completed the survey in the 7th grade and were invited to complete repeated assessments every year for five consecutive years until the end of high school (11th grade). Data were acquired through a confidential annual web-based survey, which was downloaded to school computer laboratories to ensure that time-sensitive data (e.g., reaction time) were reliably collected and that students were closely supervised when completing the battery. The scores were saved on computers until a secure connection was available and then automatically transferred to a central anonymized database through a unique identifying code.
This research complies with all relevant ethical guidelines and regulations. Ethical approval was obtained from the Research Ethics Board of the Sainte-Justine Hospital Research Center in Montreal (QC, Canada). Students and their parents or legal guardians provided informed consent before taking part in the study. This study has been registered as sub-study from Co-Venture trial with the number NCT01655615 at http://www.clinicaltrials.gov (date of first trial registration 02/08/2012).
Measures
Screen time
Screen time was assessed using a self-report question requiring participants to state how much time per day they spend using social media (i.e., Facebook, Twitter, or other social networking sites), watching shows or movies on television or on the computer, playing video games (on the computer, cell phone, game console), and using the computer to engage in another kind of activities. The amount of screen time was established according to four categories: 0 to 30 min, 30 min to 1 h and 30 min, 1 h and 30 min to 2 h and 30 min, 2 h and 30 min to 3 h and 30 min, and 3 h and 30 min or more.
Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms
ADHD symptoms were assessed using the five items of the Hyperactivity/Inattention scale of the Strength and Difficulties Questionnaire55, measuring the key symptoms of ADHD: inattention (two items), hyperactivity (two items) and impulsiveness (one item). Participants indicated whether each statement was not true, somewhat true, or certainly true, yielding a final score from zero to two points.
Impulsivity
Participant impulsivity was assessed using five items related to the impulsivity personality dimension of the Substance Use Risk Profile Scale, which consists of twenty-three items that allow identifying four personality dimensions (i.e., impulsivity, anxiety sensitivity, hopelessness, and sensation seeking)58. Participants indicated whether they agreed with each statement by selecting one of four response options (strongly disagree, disagree, agree, strongly agree), yielding scores from one to four points.
Neuropsychological functions
A modified version of the Go/No-Go Passive Avoidance Learning Paradigm (PALP) was used to measure response inhibition in order to study inhibitory control and cognitive control. The Go/No-Go PALP is a discrimination task that demands participants to inhibit a rewarded response in order to win points or prevent loss of points59. During the task, participants learn to react to “good’’ numbers and to avoid reacting to “wrong’’ numbers through trial and error. Correct and incorrect responses were respectively accompanied by reward cues and punishments. Participants received performance feedback following each response. The dependent measure on this task is the total number of errors of commission (responding to no-go numbers).
Working memory was assessed using the “Find the phone task” which is similar to the Self-Order Pointing Task60 and the spatial working memory task of the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery61. This task presented participants with a number of colored boxes, which required them to remember which boxes they already searched. Participants were required to find a phone ‘token’ hidden amongst the boxes through trial and error. The location and colour of the boxes were changed from trial to trial and with the number of boxes progressively increasing (max. 8) to prevent reliance on stereotyped search strategies. The measure of spatial working memory deficit corresponds to the number of times that participants reselected the items that had already rung.
Covariates
We assessed baseline socio-economic status with the Family Affluence Scale for adolescents62 and sex (1 = male, 2 = female).
Statistics
We applied Bayesian MLMs to investigate direct and indirect associations of screen time (independent variable, predictor) and ADHD symptoms (dependent variable, outcome). The MLMs apply linear regression analysis to describe outcome variables as a function of predictor variables at two levels of data organization, the between-person and the within-person (change over time) levels. The between-person effects (the association between the predictor and the outcome variables averaged over five time points), the concurrent within-person effects (the associations between the predictor and the outcome variables in individual participants on a given year), and the lagged-within-person effects (the associations between the predictor on a given year and the outcome variables the following year in individual participants) were assessed within the same MLM at each analytical step. To assess time-varying associations, significant within-person associations would provide support for potential causal short-term concurrent associations between variables, while significant lagged-within-person associations would reflect a lasting effect of screen time on adolescent behavior.
Figure S1 in Supplementary Information provides visual representation of observed and derived (latent) variables for an analysis of relationships between two sets of variables. Covariates (sex, socio-economic status) were included at the between level. Because mediators were also measured at every time point, they were tested at between and within level. MLMs also provide estimates of the indirect effects of the predictor through the mediator or a chained set of mediators (see Supplementary Figs. S2 and S3, respectively) that, with the use of the lagged effect, can be temporally specified.
We first tested the direct association of screen time and ADHD symptoms by entering the four types of screen time in the same MLM (Fig. 1a). Second, we ran different independent MLMs to assess the role of impulsivity and neuropsychological functions (i.e. response inhibition and working memory) as mediators of an indirect 3-year relationship between relevant types of screen time and ADHD symptoms (Fig. 1b). Mediation models were only performed for screen time variables that showed significant relationships to ADHD symptoms.
To further characterize mediation effects in the context of multiple mediation variables, we also conducted independent mediation MLMs to analyze the associations between relevant types of screen time and impulsivity through both neuropsychological functions (Fig. 1c), with the rationale that changes in cognition would first lead to changes in a core symptoms of ADHD, which, over time would contribute to the development of a larger spectrum of ADHD symptoms19. In the context of such a mediation, we then tested 4-variable chained mediation MLMs, where the multiple sequential mediators were either response inhibition and impulsivity, working memory and impulsivity (Fig. 1d).
All analyses were performed using Mplus software (version 8.3, Muthén & Muthén, Los Angeles, CA). In all statistical models, we controlled for covariates (i.e., sex, socio-economic status) and time. The time parameter of our 5-year study was coded from one to five, considering the five survey waves as time points. To model the indirect effects of screen time on ADHD symptoms, the “model constraint” function in Mplus allowed multiplying the direct associations between predictors, mediators and outcomes.
Data availability
A summary of the data that support the findings of this study is available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
References
Faraone, S. V., Sergeant, J., Gillberg, C. & Biederman, J. The worldwide prevalence of ADHD: Is it an American condition?. World Psychiatry 2, 104 (2003).
Wehmeier, P. M., Schacht, A. & Barkley, R. A. Social and emotional impairment in children and adolescents with ADHD and the impact on quality of life. J. Adolesc. Health 46, 209–217. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2009.09.009 (2010).
Zablotsky, B. et al. Prevalence and trends of developmental disabilities among children in the United States: 2009–2017. Pediatrics https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2019-0811 (2019).
Danielson, M. L. et al. Prevalence of parent-reported ADHD diagnosis and associated treatment among US children and adolescents, 2016. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 47, 199–212. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2017.1417860 (2018).
Dieleman, J. L. et al. US spending on personal health care and public health, 1996–2013. JAMA 316, 2627–2646. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.2016.16885 (2016).
Doshi, J. A. et al. Economic impact of childhood and adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in the United States. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 51, 990-1002.e1002. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2012.07.008 (2012).
Larsson, J.-O., Larsson, H. & Lichtenstein, P. Genetic and environmental contributions to stability and change of ADHD symptoms between 8 and 13 years of age: A longitudinal twin study. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 43, 1267–1275. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.chi.0000135622.05219.bf (2004).
Marciano, L., Camerini, A.-L. & Morese, R. The developing brain in the digital era: A scoping review of structural and functional correlates of screen time in adolescence. Front. Psychol. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.67181 (2021).
Lin, H. M. et al. Structural and functional neural correlates in individuals with excessive smartphone use: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph192316277 (2022).
Sigman, A. Screen dependency disorders: A new challenge for child neurology. J. Int. Child Neurol. Assoc. https://doi.org/10.17724/jicna.2017.119 (2017).
Eirich, R. et al. Association of screen time with internalizing and externalizing behavior problems in children 12 years or younger: A systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry 79, 393–405. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2022.0155 (2022).
Westbrook, A. et al. Striatal dopamine synthesis capacity reflects smartphone social activity. iScience 24, 102497. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.isci.2021.102497 (2021).
Maza, M. T. et al. Association of habitual checking behaviors on social media with longitudinal functional brain development. JAMA Pediatr. 177, 160–167. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2022.4924 (2023).
Nagata, J. M. et al. Contemporary screen time modalities and disruptive behavior disorders in children: A prospective cohort study. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 64, 125–135. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.13673 (2023).
Rideout, V. & Robb, M. The common sense census: Media use by tweens and teens. Common Sense Media (2019).
Schmidt, S. C. et al. Physical activity and screen time of children and adolescents before and during the COVID-19 lockdown in Germany: A natural experiment. Sci. Rep. 10, 1–12. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-78438-4 (2020).
von Soest, T. et al. Adolescents’ psychosocial well-being one year after the outbreak of the COVID-19 pandemic in Norway. Nat. Hum. Behav. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-021-01255-w (2022).
Xiang, M., Zhang, Z. & Kuwahara, K. Impact of COVID-19 pandemic on children and adolescents’ lifestyle behavior larger than expected. Prog. Cardiovasc. Dis. 63, 531. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pcad.2020.04.013 (2020).
Choudhury, S. & McKinney, K. A. Digital media, the developing brain and the interpretive plasticity of neuroplasticity. Transcult. Psychiatry 50, 192–215. https://doi.org/10.1177/1363461512474623 (2013).
Christakis, D. A. The effects of infant media usage: What do we know and what should we learn?. Acta Paediatr. 98, 8–16. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1651-2227.2008.01027.x (2009).
Giedd, J. N. The digital revolution and adolescent brain evolution. J. Adolesc. Health 51, 101–105. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2012.06.002 (2012).
Meshi, D., Tamir, D. I. & Heekeren, H. R. The emerging neuroscience of social media. Trends Cogn. Sci. 19, 771–782. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2015.09.004 (2015).
Sigman, A. Visual voodoo: The biological impact of watching TV. Biologist 54, 12–17 (2007).
Bari, A. & Robbins, T. W. Inhibition and impulsivity: Behavioral and neural basis of response control. Prog. Neurobiol. 108, 44–79. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pneurobio.2013.06.005 (2013).
Vassileva, J. & Conrod, P. J. Impulsivities and addictions: A multidimensional integrative framework informing assessment and interventions for substance use disorders. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 374, 20180137. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2018.0137 (2019).
Barkley, R. A. Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychol. Bull. 121, 65. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.121.1.65 (1997).
Martinussen, R., Hayden, J., Hogg-Johnson, S. & Tannock, R. A meta-analysis of working memory impairments in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 44, 377–384. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.chi.0000153228.72591.73 (2005).
Barkley, R. A. Response inhibition in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Ment. Retard. Dev. Disabil. Res. Rev. 5, 177–184. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1098-2779(1999)5:3%3c177::AID-MRDD3%3e3.0.CO;2-G (1999).
Kasper, L. J., Alderson, R. M. & Hudec, K. L. Moderators of working memory deficits in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): A meta-analytic review. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 32, 605–617. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2012.07.001 (2012).
Pennington, B. F. & Ozonoff, S. Executive functions and developmental psychopathology. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 37, 51–87. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1996.tb01380.x (1996).
Frank, M. J., Santamaria, A., O’Reilly, R. C. & Willcutt, E. Testing computational models of dopamine and noradrenaline dysfunction in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology 32, 1583–1599. https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.npp.1301278 (2007).
Barkley, R. A., Grodzinsky, G. & DuPaul, G. J. Frontal lobe functions in attention deficit disorder with and without hyperactivity: A review and research report. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 20, 163–188 (1992).
Grodzinsky, G. M. & Diamond, R. Frontal lobe functioning in boys with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Dev. Neuropsychol. 8, 427–445. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565649209540536 (1992).
Firth, J. et al. The “online brain”: How the Internet may be changing our cognition. World Psychiatry 18, 119–129. https://doi.org/10.1002/wps.20617 (2019).
Smirni, D., Garufo, E., Di Falco, L. & Lavanco, G. The playing brain. The impact of video games on cognition and behavior in pediatric age at the time of lockdown: A systematic review. Pediatr. Rep. 13, 401–415. https://doi.org/10.3390/pediatric13030047 (2021).
Frein, S. T., Jones, S. L. & Gerow, J. E. When it comes to Facebook there may be more to bad memory than just multitasking. Comput. Hum. Behav. 29, 2179–2182. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2013.04.031 (2013).
Turel, O. & Osatuyi, B. A peer-influence perspective on compulsive social networking site use: Trait mindfulness as a double-edged sword. Comput. Hum. Behav. 77, 47–53. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2017.08.022 (2017).
Swing, E. L., Gentile, D. A., Anderson, C. A. & Walsh, D. A. Television and video game exposure and the development of attention problems. Pediatrics 126, 214–221. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2009-1508 (2010).
Johnson, J. G., Cohen, P., Kasen, S. & Brook, J. S. Extensive television viewing and the development of attention and learning difficulties during adolescence. Arch. Pediatr. Adolesc. Med. 161, 480–486. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpedi.161.5.480 (2007).
Ra, C. K. et al. Association of digital media use with subsequent symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder among adolescents. JAMA 320, 255–263. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.2018.8931 (2018).
Baumgartner, S. E., van der Schuur, W. A., Lemmens, J. S. & te Poel, F. The relationship between media multitasking and attention problems in adolescents: Results of two longitudinal studies. Hum. Commun. Res. 44, 3–30. https://doi.org/10.1093/hcre.12111 (2018).
Beyens, I., Valkenburg, P. M. & Piotrowski, J. T. Screen media use and ADHD-related behaviors: Four decades of research. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 115, 9875–9881. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1611611114 (2018).
Valkenburg, P. M., Peter, J. & Walther, J. B. Media effects: Theory and research. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 67, 315–338. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033608 (2016).
Holbert, R. L. & Stephenson, M. T. The importance of indirect effects in media effects research: Testing for mediation in structural equation modeling. J. Broadcast. Electron. Media 47, 556–572. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15506878jobem4704_5 (2003).
Conrod, P. J. & Nikolaou, K. Annual research review: On the developmental neuropsychology of substance use disorders. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 57, 371–394. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12516 (2016).
Dekkers, T. J. & van Hoorn, J. Understanding problematic social media use in adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): A narrative review and clinical recommendations. Brain Sci. 12, 1625 (2022).
American Psychiatric Association, D. & Association, A. P. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5. Vol. 5 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013).
Faraone, S. V. et al. The World Federation of ADHD International Consensus Statement: 208 evidence-based conclusions about the disorder. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 128, 789–818. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.01.022 (2021).
Yang, A. et al. Longer screen time utilization is associated with the polygenic risk for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder with mediation by brain white matter microstructure. EBioMedicine 80, 104039. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ebiom.2022.104039 (2022).
Chen, Q. & Yan, Z. Does multitasking with mobile phones affect learning? A review. Comput. Hum. Behav. 54, 34–42. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2015.07.047 (2016).
Boers, E., Afzali, M. H. & Conrod, P. A longitudinal study on the relationship between screen time and adolescent alcohol use: The mediating role of social norms. Prev. Med. 132, 105992. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2020.105992 (2020).
De Boo, G. M. & Prins, P. J. Social incompetence in children with ADHD: Possible moderators and mediators in social-skills training. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 27, 78–97. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2006.03.006 (2007).
Nixon, E. The social competence of children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A review of the literature. Child Psychol. Psychiatry Rev. 6, 172–180 (2001).
Tandon, P. S. et al. Physical activity, screen time, and sleep in children with ADHD. J. Phys. Activity Health 16, 416–422. https://doi.org/10.1123/jpah.2018-0215 (2019).
Goodman, R. Psychometric properties of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 40, 1337–1345. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-200111000-00015 (2001).
O’Leary-Barrett, M. et al. A cluster-randomized controlled trial evaluating the effects of delaying onset of adolescent substance abuse on cognitive development and addiction following a selective, personality-targeted intervention programme: The co-venture trial. Addiction 112, 1871–1881. https://doi.org/10.1111/add.13876 (2017).
Anderson, M. & Jiang, J. Teens, social media & technology 2018. Pew Res. Center 31, 1673–1689 (2018).
Woicik, P. A., Stewart, S. H., Pihl, R. O. & Conrod, P. J. The substance use risk profile scale: A scale measuring traits linked to reinforcement-specific substance use profiles. Addict. Behav. 34, 1042–1055 (2009).
Newman, J. P., Widom, C. S. & Nathan, S. Passive avoidance in syndromes of disinhibition: Psychopathy and extraversion. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 48, 1316. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.48.5.1316 (1985).
Owen, A. M., Downes, J. J., Sahakian, B. J., Polkey, C. E. & Robbins, T. W. Planning and spatial working memory following frontal lobe lesions in man. Neuropsychologia 28, 1021–1034. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(90)90137-d (1990).
Cragg, L. & Nation, K. Self-ordered pointing as a test of working memory in typically developing children. Memory 15, 526–535. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658210701390750 (2007).
Currie, C. et al. Researching health inequalities in adolescents: The development of the Health Behaviour in School-Aged Children (HBSC) family affluence scale. Soc. Sci. Med. 66, 1429–1436. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2007.11.024 (2008).
Acknowledgements
The study was supported by a grant concerning the Co-Venture program awarded by the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (FRN114887), as well as by the Dr. Julien/Marcelle and Jean Coutu Foundation Research Chair in Community Social Pediatrics, the Canada Research Chair in Preventive Mental Health and Addiction, the CFREF-IVADO grant (PRF2020) and the ERA-NET NEURON grant (JCT-2018).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
J.W., E.B., and P.C. designed the research. J.W. and E.B. did the literature research. J.W., E.B., J.O., M.A., and P.C. analyzed the data. J.W. and P.C. interpreted the data. J.W. and E.B. wrote the report. J.W. prepared figures and tables. J.O., M.A., and P.C. critically revised the report. P.C. conceived and supervised the study. All authors approved the final manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Wallace, J., Boers, E., Ouellet, J. et al. Screen time, impulsivity, neuropsychological functions and their relationship to growth in adolescent attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms. Sci Rep 13, 18108 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-023-44105-7
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-023-44105-7
This article is cited by
-
Interconnections of screen time with neuroinflammation
Molecular and Cellular Biochemistry (2024)