Introduction

In light of decreasing volunteering rates across the nation1, and to enhance societal and individual well-being, researchers are seeking to understand and promote pathways that increase volunteering. There are four key reasons it is critical to study health and well-being factors that may predict subsequent volunteering. First, volunteers have a substantial impact on our economy. In 2018, approximately 22 million Baby Boomers volunteered 2.2 billion hours in the United States, generating $54.3 billion in value to their communities2. Second, growing evidence also suggests that volunteers themselves reap health and well-being benefits from their volunteering3. Third, the COVID-19 pandemic has brought to light substantial and persistent health disparities as the virus has not affected all sociodemographic groups equally. As inequality grows, there is a need to implement society-wide policies, health care programs, and prevention/intervention efforts that improve the lives of all members of society across the sociodemographic spectrum. While formal volunteering may not be accessible to all sociodemographic groups, for those who are willing and able to volunteer ~ 2 h per week4,5, volunteering offers a unique route to improved health and well-being. Fourth, populations are rapidly aging in many countries throughout the world6. In the United States, the population of older adults (aged ≥ 65 years) is projected to increase nearly 50% in the next 15 years7. With rapid population aging, researchers and policymakers are increasingly interested in health assets that enhance our society’s health and well-being8,9. Indeed, the health benefits of volunteering further benefit society through reduced health care costs, supplements to workforce shortages, and the promotion of health that facilitate independence and aging in place. Thus, physicians and policymakers are being encouraged to consider “prescribing” volunteering as it simultaneously benefits society and individuals4,10,11 to promote health across the age span, with consideration to local, community-based, and culturally relevant volunteering programs and initiatives12,13,14.

While a fairly large literature exists on how sociodemographic factors (e.g., age, education)11,15 predict subsequent volunteering (see Discussion), we focused on non-sociodemographic candidate predictors. Growing evidence documents how health behaviors (e.g. more physical activity)16, indicators of physical health (e.g., enhanced functional- and cognitive-functioning)17,18, psychological factors (e.g., more perceived behavioral control (i.e., perceived ease or difficulty of performing volunteering))19,20,21,22, and social factors (e.g., larger social networks, more social capital)11,15,17, have been positively associated with volunteering. However, in other studies, indicators of physical health (e.g., number of comorbidities)23 are not associated with volunteering. Further, some predictors are linked to conflicting findings (e.g., both increased depression24 and decreased depressive symptoms25,26 are associated with increased volunteering).

These existing studies have given us great insights into potential predictors of increased volunteering, but they remain limited from a causal inference point of view. First, while an increasing number of studies use longitudinal designs17, many prior studies are cross-sectional, making it difficult or impossible to address issues of potential reverse causation27. Second, most studies evaluate predictors of volunteering accumulated across the life-course, rather than changes in predictors. Thus, in these studies, we are unable to see the dynamic associations between volunteering and changes in key predictors. Third, many studies do not adequately adjust for key potential confounders. Fourth, most studies only evaluate a limited range of predictors. Fifth, many studies use data from small samples or very specific subpopulations, and results may not generalize to broader populations.

In our study, we asked a slightly different question than most past observational studies have asked: how might change in a predictor lead to change in volunteering behavior over time? To begin addressing this question, we used a new lagged exposure-wide analytic approach which is described further in the “Statistical analysis” section28. This allowed us to prospectively test if changes in 61 predictors spanning physical health, health behaviors, and psychosocial well-being factors (over a 4-year period) were associated with subsequent volunteering (4 years later). Exposure-wide analyses are a hypothesis-generating, data-driven approach aimed at discovering promising predictors of volunteering, which may then undergo further investigation in future studies. We chose these 61 predictors for four main reasons. First, because they are frequently included in the conceptualization of key gerontological models that characterize the antecedents, processes, and outcomes that foster people’s ability to age well29,30,31,32,33. Second, most of our candidate predictors have either shown evidence that they can be modified through intervention, or are likely modifiable with further research. Third was a very practical reason, that these predictors are available in the Health and Retirement Study (HRS) dataset. Many other factors (e.g., ease of transportation) may influence individuals’ ability or motivation to volunteer but are not available in the HRS dataset. Fourth, prior studies assessing predictors of volunteering have assessed micro-level contributors (incentive factors) and inhibitors (obstructive factors) of volunteering at the individual level in several key domains (health-related, psychological, etc.), creating a strong foundation for us to now assess how changes in these individual-level factors are associated with subsequent volunteering34. Future studies should further investigate other potential predictors (e.g., meso- and macro-level factors, factors available outside of HRS, etc.)34.

Methods

Study population

We used data from the Health and Retirement Study (HRS)—a national panel study of adults aged > 50 in the United States. In 2006, when psychosocial data were first collected, about half of HRS respondents completed an enhanced face-to-face (EFTF) interview (the other half of respondents were assessed in 2008). Participants then completed a psychosocial questionnaire (response rates: 88% in 2006, 84% in 2008)35 which they mailed to the University of Michigan upon completion. These sub-cohorts alternate reporting on psychosocial factors, with each participant reporting psychosocial data every 4 years. To increase sample size and statistical power, data from the 2006 and 2008 sub-cohorts were combined to create the pre-baseline wave. Participants were excluded if they did not report psychosocial data in this pre-baseline wave since over half of the study predictors were psychosocial factors. This resulted in a final sample of 13,771 participants.

We used data from three time points spaced four years apart. Covariates were assessed in the pre-baseline wave (t0; 2006/2008), candidate predictors were assessed in the baseline wave (t1; 2010/2012), and our outcome (volunteering) was assessed in the outcome wave (t2; 2014/2016). The HRS (see further study documentation on the HRS website: http://hrsonline.isr.umich.edu/) is sponsored by the National Institute on Aging (NIA U01AG009740) and conducted by the University of Michigan36. The HRS has been approved by various ethics committees, including the University of Michigan Institutional Review Board. Informed consent has been obtained from all participants. The ethics board at the University of British Columbia exempted the present study from review because it used de-identified and publicly available data. All methods were performed in accordance with the relevant guidelines and regulations.

Measures

Volunteer activity

Respondents were asked: “Have you spent any time in the past 12 months doing volunteer work for religious, educational, health-related, or other charitable organizations?” If they answered yes to this question, respondents were asked how many hours they volunteered: 1–49 h, 50–99 h, 100–199 h, or ≥ 200 h. Results for the binary volunteering variable (no volunteering vs. volunteering) are presented in the main text, while (1) the binary volunteering variable at the 100 h threshold (< 100 h vs. ≥ 100 h), (2) the 4-category ordinal volunteering variable (0 h, 1–49 h, 50–99 h, ≥ 100 h), and (3) the 3-category ordinal volunteering variable (0 h, 1–99 h, ≥ 100 h) are available in the supplementary materials. The 100 h threshold was set based on past findings which suggest that 100 volunteering h/year may be an inflection point for improved health and well-being4,5,37. We also collapsed the top two groups (100–199 h, ≥ 200 h) since only ~ 5% of the sample is in the ≥ 200 h group. The 3-category ordinal volunteering variable was added based on prior work assessing volunteering in older adults that distinguished between 0 h (none), 1–99 h (low), and medium or high (100–199 h (medium), and ≥ 200 h (high))38.

Covariates

We adjusted for a large number of covariates in the pre-baseline wave (t0; 2006/2008). Covariates included: sociodemographic factors (age (continuous), gender (male/female), race/ethnicity (White, African-American, Hispanic, Other), marital status (married/not married), income (< $50,000, $50,000–$74,999, $75,000–$99,999, ≥ $100,000), total wealth (based on quintiles of the score distribution for total wealth in this sample), educational attainment (no degree, GED/high school diploma, ≥ college degree), health insurance (yes/no), geographic region (Northeast, Midwest, South, West), personality (openness, conscientiousness, extraversion, agreeableness, neuroticism; continuous), and childhood physical abuse (yes/no)). We adjusted for prior values of all covariates and predictor variables to examine change in each predictor variable. To reduce the possibility of reverse causation, we also adjusted for pre-baseline volunteering. The Supplementary Information (Supplementary Methods) and HRS materials provide further details about each of the variables described in the “Covariates” and “Predictors” sections35,39,40.

Predictors

We evaluated 61 candidate predictors in the baseline wave (t1; 2010/2012). These included measures of: physical health (number of chronic conditions, diabetes, hypertension, stroke, cancer, heart disease, lung disease, arthritis, overweight/obesity, physical functioning limitations, cognitive impairment, chronic pain, self-rated health, eyesight, hearing), health behaviors (heavy drinking, smoking, physical activity, sleep problems), psychological well-being (positive affect, life satisfaction, optimism, purpose in life, mastery, health mastery, financial mastery), psychological distress (depression, depressive symptoms, hopelessness, negative affect, perceived constraints, anxiety, trait anger, state anger, cynical hostility, stressful life events, financial strain, daily discrimination, major discrimination), social factors (loneliness, living with a spouse/partner, frequency of contact with (1) children, (2) other family, and (3) friends, closeness with spouse, number of close (1) children, (2) other family, (3) friends, positive social support from: (1) spouse, (2) children, (3) other family, (4) friends, negative social strain from: (1) spouse, (2) children, (3) other family, (4) friends, religious service attendance, helping friends, neighbors, and relatives, social status ladder ranking, and change in social status ladder ranking), and work (in the labor force).

Multiple imputation

All missing exposures, covariates, and outcome variables were imputed using imputation by chained equations, and five datasets were created. This method may be more flexible than other methods of handling missing data41,42,43 and addresses problems that arise from attrition44,45,46,47,48,49.

Statistical analysis

We used a lagged exposure-wide analytic approach28 (see Supplementary Equations) and ran separate models for each exposure. Because volunteering was a binary outcome with a prevalence ≥ 10%, we used generalized linear models (with a log link and Poisson distribution) to individually regress volunteering in the outcome wave (t2:2014/2016) on baseline (at t1; 2010/2012) candidate predictors. All continuous predictors were standardized (mean = 0, standard deviation = 1) so that their effect sizes could be interpreted as a standard deviation change in the exposure variable. For categorical exposures, the effect estimate corresponds to associations between the exposures at baseline (t1; 2010/2012) and volunteering in the outcome wave (t2:2014/2016), conditional on the exposures in the pre-baseline wave (t0; 2006/2008). We marked multiple p-value cutoffs (including Bonferroni-corrected) in our tables as multiple testing practices vary widely and are continuously evolving and also, perhaps preferably, gave exact confidence intervals50,51.

Additional analyses

We conducted several additional analyses. First, we conducted E-value analyses to assess the minimum strength of unmeasured confounding associations on the risk ratio scale (with both the exposure and the outcome) needed to explain away the association between the exposure and outcome so as to evaluate the robustness of our results to potential unmeasured confounding52. Second, we repeated these analyses only including the subpopulation of people who volunteered at baseline (Supplementary Table 3). This identified what factors predict continued volunteering, four years later, for people who were already volunteering at baseline. Third, we repeated these analyses only including the subpopulation of people who did not volunteer at baseline (Supplementary Table 4). This identified what factors predict volunteering, four years later, for people who did not volunteer at baseline. Fourth, we repeated all analyses using the 100 h threshold binary volunteering variable (Supplementary Tables 57). Fifth, we repeated all analyses using the 4-category ordinal volunteering variable (Supplementary Tables 810). Sixth, we repeated all analyses using the 3-category ordinal volunteering variable (Supplementary Tables 1113). Finally, we re-analyzed all models using only complete-cases to assess the impact of multiple imputation on results (Supplementary Table 14).

All HRS data is publicly available (https://hrs.isr.umich.edu/data-products) and code is available upon request.

Results

In the pre-baseline wave (t0; 2006/2008), participants were on average 69 years old (SD = 10), predominantly women (58%), and married (62%). Table 1 provides the distribution of covariates in the pre-baseline wave. Supplementary Table 1 describes the changes in volunteering from the pre-baseline wave (t0) to the outcome wave (t2), and Supplementary Table 2 shows the changes in candidate predictors from the pre-baseline wave (t0) to the baseline wave (t1).

Table 1 Characteristics of participants at pre-baseline (N = 13,755)a,b,c.

Those who participated in frequent (≥ 1x/week) physical activity (at baseline t1; 2010/2012) had an 18% increased likelihood of volunteering (95% CI 1.08, 1.29), and those who reported smoking at baseline had a 21% decreased likelihood of volunteering (95% CI 0.63, 0.98) four years later (Table 2). However, there was little evidence of associations between other health behaviors (e.g., heavy drinking and sleep problems) and subsequent volunteering.

Table 2 Candidate predictors of volunteering (volunteering vs. no volunteering) (Health and Retirement Study [HRS]: N = 13,771)a,b,c.

Among physical health factors, those who reported stroke (at baseline t1; 2010/2012) were 22% less likely to volunteer (95% CI 0.62, 0.99), those with physical functioning limitations were 21% less likely to volunteer (95% CI 0.70, 0.89), and those with cognitive impairment were 23% less likely to volunteer (95% CI 0.68, 0.86) four years later. Further, for each standard deviation increase in self-rated health, participants were 7% more likely to volunteer (95% CI 1.02, 1.13). However, there was little evidence of associations between other physical health indicators (e.g., arthritis) and subsequent volunteering.

Among psychological factors, participants were more likely to volunteer for each standard deviation increase in positive affect (RR = 1.12, 95% CI 1.06, 1.17), life satisfaction (RR = 1.07, 95% CI 1.01, 1.13), purpose in life (RR = 1.10, 95% CI 1.04, 1.16), and mastery (RR = 1.05, 95% CI 1.00, 1.10) four years later. Further, participants were less likely to volunteer for every standard deviation increase in depressive symptoms (RR = 0.93, 95% CI 0.88, 0.98), hopelessness (RR = 0.94, 95% CI 0.89, 0.99), constraints (RR = 0.90, 95% CI 0.85, 0.96), anxiety (RR = 0.94, 95% CI 0.89, 0.99), and state anger (RR = 0.94, 95% CI 0.89, 1.00). There was little evidence of associations between other psychological factors (e.g., financial strain) and subsequent volunteering.

For social factors, participants who were in greater contact with friends (e.g., ≥ 3x/week) were 46% more likely to volunteer (95% CI 1.27, 1.67) four years later. For every standard deviation increase in positive social support from friends and negative social strain from friends, participants were 7% (95% CI 1.03, 1.12) and 4% (95% CI 1.00, 1.09) more likely to volunteer, respectively. Participants who frequently attended religious services (≥ 1x/week) were 130% more likely to volunteer (95% CI 1.97, 2.68), and participants who spent time helping friends, neighbors, and relatives (e.g., ≥ 200 h/year) were 30% more likely to volunteer (95% CI 1.12, 1.51). There was little evidence of associations between other social factors (e.g., loneliness) and subsequent volunteering. Employment status was also not associated with subsequent volunteering.

Additional analyses

We conducted several additional analyses. First, E-values suggested that many of the observed associations were moderately robust to unmeasured confounding (Table 3). For example, for cognitive impairment, an unmeasured confounder associated with both volunteering and cognitive impairment by risk ratios of 1.94 each (above and beyond the covariates already adjusted for) could explain away the association, but weaker joint confounding associations could not. Further, to shift the CI to include the null, an unmeasured confounder associated with both volunteering and cognitive impairment by risk ratios of 1.58 could suffice, but weaker joint confounding associations could not. Second, results were different when comparing candidate predictors for the subpopulations of people who volunteered at baseline vs. those who did not volunteer at baseline (Supplementary Tables 34). Third, results were also different comparing results when using the 100 h threshold binary volunteering variable, amongst the entire sample, only people who volunteered regularly at baseline, and only people who did not volunteer regularly at baseline, (Supplementary Tables 57). Fourth, results with the 4-category ordinal coding of the volunteering variable (Supplementary Tables 810) and the 3-category ordinal coding (Supplementary Tables 1113) show associations between candidate predictors and subsequent volunteering on an ordinal scale. Fifth, complete-case analyses showed similar results to those from the main imputed analyses (Supplementary Table 14).

Table 3 Robustness to unmeasured confounding (E-values) for the associations between candidate antecedents and subsequent volunteering (N = 13,771)a.

Discussion

In a large, longitudinal, and national sample of U.S. adults aged > 50, we examined the associations between changes in 61 candidate predictors (across physical health, health behaviors, and psychosocial factors) and subsequent volunteering. Some health behaviors (e.g., frequent physical activity), physical health conditions (e.g., physical functioning limitations), psychological well-being (e.g., purpose in life), psychological distress (e.g., constraints), and social factors (e.g., contact with friends) were associated with volunteering four years later. However, there was little evidence of associations between other health behaviors (e.g., sleep problems), physical health conditions (e.g., heart disease), psychosocial factors (e.g., loneliness), employment (e.g., work), and subsequent volunteering. Specifically, the candidate antecedents of greatest predictive importance included physical activity, smoking, stroke, physical functioning limitations, cognitive impairment, sense of purpose in life, positive affect, contact with friends, religious service attendance, and helping friends, neighbors, and relatives, with religious service attendance and contact with friends having the largest effect sizes. It appears that regardless of an individual’s psychological distress (e.g., depression), fostering positive psychological factors (purpose in life) might still increase subsequent volunteering (and vice versa), and likewise regardless of loneliness, fostering greater social engagement might increase volunteering53.

Our findings converge with previous studies that observed how increased physical activity16, less functional limitations17, less cognitive impairment18, and increased religious service attendance17 were positively associated with volunteering. The latter finding, given it has the largest effect size and is consistent with a large prior body of research17, may be of special interest insofar as religious communities represent an important social setting for volunteer mobilization and understanding why this is the case would offer many lessons for other social institutions that might be mobilized to expand volunteering54,55. While religious service attendance does provide an additional context in which people can volunteer, the correlation between religious service attendance and volunteering was lower than some might expect. In our study, among people who regularly attended religious services in the baseline wave (≥ 1x/week), only 45% engaged in volunteering in the outcome wave, with fewer than 20% volunteering ≥ 100 h/year. In prior work, there is reciprocity between religiosity and volunteering, but the association of religious volunteering on religiosity is stronger than the other way around56. Further, religious service attendance and secular volunteering are reciprocally related, but the reciprocal relation between them is weaker than that of religious volunteering and religious service attendance56.

Our findings further converged with previous research that has observed that number of comorbidities is not associated with volunteering23. However, our findings diverge from prior work which observed that other factors (e.g., larger social network size)17 are associated with increased volunteering. There are many potential reasons for these discrepancies, including differences in: (1) study design (e.g., different follow-up periods), (2) sample composition, (3) measurement and categorization of exposure (e.g., we assessed changes in predictors) and outcome variables (e.g., measuring intention to volunteer vs. actual volunteering), and (4) covariate measurement (we used more extensive covariate adjustment than most previous studies). For example, while previous studies have observed that people with a higher perceived ease of volunteering have increased intentions of volunteering19,20,21,22, many of these studies are about intention or motivation to volunteer, rather than actual volunteering, which may explain discrepancies in our findings. Further, prior studies have observed that both increased depression and decreased depressive symptoms are associated with increased volunteering24,25,26. While depressive symptoms were associated with a decreased risk of volunteering in our study, depressive symptoms may both decrease volunteering (due to depressive symptoms acting as a barrier to continued volunteering) and increase volunteering (as older adults attempt to compensate for an increased risk of depression derived from role losses and functional decline by volunteering)24,26, which may explain our null results for depression.

These diverging results also highlight the importance of several future directions. First, future studies should consider important social structural moderators of the associations between candidate antecedents and volunteering. Prior research has shown that volunteering rates differ across key social structural moderators (e.g., age, race, gender, country, social class, education)11,15,57. Women are slightly more likely to volunteer than men in North America15. Given the importance of purpose in life in predicting volunteering in our study, and the social factors that encourage women in the United States to both develop a stronger sense of purpose and perform more altruistic service overall (including for family and friends and not limited to formal volunteering), this finding is not surprising58. However, further research is needed as these findings are often mixed or ambiguous and variation is apparent across nations (e.g., volunteering rates based on gender may differ based on life stage, with women volunteering more than men at earlier stages in life, and this pattern reversing among older adults; women volunteer more than men in some countries and less than men in others)15. Second, there are many other factors that might predict volunteering that we could not assess in the HRS dataset (e.g., various motivational factors)11,59,60. For instance, there is evidence that altruism can potentially be intervened upon61 which is one potential way we could increase volunteering. Further, there are nuances within our list of predictors (e.g., types of social contact including virtual methods that may be more relevant during this time of COVID-19)62 which were not assessed in the HRS dataset. Third, future studies may benefit from assessing predictors of different types of volunteering (e.g., intergenerational volunteering63,64, grandparenting roles65, etc.) since different modes of volunteering may differentially influence well-being on an individual level and have different societal implications66. Indeed, associations between some predictors (e.g., religious service attendance) and volunteering may differ based on the type of volunteering (e.g., religious or secular) being done56. Fourth, our volunteering measure contained broad categories of the number of hours spent volunteering and future studies should investigate more nuanced volunteering categories. For older and mostly retired adults, the 100 h threshold might not be as relevant as it is for still-working adults (e.g., older adults may benefit from 4 h per week, rather than 2)5. Fifth, future studies should consider mechanistic pathways that explain associations between key predictors in our study (e.g., frequent physical activity) and subsequent volunteering to facilitate increased volunteering. Investigating these mechanistic pathways may explain why some relationship types (e.g., contact with friends) are associated with subsequent volunteering while others (e.g., contact with children) are not. Sixth, examining predictors of volunteering with consideration to relevant life stage transitions and major life events (e.g., changes to caregiver status67,68 or employment25,69 (e.g., retirement, part-time work, etc.)) may inform volunteer engagement program designs by incorporating time-, situational-, or life event-based approaches to increasing volunteering11.

Although there was some variability in the findings reported in the Supplementary Information, which included alternative ordinal outcome measures and limiting the sample to either those with previous volunteer experience or none at all, several variables predicted volunteering across all models. Religious service attendance was perhaps the most consistent. This variable predicted increased volunteering for both previous volunteers and non-volunteers alike, a finding that further underscores the importance of religious settings as a potential site for the study of the social organization of benevolent service to others, and therefore a strategic setting for the advancement of the emerging science of well-doing70. Our supplemental analyses also highlight the role of friendship networks in predicting volunteering. Serving others is perhaps most satisfying and positively reinforcing when done in the company of friends, even for those with no previous history of volunteering. Notably, some objective indicators of social contact (e.g., contact with friends) were associated with subsequent volunteering, while some subjective indicators (loneliness) were not. Loneliness is the subjective perception of feeling socially disconnected71, whereas social isolation is the objective lack of social interactions (e.g., smaller social network)72. Objective and subjective factors may operate through different mechanisms to influence subsequent volunteering engagement. While frequent contact with friends might encourage joining in on group activities (such as volunteering), the experience of loneliness might not directly predict future volunteering. Perhaps, increased confidence in social skill development may be needed to encourage volunteering in those with high loneliness. However, prior work has found that volunteering is associated with subsequently decreased loneliness37, suggesting that while increased loneliness might not lead to volunteering engagement, volunteering may decrease subsequent loneliness. More generally, there is great value in understanding the factors that help shape whether an individual will shift from not volunteering at all to volunteering at a low level, especially if this indicates the beginning of a longer-term trajectory that might result in that person eventually crossing the 100 h/year threshold4,5. But any increase in volunteering is potentially beneficial to those being served, regardless of whether the volunteer experiences greater health or well-being. Supplementary Tables 810 reveal a broader range of predictors, and therefore more opportunities for social policy interventions to increase volunteering, than we found in our primary analysis. Consideration of these results also permit policy makers to target interventions towards those who have (or have not) had previous volunteering experience.

There is evidence for bidirectional associations between volunteering and health and well-being factors. Many prior studies have assessed how volunteering is associated with subsequent health and well-being outcomes. Observational studies show that volunteering is associated with reduced risk of several chronic conditions (e.g., cognitive impairment, cardiovascular disease)73,74, and mortality37,60,75,76,77,78,79,80. Volunteering has been repeatedly associated with well-being3, and several mechanisms are hypothesized to underlie the health benefits of volunteering, including the cultivation of psychological well-being (e.g., positive affect, purpose in life, self-efficacy, and reduced depression)37,75,81,82,83,84 and social well-being (e.g., reduced loneliness and increased frequency of contact with friends)37. In turn, these assets might promote healthier behaviors (e.g., increased: use of preventive healthcare services and physical activity)37,85 and better biological functioning (e.g., lower: blood pressure and inflammation)86,87. Results from experimental volunteering studies generally converge with results from observational studies (e.g., healthier: physical health (physical function, cognitive/neural function)88,89,90,91,92, biological functioning (healthier inflammation- and cholesterol-levels)93,94, health behaviors (more physical activity)90,95,96,97, and psychosocial health (higher social integration, lower depression))88,90. While these prior studies have been interested in how volunteering is associated with subsequent health and well-being outcomes, our study assessed how health and well-being predictors were associated with subsequent volunteering. Prior work in the HRS, using an outcome-wide analytic approach, assessed how changes in volunteering (between t0: 2006/2008 and t1: 2010/2012) were associated with 34 indicators of physical-, behavioral-, and psychosocial health and well-being four years later37. They observed that volunteering was associated with improved physical- (e.g., reduced risk of physical functioning limitations), behavioral- (e.g., higher physical activity), and psychosocial- (e.g., lower loneliness) health and well-being outcomes. Conversely, we assessed how changes in indicators of physical-, behavioral-, and psychosocial-factors (between t0: 2006/2008 and t1: 2010/2012) were associated with subsequent volunteering. Some key factors associated with volunteering in the previously mentioned outcome-wide study (e.g., physical functioning limitations, physical activity) were also predictors of increased subsequent volunteering, suggesting potential bidirectional associations between some indicators of health and well-being and volunteering. Other studies have found further evidence of potential bidirectional associations between volunteering and health and well-being factors. For example, net of cognitive functioning selection into volunteering, volunteering has been associated with benefits to cognitive function38. Future research should further investigate these bi-directional associations.

Our study had several limitations. First, many physical health outcomes, health behaviors, and our measure of volunteering were self-reported in the HRS and thus these measures are vulnerable to self-report bias. However, study participants were unaware of this study’s hypothesis at the time of the study. Second, there is potentially unmeasured confounding. However, we mitigated concerns of unmeasured confounding with robust covariate adjustment, use of a longitudinal design, and E-value analyses. Third, we were unable to evaluate several other potential predictors of volunteering because they were not included in the HRS dataset (e.g., type of culture (independent vs. interdependent), country, the presence of social contacts who volunteer, motivation to volunteer (e.g., extrinsic motivation from an organization, such as church, work, or school, vs. intrinsic motivation to help others), social determinants of health (e.g., ease of transportation) that might impact volunteer engagement, etc.). But our study had several strengths, including the use of a large, diverse, prospective, and national sample of U.S. adults aged > 50 years.

The age group of our sample (older US adults aged > 50) likely plays an important role in our findings. Some of our findings might be understood through the lens of role theory and the role accumulation hypothesis98, which hypothesizes that our roles in society and transitions between them (e.g., retirement) exert a powerful influence on our lives as they provide an underlying architecture of societal expectations and reciprocal obligations which elicit new behavioral demands. Certain predictors (e.g., changes to physical health such as physical functioning limitations), are more salient in older populations, while others (e.g., having children younger than 18 years)99 are less important in this age group. Likewise, depending on the life course stage, some social relationships (e.g., friends) may be more important than family, which is consistent with our findings that contact with friends was associated with subsequent volunteering, but contact with other family was not. Major life stage transitions may be more relevant in older populations. For example, the effect of employment on volunteering may be influenced by life stage, such that those experiencing work-retirement transitions may be more involved in volunteering than non-retired populations100, and other new roles in later life (e.g., caregiving) may influence later life volunteering67. Future work will need to continue to investigate factors at the micro-, meso-, and macro-levels that predict volunteering specifically in older populations, predictors of volunteering across the lifespan, and predictors that may be more or less salient at different points during the life course34.

As our nations pause and reevaluate our priorities in light of the widespread change that COVID-19 has caused, our public and our policymakers have a rare opportunity to pursue new courageous directions that can simultaneously alleviate important social problems and also help larger portions of our rapidly aging society experience healthy aging. One promising direction is the enactment of bold policies and civic structures, as well as innovative volunteer program designs, that encourage more volunteering among older adults. Many of our study predictors (e.g., positive psychological well-being factors) may be targeted as potential intervention factors to increase subsequent volunteering. For example, there is evidence of life satisfaction being increased through both individual- and population-level interventions and policies101,102,103,104,105. A better understanding of how (and why) religious service attendance is so effective at motivating and sustaining volunteering might help support the role of these institutions and also provide clues for the creation of successful non-religious public policies17,54. Our aging population represents one of our nation’s only “increasing natural resources,” and this group possesses the skills, experiences, and desire to remain highly productive and contributing members of society64. The purpose of our study was to draw attention to specific physical-, behavioral-, and psychosocial-factors that might increase volunteering in older adults. We aimed to explore a more comprehensive pool of options that future investigators might consider as determinants of volunteering. Our study highlights key intervention targets that volunteering program designers may consider in recruiting older volunteers and designing sustainable volunteering opportunities for older adults. Likewise, innovators in the volunteering industry may further investigate these potential intervention targets in predictive models and engagement growth opportunities. As we consider elevating increased volunteering as an important policy and public health goal, our study highlights key intervention targets that governments, volunteering organizations, and corporations can aim at to increase volunteering rates in our nation. Increasing the volunteer workforce may benefit individuals and caregivers (e.g., increasing the healthspan, promoting aging in place), communities (e.g., increasing social cohesion), and the economy (e.g., benefiting communities and reducing health care costs).