Introduction

The recent Corona virus (COVID-19) pandemic has been occupying mental and physical health facilities for 2 years now. Hard lockdown regulations in almost all countries early during the pandemic (April until June 2020) to prevent further spreading of the virus entail increased social isolation. The steady and massive health threat from the virus in combination with the missing social buffering effect of everyday social encounters lead to or amplified psychosocial problems that could have long-term consequences for mental and physical health1,2,3,4. E.g., loneliness, as the subjective and emotional component of social exclusion, is a highly topical and public health issue in modern societies, where social isolation and anonymity become increasingly prevalent5,6. It has been previously defined as a psychological aversive state that entails a perceived lack of intimacy or social companionship and the subjective feeling that social relationships are deficient in either quality or quantity7, which forms the basis of recent research on the topic8. By contrast, social isolation is defined as the objective state of being alone7,9. According to the belongingness-hypothesis, loneliness is rooted in the human need to socially belong, or the pervasive drive to form and maintain lasting positive and significant social relationships10. It has been shown that the sense of belonging in early adolescents is mainly achieved through the acceptance by peers, whereas in late adolescence and adulthood, it is achieved especially by romantic relationships, marital status and close friends11. On the other hand, lacking feelings of belonging are assumed to be associated with loneliness and negative physical and mental health outcomes in a long-term10. Both loneliness and social isolation are significantly related to indices of physical and mental health, such as psychosocial stress12, depression13, generalized anxiety6, cardiovascular diseases14, chronic obstructive pulmonary disease15, and mortality8,9,16,17,18,19. Chronic loneliness may hamper the formation of new social relationships by inducing negative cognitive biases such as interpersonal distrust20. Furthermore, loneliness is associated with neuroendocrine parameters, like elevated cortisol levels21,22,23 and altered cortisol awakening responses23,24. As one of the main effector hormones of the hypothalamus–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis, the steroid cortisol is secreted in response to external and internal stressors in order to re-establish homeostasis25. Previous studies suggest that cortisol may serve as a potential short-term correlate of loneliness, predicting poor physical or mental health outcomes in the long-term21,22.

According to the social buffering hypothesis26, social relationships play a beneficial role in physical and mental health26,27,28,29. Among the most intense social relationships are romantic relationships, as they serve as the primary source of support, fulfilling needs such as intimacy, attachment, and emotional support30. Supportive and affectionate interactions with the partner reduce stress, pain, and psychological distress. They even influence the immune system, wound healing or mortality rates31,32,33,34,35. Being in a relationship has been found to be associated with lower loneliness levels, compared to never-married, divorced, and widowed individuals36,37,38. Especially in the middle and higher age, romantic relationships become important buffers for loneliness39. Additionally, romantic relationships directly affect physiological stress responses, such as cortisol secretion. Individuals who are in a close relationship, show lower aggregated cortisol levels than singles40 and affectionate couple interaction can reduce cortisol levels41,42. On the other hand, the loss of a partner, for example due to breakup or death, is considered one of the most stressful life events in adulthood, being associated with reduced mental and physical health outcomes43. Divorced and widowed individuals show significantly higher loneliness scores than married individuals44,45,46. Moreover, partner loss is accompanied by altered HPA axis functioning, resulting in elevated cortisol levels and flattened diurnal cortisol slopes47.

Although being in a relationship protects against feelings of loneliness, couples can also experience higher levels of loneliness. As one important factor, relationship quality has been shown to be negatively associated with loneliness48,49,50,51,52,53. In times of extreme social isolation, relationship quality might become an important moderator, especially if couples do not live together and thus are unable to see their partner and potentially have to rely on non-physical relationship qualities. Living alone has become increasingly prevalent, with one-person households accounting for more than 40% of all households in Scandinavian nations, more than 33% of all households in France, Germany, and England; and more than 25% of all households in the United States, Russia, Canada, Spain, and Japan54. In Germany, in the young adult age of 18 to 30 years, more than 30% live without a partner55. An important distinction in this context is between partnerships with and without a common household (the latter being called “living apart together”). In general, living alone has been seen as a risk factor for poor physical and mental health54,56. For instance, the living situation predicts mortality risk57,58 and people who are living alone show higher loneliness levels59. Cross-sectional studies suggest that during the pandemic, being married served as a protective factor against loneliness60, whereas being divorced or widowed increased the risk of loneliness61. Furthermore, living with others has been found to protect against loneliness62, even when controlling for relationship status63 and loneliness during lockdown predicted psychological distress64. However, it has not been investigated yet, whether relationship status and living situation during lockdown affected biological, specifically neuroendocrine, health parameters, such as cortisol levels. In previous studies, living alone had been positively correlated with cortisol levels65. Likewise, the buffering effect of living situation and relationship status with regard to psychobiological outcomes during stress-exposure (i.e. the world-wide considerable psychological stress through COVID-19) has not been examined yet. Previous research suggests that the separation from a partner is linked to elevated feelings of loneliness and cortisol levels in general66,67,68. In adolescents, significant correlations between self-reported loneliness and cortisol awakening responses during COVID-19 lockdown were found69. Nonetheless, moment-to-moment associations of loneliness and cortisol have not been investigated in adults yet. Furthermore, it is still elusive if relationship status and living situation moderate these associations. Lastly, the effect of psychological variables such as relationship satisfaction, on the association between living arrangements and loneliness during lockdown has not yet been addressed.

Study objectives

The purpose of this study was to investigate relationship status and living situation as potential moderators for trait and state loneliness as well as momentary cortisol levels during the COVID-19 pandemic and during lockdown. We aimed to replicate findings about the association between relationship status and trait loneliness, showing that being in a relationship is associated with lowest levels of loneliness, followed by singlehood and divorce/widowhood (Hypothesis 1). In order to explore state loneliness and cortisol in every-day life, we used an ecological momentary assessment (EMA) approach. Secondly, we expected that the current living situation and relationship status have an impact on momentary (state) loneliness (Hypothesis 2) and cortisol levels (Hypothesis 3). Based on previous studies59,60,61,62,63,64,65,66,67,68,69, we assumed that being in a relationship and living with others are associated with lower loneliness and cortisol compared to being single and living alone. Additionally, we hypothesized a positive association between momentary (state) loneliness and momentary (state) cortisol levels (Hypothesis 4) and expected the relationship status and living situation to moderate this association (Hypothesis 5). Specifically, we hypothesized that being in a relationship and living with others buffers the effects of momentary loneliness on cortisol levels. Lastly, we hypothesized that relationship quality moderates the association between living situation and momentary (state) loneliness levels in individuals being in a relationship (Hypothesis 6). More precisely, we expected that the negative effect of living apart together on loneliness is buffered through high relationship quality.

Methods

Participants

This study was approved by the Heidelberg Medical Faculty’s Ethics Committee (Heidelberg University, approval no. S-214/2020) and performed in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. All participants signed an informed consent and were recruited between April 1st and July 30th 2020 via online media and local newspapers. Inclusion criteria were: Fluency in German, minimum age of 18 years and willingness to participate voluntarily. In total, 1483 individuals agreed to participate, from which 1054 participants filled out the questionnaires of interest (see Fig. 1). The mean age of the participants was M = 36.32 years (SD = 14.75, Range = 18; 81), with 77.7% being female (n = 819). Demographic characteristics are displayed in Table 1.

Figure 1
figure 1

Flowchart of the recruitment process. Note. Participants were recruited between April 1st and July 30th 2020 via online media and local newspapers. Inclusion criteria were: Fluency in German, minimum age of 18 years and willingness to participate voluntarily. In total, 1483 individuals agreed to participate, from which 1054 participants filled out the questionnaires of interest.

Table 1 Demographic characteristics of study 1 (online survey).

Of the participants in the online survey, 472 showed interest in the EMA with the salivary sampling. Of those 472 participants, 54% (n = 257) took part in the EMA study. After excluding individuals who did not react to our messages and dropouts during data collection (n = 10), the remaining 247 cases were included in the analyses. The participants’ mean age was M = 32.6 years (SD = 13.12, Range = 18; 78), with 70% being female (n = 173). Demographic characteristics of the EMA study sample are displayed in Table 2.

Table 2 Demographic characteristics of the EMA study.

Measures

Loneliness

To measure trait loneliness in the online survey, we employed the German version of the revised 20-item University of California at Los Angeles (UCLA) loneliness scale70,71. Within our study, the scale displayed high internal consistency (Cronbach’s α = .91). Participants are asked to answer, how often they felt a certain way during the past two weeks, on a 4-point Likert scale with higher scores indicating more loneliness. Exemplary items are ‘I feel isolated from others.’ or ‘I do not feel alone.’ (negatively scored item). In order to assess momentary levels of loneliness in the EMA study, we used a single item measure (“Do you feel lonely at the moment?”) with a visual analogue scale (VAS; 0—not at all, to 100—very lonely).

Salivary cortisol

Saliva samples for determination of cortisol concentrations were collected at the same times as EMA. Sampling times were adapted to the individual wake-up time. Samples were taken at six time-points on two consecutive days: directly after awakening, 30 min, 45 min, 2½ h and 8 h after awakening and immediately before going to sleep. Participants stored the samples in their freezer until collected on dry ice and stored at − 80 °C until analysis. Analyses were conducted in the biochemical laboratory at Heidelberg University Hospital’s Institute of Medical Psychology using commercial enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay (ELISA, Demeditec Diagnostics, Germany) procedures with reported detection limit of .019 ng/mL. Intra- and interassay variability for cortisol were 2.95% and 7.51% respectively. Log-transformed (ln) momentary as well as mean cortisol levels were used as outcome measures.

Relationship quality

Relationship quality was assessed via the short version of the Partnerschaftsfragebogen (PFB)72. It consists of 9 items that can be answered on a 4-point Likert scale. In our sample, the internal consistency of the PFB was very good (Cronbach’s α = .85). We used the global PFB score by adding up all items. The total score ranges between 0 and 36.

Control variables

For both trait and state loneliness as outcome, we included age and sex as control variables (CVs), as they have been previously shown to influence loneliness during the lockdown73. For momentary cortisol as outcome, CVs were assessed on both the momentary level and the trait level, based on expert consensus guidelines74,75. The following CVs were assessed on a momentary level: sleep duration, sleep quality, sleeping problems, sleep medication, forced awakening, brushing teeth, eating behaviour, drinking behaviour, medication, alcohol consumption, nicotine consumption, caffeine consumption, and physical activity (with respect to the last sample), assessment time-point (1 variable for the rise from time-point 1 to 2, and 1 variable for the fall from time-point 2 to 6), and day (1 vs. 2). Trait level control variables were age, sex, and body mass index (BMI). As the momentary level CVs were of a high number and we wanted to reach a somewhat parsimonious model, we first determined, which of the theoretically included CVs had an impact on cortisol at all. We thus run an initial hierarchical linear regression with momentary cortisol levels as outcome and all CVs as predictors. The variables that had no significant association with cortisol levels (p > .05) were excluded from our final analyses. Significant CVs for cortisol as outcome were: eating, drinking, alcohol consumption, caffeine and physical activity (yes/no). As the results of the more parsimonious model and the full model were identical, we decided to report on the parsimonious model for easier interpretation. However, both models are included in Appendix B.

Procedure

The study was part of a large-scale longitudinal study that aims to investigate long-term consequences of COVID-19 lockdown on psychobiological health. Results within this paper entail data from time-point 1 (first lockdown in Germany). The online survey as well as the EMA were both conducted with the platform soscisurvey.de and participation was completely anonymous. After completing the online survey, participants were asked whether they wanted to take part in the EMA. Those who were interested, were contacted via email. The responders received Salicap® tubes for saliva collection with additional informational documents via mail and specific instructions via phone. The assessment of the saliva samples took place between April 9th and June 3rd 2020. On two consecutive days, the participants received the respective link via SMS to a short online survey including instructions for saliva sampling six times per day. Participants were asked to refrain from food or caffeine before they provided three saliva samples which were stored in the freezer. Then, they were asked to answer further questions about their sleeping behaviour, consumption behaviour, and physical activity. Commitment was constantly monitored online: if the participants have not yet accessed the link 5 min after it was sent, they were reminded by phone to do so. After completion of the two sampling days, data were stored on an institute-internal data server and saliva samples remained in the participants’ home freezer until collection.

Data processing and statistical analyses

Hypotheses 1–3 focused on between-person effects and only included level 2 predictors (relationship status and living situation). Thus, these hypotheses were tested with analyses of covariance (ANCOVA). For hypothesis 1, family status (married/in a romantic relationship vs. single vs. divorced/widowed) served as independent variable (IV) and UCLA loneliness scores as dependent variable (DV). Post-hoc contrasts coding was conducted in order to analyse the linear trend of the means. For hypotheses 2 and 3, relationship status (single vs. in a relationship) and living situation (alone vs. with others) served as IVs. In this step we were interested in overall loneliness and cortisol in every-day life, thus the aggregated momentary loneliness and cortisol levels were used as DVs. As the distribution of the cortisol data was positively skewed, we natural-log-transformed the data in order to normalize their distribution. In case the assumptions of conducting an ANCOVA were violated, we used bootstrapping estimates (n = 1000) in order to achieve more robust results76. To test pairwise differences in momentary loneliness scores between the living situation and relationship status groups (in case the main effects were significant), we calculated Tukey Honestly Significant Differences (HSD) with Bonferroni-corrected p values adjusted for multiple comparisons. We further calculated partial η2 in order to receive the effect sizes, with η2 ≥ .01 indicating a small, η2 ≥ .06 a medium, and η2 ≥ .14 a large effect.

As hypotheses 4 and 5 included a cross-level interaction, we conducted multilevel modelling (MLM) regression analyses, which enabled us to assess the within- and between-person effects of momentary loneliness on momentary cortisol levels. By using MLM we were able to represent the hierarchical structure of the data, which was necessary in order to depict the multilevel-predictors. The individual levels of loneliness were centred on the person’s mean in order to test the within-person effect on cortisol levels. In order to assess the between-person effects, we centred the individuals’ mean loneliness levels on the grand mean. For hypothesis 5, relationship status (single vs. in a relationship) and living situation (living alone vs. living with others) were included as dichotomous moderators in order to assess their interaction with level 1 loneliness scores (the exact formulas for hypotheses 4 and 5 are displayed in Appendix A in the supplement). For hypothesis 6, we conducted an ANCOVA with the sub-dataset of participants in a relationship, using living situation (alone vs. not alone), grand-mean-centred relationship quality (PFB) and their interaction as predictors, as well as age and sex as covariates. ANCOVA analyses were conducted with SPSS Statistics Version 27 ©, whereas MLM analysis were conducted via R Version 4.0.3.

Results

In the following, we will report results from all hypotheses separately. Descriptive statistics of the outcomes of interest are shown in Tables 3 and 4, respectively.

Table 3 Means and standard deviations of the UCLA loneliness scale (online survey).
Table 4 Means and standard deviations of momentary loneliness levels (EMA study).

Trait loneliness depending on family status (Hypothesis 1)

On average, participants had a loneliness score of M = 38.95 (SD = 10.89; Range = 20–77). There was a significant effect of family status on trait loneliness after controlling for sex and age (F(1, 1035) = 26.67, p < .001, η2 = .049). Sex was significantly related to self-reported loneliness, with women showing higher loneliness scores than men (F(1, 1035) = 6.39, p = .012, η2 = .006). The subsequently planned contrasts revealed a significant linear trend (F(2, 1035) = 26.67, p < .001, η2 = .049), indicating that married people/people in a relationship displayed the lowest loneliness scores, followed by singles and divorced/widowed individuals.

Association of relationship status and living situation with loneliness in every-day life (Hypothesis 2)

Participants in the EMA study reported an overall loneliness of M = 27.36 with highly varying scores (SD = 20.94).

Results indicate significant associations of both living situation (F(1, 234) = 12.93, p < .001, partial η2 = .05) and relationship status (F(1, 234) = 8.57, p = .004, η2 = .04) with mean loneliness levels. People living alone reported significantly higher loneliness than people living with others. Also, individuals who were in a relationship reported significantly lower loneliness levels than singles. A third ANCOVA yielded a significant interaction between living situation and relationship status on mean loneliness (F(1, 233) = 7.27, p < .001; η2 = .11). Post-hoc Tukey’s HSD test indicated significant differences for the following pairwise comparisons (see Fig. 2): in a relationship living alone versus in a relationship living with partner (p = .016), single living with others versus in a relationship living with partner (p = .028), single living alone versus in a relationship living with partner (p = .001), in a relationship living alone versus in a relationship living with others (p = .056), and single living alone versus in a relationship living with others (p = .005).

Figure 2
figure 2

State loneliness levels (visual analogue scale) as a function of relationship status and living situation in the EMA study. Notes. Results of the Tukey’s HSD test assessing differences in mean loneliness levels of the EMA sample as a function of relationship status and living situation. ** represents p < .001, * represents p < .05, and # represents p < .1. Error bars depict confidence intervals based on the t-distribution.

Association of relationship status and living situation with cortisol in every-day life (Hypothesis 3)

Descriptive statistics of the variables of interest are displayed in Table 5. Mean cortisol levels in the entire EMA-sample were M = 8.6 ng/mL (SD = 2.22). Results show a significant effect of relationship status on mean cortisol levels (F(1, 219) = 4.58, p = .034, partial η2 = .02), with singles having significantly higher mean cortisol levels than individuals with a partner. Living situation did not have a significant effect on mean cortisol levels (F(1, 219) = .04, p = .840). Furthermore, BMI had a significant effect on cortisol, with higher BMI levels predicting higher cortisol levels (F(1, 219) = 15.16, p < .001).

Table 5 Means and standard deviations of salivary cortisol levels (EMA study).

Association of momentary loneliness, relationship status, and living situation with cortisol levels (Hypotheses 4 and 5)

The Intraclass Correlation Coefficient (ICC) within the empty MLM was .007, indicating that .7% of the variance in cortisol levels was accounted by between-person differences and 99.3% by within-person differences. As 22 cases had missing values on level 2 variables, a total of 225 cases and 1722 data points were included in the analyses. The random intercept and slopes model (with level 1-loneliness set as random predictor) showed a better fit to the data compared to the random intercepts-only model, (χ2(2) = 7.52, p = .020), therefore we report results from this model. There was a non-significant within-person effect of self-reported loneliness on cortisol levels (b = .002, t(1487) = 1.34, p = .179). Importantly, we observed a significant interaction between relationship status and momentary loneliness levels (b = − .004, t(1487) = − 2.88, p = .004). Therefore, the association between a person’s momentary loneliness levels and momentary cortisol levels was smaller for participants who were in a relationship than for those who were single. Pseudo R2 for this interaction was .1315, showing that the amount of unexplained variance in cortisol levels was reduced by 13.15%. The interaction between living situation and momentary loneliness levels was not significant (b = .002, t(1487) = .96, p = .361). Results of the reduced model (with significant CVs only) and the full model (with all CVs) are shown in supplementary Tables 14 in Appendix B in the supplements.

Relationship satisfaction as moderator of the associations between living arrangements and loneliness (Hypothesis 6)

In the subsample of participants who were in a relationship, participants displayed self-reported mean relationship quality of M = 20.22 (SD = 4.87; Range = 6–27). ANCOVA revealed a significant association between relationship quality and self-reported mean state loneliness levels (F(1, 149) = 5.02, p = .03, η2 = .03)). Furthermore, participants who were living alone, showed significantly higher state loneliness levels compared to participants who were living with others (F(1, 149) = 9.77, p = .002, η2 = .06). However, the interaction between relationship quality and living situation was not significant (F(1, 149) = 1.97, p = .16, η2 = .01), indicating that relationship quality did not moderate the association between living situation and loneliness.

Discussion

This study examined the (separate and joint) associations between structural (relationship status and living situation), psychological factors (relationship quality) and loneliness and cortisol during COVID-19 lockdown.

All in all, our results provide further evidence for the belongingness-hypothesis, showing that romantic relationships, as a source for meaningful interactions and intimacy, as well as living with others protect against loneliness and neuroendocrine stress-responses, in this case diurnal cortisol levels36,37,38,54,59. Moreover, divorced/widowed participants showed the highest trait loneliness, followed by singles (never-married). Thus, the loss of previously experienced positive relationship aspects such as romantic support, solace, and physical proximity, may be associated with feelings of loneliness. Furthermore, individuals who were in a relationship and living alone (“living apart together”), were lonelier than those who were living with their partner, but did not differ in their momentary loneliness levels compared to singles living alone. Being in a relationship and living with others was associated with similar levels of loneliness compared to being single and living with others. This indicates that, during extreme physical isolation and contact restrictions, having a partner per se does not protect against loneliness, but rather living with others becomes an increasingly important buffer for loneliness. As during hard lockdown, intimacy and physical closeness are lacking in couples who are living apart, these important stress-buffering factors in the romantic relationship are suddenly missing, which is experienced as aversive68. Contrary to this finding, Greenfield and Russel found higher loneliness levels in couples who were living apart but with others59. One explanation for these conflicting findings could be that during lockdown, there were no alternatives for direct social interactions outside the apartment and thus the co-habitants became an especially important substitute for any direct contact with the romantic partner. We further found that higher relationship quality predicted lower momentary loneliness levels, which is in line with cognitive approaches to loneliness assuming that quality rather than quantity of social relationships buffers short-term psychological burden. However, relationship quality did not moderate the association between living situation and loneliness. Thus, the protective effect of living together during the COVID-19 lockdown was evident irrespectively of the relationship quality. In the online survey, female participants reported significantly higher trait loneliness levels than male participants. This adds to numerous studies revealing female gender as a risk factor for loneliness77,78. Interestingly, however, recent neuroimaging studies indicate that loneliness-associated neural effects may be more pronounced in high lonely men than women79,80.

Although the results support our hypotheses about the importance of structural and psychological factors for self-reported loneliness, there are many other potential psychological mediators explaining these associations. It is important to keep in mind that romantic relationships buffer against negative mental and physical health consequences only under certain circumstances, for instance if marital functioning is perceived as positive33. Moreover, social dimensions such as perceived social proximity, knowing that there is someone you can count on, as well as actually perceived support, may be important underlying mechanisms influencing psychobiological health29.

On a neuroendocrine level, being in a relationship buffered momentary cortisol levels and their association with loneliness. This is also in line with theoretical and empirical literature indicating that having a romantic partner serves as a biological zeitgeber. It has been suggested that social interactions on a regular and high frequent basis help regulating optimal physiological stimulation levels by modulating arousal to be medium high and attenuating maladaptive stress81. These results show us that romantic relationships have a direct impact on neuroendocrine stress responses, which in a long-term may have a positive effect on health-related outcomes21,22. Contrary to our hypothesis, living arrangements by themselves neither affected cortisol levels nor moderated the association between momentary loneliness and cortisol levels. One reason why these associations were only found with relationship status, could be, that there may be operators that are unique in relationships. For instance, feelings of connectedness82, intimacy41 or affective touch83 are specific driving factors in romantic relationships. As they are not characteristic for other relationships such as co-habitants, they only come into use when romantic relationships are investigated.

This study adds to previous research on social buffering17,26,27,29 in the context of enduring stress and extreme physical isolation. As lockdown-related long-term psychological health problems are increasingly revealed, it is important to study structural and psychological factors that might influence those consequences. Likewise, short-term neuroendocrine responses during lockdown could help unravel the neurobiological mechanisms underlying detrimental effects of loneliness and social isolation for mental health. Using a psychobiological EMA design, we were able to assess not only trait loneliness levels, but also moment-to-moment variations in loneliness and salivary cortisol in a naturalistic setting. The every-day life assessments took place in the individuals’ personal environments, which yielded highly ecologically valid data. Moreover, as the participants’ current loneliness levels were directly assessed, reporting errors due to retrospective assessment could be reduced. In order to represent the hierarchical structure of the data, MLM was used, enhancing statistical power of the analyses. Moreover, due to the close supervision of the participants, we were able to keep their commitment high and thus collect high-quality data. Another strength of this study is the wide range of the participants’ age, making the sample more representative for every age group. The collection of saliva samples in the participants’ every-day life enabled us to integrate psychobiological measures and provide a multi-level view on stress experiences during COVID-19.

This study has several limitations that need to be addressed. First of all, sample sizes differed between relationship subgroups due to recruitment of a convenience sample, reducing statistical power of the analysis and potentially biasing the results. As widowers/widows and divorced individuals are on average older and less technically involved than singles, they are more difficult to recruit for an online survey. To address this problem, we analyzed the data using bootstrapping and non-parametric test. Both analyses revealed comparable results. Noteworthy, sensitivity analyses show that only medium but not small effect sizes could have been reliably detected within our sample. Thus, the results should be interpreted with caution. Another limitation is the cross-sectional design of the study, which makes it impossible to draw causal conclusions on long-term (mental) health outcomes. Furthermore, there is no baseline assessment of the variables of interest before lockdown, therefore we were not able to control for the participants’ pre-lockdown levels of loneliness and cortisol. Thus, our results can only be seen as a “snapshot” of the current situation. In addition, the data collection during this specific phase of lockdown in which the majority of participants worked from home hampers generalization of our data to other situations.

There are several aspects that could be addressed in future research. Although we found main effects of relationship status, living situation, and relationship quality, they only explained a small amount of variance in the outcomes. This indicates that there are additional predictor and moderator variables influencing the outcomes. For example, previous research has shown that level of education of the own partner has an influence on mental and physical health84. Additionally, the stress-buffering effects of close relationships is not restricted to romantic relationships. For example, having meaningful relationships with close friends or relatives38 could be one protective factor. In addition, longitudinal assessments with repeated within-person measurements of loneliness and cortisol over a longer period of time could be implemented, in order to probe long-term psychological and physiological consequences of COVID-19 and strict lockdowns.

All in all, our study reveals further evidence for romantic relationships as a protective factor against trait and state loneliness, both on a structural level (alone vs. in a relationship) and a psychological level (relationship quality), as well as momentary cortisol levels during the ongoing stress of the pandemic and social isolation. Additionally, living with others during lockdown protects against loneliness in every-day life. The fact that individuals who were living apart from their partner displayed similar levels of loneliness compared to singles, implicates that especially in times of social isolation, the lack of direct physical contact to the partner makes a difference when it comes to psychological burden. This joint role of partnership and living situation should be taken into account when analysing structural factors for negative mental health outcomes, but also identifying resources for resilience. Moreover, it is especially important to consider not only relationship status, but also relationship quality as an important psychological aspect of romantic relationships and a buffering factor for loneliness in couples, potentially counter-balancing the negative effects of living alone. This is in line with previous epidemiological research suggesting that rather than being married, it is the satisfaction with the relationship (e.g., the amount of support or criticism from a partner), which influences health-related outcomes85. In the context of clinical interventions, the results implicate that especially singles and divorced individuals, women, couples with low relationship quality as well as alone living residents (whether single or in a relationship) should be offered psychosocial support in order to prevent them from long-term negative health consequences. More importantly, on the one hand, individuals who are living apart from their partner, could profit from interventions to enhance their perceived relationship quality, on the other hand, alone living single individuals should be offered help in re-establishing meaningful social bonds with their close friends in order to counter-regulate their feelings of loneliness. Finally, public health campaigns should address and sensitize the society towards loneliness and mental health symptoms in those different groups to empower individuals to actively approach social offers and use them as resource.