Introduction

Male wing vibration is a behavior linked to courtship in many Tephritid flies1 like Bactrocera cucurbitae2, Bactrocera tryoni3, Anastrepha suspensa4 and Ceratitis capitata5 that spread male pheromone with this behavior. Wing sexual dimorphism in Tephritidae -where the male wing possesses microtrichia along the Cu + A1 vein and is also wider than the female wing- is considered an adaptation to serve this purpose6,7.

For Bactrocera oleae, the sound produced by wing vibration/stridulation of microtrichia against the abdominal pecten is audible to the human ear and has been described and recorded8,9,10. However, the role of this produced sound on mating and the reproductive success of B. oleae, if any, has not yet been clarified. The courtship sounds of C. capitata11, B. tryoni12, B. cucurbitae13 and A. suspensa14 have been recorded and analyzed acoustically. Broadcasts of recorded calling sounds of C. capitata males elicit increased captures of females in traps15. Caribbean fruit fly (A. suspensa) females respond to the calling sounds of a male conspecific and their response was enhanced when they had been previously exposed to the male pheromone16. Acoustical parameters of wing vibration can differ according to the social context i.e., the presence of a conspecific in A. suspensa17 and in the parasitoid wasp Psyttalia concolor18.

In tephritid flies, physical and olfactory cues play an important role during courtship and mating rituals. Especially olfactory stimuli are crucial during the mate-searching phase1. The male wing vibration/stridulation is associated with the production of visual, olfactory, and auditory stimuli19,20, in B. dorsalis21, B. tryoni22, B. cucurbitae23 and B. oleae1. The release and perception of semiochemicals are often accompanied by a range of intense behavioral interactions, including wing vibration, buzzing and head rocking24.

Physical cues (i.e. semiophysicals) may include substrate-borne vibrations, sounds, lights and colors25. Bands and spots on the wings of many tephritid species are visual cues that, particularly when sexually dimorphic, could play a role in courtship and mating sequences. Also, other body parts that are brightly colored and/or patterned could have communicative functions in sexual behaviour1. In mating communication, vibrational signals allow the expression of many behavioral traits that also carry information of individual fitness. A species can produce a variety of signals, which are characterized by certain spectral and temporal features that eventually drive mate choice. Signals are also significant in intra-sexual competition (rivalry between males), in inter-species or antagonistic interactions26.

The olive fruit fly has been an important pest of olives in Mediterranean countries for at least 2000 years27 and in 1998 it was first detected in California28. The distribution of the pest now covers the Mediterranean basin, north and sub-Saharan Africa, south-west Asia and North America29. Mating in B. oleae occurs during the last hours of the photophase and at that time high-pitched sounds have been recorded in cages with males30.

It is common in tephritid male flies to fight for territories before the initiation of courtship behavior31. Olive fruit fly males form swarms on the windward side of trees in late afternoon and within the swarm, displays of aggressive territoriality take place between males in order to exclude male conspecifics before the beginning of courtship displays. Behaviors like synchronous wing waving, fast running towards the opponent, pouncing and boxing on the head and thorax of the opponent are part of the aggressive actions between males32. Tephritid males, when engaging in synchronous wing waving, hold their wings perpendicular to the long axis of their body and move them up and down repeatedly, often while moving from side to side in front of the other male (a behavior that differs greatly from the wing vibration during courtship)33. In B. tryoni, male synchronous wing waving is a highly significant behavioral difference between successful and unsuccessful males and may represent either a simplified courtship dance used by females to recognize males, or an indication of male activity that improves their mating success34.

Clarifying the involvement of male sounds in the courtship ritual, mating and reproductive success could be crucial for the improvement of the mass rearing of the fly and the application of the SIT method35,36.

The mechanism of sound perception in Tephritid flies is unknown. In mosquitoes and Drosophila species auditory stimuli are received by their antennas, which act as acoustic oscillators37. They can detect the particle velocity component of the sound, which attenuates rapidly with increasing distance from the source. Therefore, insects with antennal hearing have evolved to detect low-frequency sound sources in the near field (some tens of centimeters)38. In this type of acoustic signal, the temporal components of the sound are important for species recognition and sexual selection39. In the olive fruit fly, courtship with wing vibrations of higher frequency resulted in successful matings. The parameters of male olive fruit fly wing vibration were acquired via video captures and frame-by-frame analysis7.

In the present work, our aim was firstly to obtain a high-quality recording of the sound produced by the male olive fruit fly wing vibration/stridulation and analyze the air-borne component of the vibration. We then tested in broadcasting bioassays the attraction and arrestment effect of the sound on the olive fruit flies. Secondly, we explored how the male’s wing vibration behavior is affected by the presence of conspecifics and we compared the acoustic parameters of sounds produced by solitary males with those produced in the presence of a male or female conspecific.

Results

Test 1: Audio recording of male wing vibration/stridulation and flight sounds

The sound produced by the male wing vibration/stridulation consisted of a series of intermittent pulses of a duration ranging from 0.06 to 1.58 s and of a fundamental frequency ranging from 300 to 394 Hz. The fundamental frequency was also the dominant frequency (the one with the maximum power level) and there were higher harmonics of lower power, at about 800 Hz, 1200 Hz, up to 10 kHz. (Fig. 1). While no signals were found in the females’ box, the males’ only box had signaling behavior. The mean fundamental frequency of the flight sound was similar between males (180.94 Hz) and females (179.10 Hz) and they did not differ significantly (t = 0.662, P = 0.516, n = 30). The waveform, spectrogram and frequency analysis are shown in Supplementary Fig. S3 in Supporting Information.

Figure 1
figure 1

Waveform (a), spectrogram (b), and frequency analysis (c) of the sound produced by the male olive fruit fly vibration/stridulation. In (a) the sound waveform of eight pulses is shown of about 0.15 s duration each. Pulse duration (PD) and interpulse interval (IPI) are depicted with bars. The horizontal axis corresponds to time (s) and the vertical to the relative amplitude of the sound in decibels (dB). In (b) the spectrogram of the same sound is shown, where the vertical axis corresponds to frequency (in kHz) and the horizontal to time (s). The more intense colored areas of the spectrogram are related to the higher power of the corresponding sound. The maximum power is concentrated at about 380 Hz which is the fundamental frequency (FF) of the sound. In (c) the frequency analysis of the sound is shown, where the vertical axis corresponds to the relative power (in dB) and the horizontal axis to the frequency (Hz). The first peak of the spectrum is the fundamental frequency of the sound, at about 380 Hz, followed by harmonics of lower power, at approximately 600 Hz, 900 Hz and 1200 Hz.

Test 2: Response of olive fruit flies to broadcasts of recorded wing vibration

There was no significant statistical difference between the mean number of flies that remained at the area near the sound source (7.75 ± 1.68 females, 9.40 ± 2.77 males) and at the opposite side of the experimental arena (8.85 ± 1.92 females, 7.70 ± 3.01 males) after 1 h of broadcast bioassay for both sexes (t = 1.923, P = 0.06 for females, t = − 1.855, P = 0.07 for males, n = 20 for each sex).

Test 3: Wing sexual dimorphism

Our experiments showed that the mean wing length was significantly longer in females (4.59 ± 0.22 mm) than in males (4.49 ± 0.15 mm) (t = 3.233, P = 0.003, n = 30). Wing surface (size) was significantly larger in adult females (6.14 ± 0.54 mm2) than in males (5.87 ± 0.32 mm2) (t = 3.241, P = 0.003, n = 30). Yet, the wing width did not differ between females (2.24 ± 0.09 mm) and males (2.27 ± 0.12 mm). However, the curvature index of the Cu1 cell was significantly longer in males (143.88 ± 10.21 mm) than in females (104.16 ± 11.81 mm) (t = 6.238, P < 0.0001, n = 30) (Table 1 and Supplementary Fig. S1 and S2 in Supporting information).

Table 1 Anatomical characteristics of male and female wings of B. oleae.

Test 4: Observation of male wing vibration behavior and mating in relation to age and time of day

Male olive fruit flies engaged in wing vibration behavior only in the late afternoon hours (18:00–20:00). No wing vibration was observed in the morning or early afternoon hours. The behavior started from the 4th day after adult emergence when 4% (95% CI 1–9%) of males in male–male pairs were engaging in wing vibration. Maximum occurrence of wing vibrating males occurred on the 10th day of age, when 68% (95% CI 58–76%) of males were engaging in this behavior, which continued until the 12th day of age (Fig. 2).

Figure 2
figure 2

Percentages (mean and 95% CI) of wing vibrating males (a) and mating percentages (b) in different days of age.

On the 8th day of age, we observed 10% matings and a maximum of 56% matings was reached on the 12th day of age (Fig. 2). The time that each mating started during the day was also noted (Supporting information, Supplementary Fig. S4). First matings occurred 4 h before the end of the photophase and the maximum was reached 2–2.5 h later.

Test 5: Male wing vibration parameters and behaviors in different social situations

In single males, 4% (95% CI: 1–9%) of the individuals were observed engaging in wing vibration during the last 2 h of the photophase and the mean time spent signaling was 113.00 ± 15.55 s. In the presence of a virgin female, 28% (95% CI: 20–37%) of males were observed engaging in wing vibration for a mean time of 94.70 ± 65.79 s. Copulations followed quickly in the majority of observations, but when females were not immediately receptive for mating, the males were seen wing-vibrating in several observations and attempted unsuccessful copulations. In the presence of a mated female, 36% (95% CI: 27–45%) of males were observed wing vibrating for a mean time of 100.55 ± 97.60 s and no copulations occurred. In male–male pairs, 66% (95% CI 56–74%) of males engaged in wing vibration for a mean time of 145.73 ± 72.13 s (Fig. 3).

Figure 3
figure 3

Percentages (mean and 95% CI) of wing vibrating males (a) and boxplots of duration of wing vibration (b) in different social situations.

Rivalry behaviors like fighting (including boxing with front legs, often followed by dropping to the bottom of the cage) were observed in 70% (95% CI: 60–78%) of the male–male pairs. 48% (95% CI: 38–57%) of the males attempted copulation with the other males. 30% (95% CI: 21–39%) of the males were observed doing the synchronous wing waving behavior, while they were at opposite positions and with eye contact with each other (Table 2). In single males, the majority were stationary during the observation period, engaging in feeding or cleaning activities and only an extremely low proportion of 4% (95% CI: 1–9%) were seen wing vibrating at 1–2 occasions.

Table 2 Proportion (mean and CI) of males exhibiting antagonistic behaviors in male–male pairs.

The mean fundamental frequency (FF) varied depending on the social context of the male i.e., mean (± SD) fundamental frequency was low in single males (324.40 ± 17.10 Hz), increased when males were maintained in pairs with mated or virgin females (352.93 ± 11.43 Hz and 347.68 ± 19.27 Hz respectively) and was very high when males were maintained with other males (365.23 ± 15.78 Hz). One-way Anova provided evidence of significant differences in fundamental frequency according to the social context (F = 11.58, df = 3, P > 0.0001). Tukey post-hoc test showed that the FF of wing vibration in male–male pairs was higher than in male-virgin female pairs and solitary males (Table 3).

Table 3 Fundamental frequency (FF), pulse duration (PD), interpulse interval (IPI) (mean (SD), median, range and sample size) of the sound produced by the wing vibration/stridulation of a single virgin male and in the presence of a mated female, a virgin female and a male.

The mean (± SD) pulse duration (PD) varied from 0.15 ± 0.06 s for single males, 0.33 ± 0.38 s for males with mated females, 0.28 ± 0.36 s for males with virgin females and 0.13 ± 0.07 s for male–male pairs. Kruskal–Wallis test showed a slightly significant difference between treatments (H = 7.990, df = 3, P = 0.0462). Mann–Whitney post-hoc test for pairwise comparisons showed that PD in male–male pairs was shorter than in male–female pairs (mated or virgin) (Table 3).

The mean (± SD) interpulse interval (IPI) was very low (0.27 ± 0.07 s) in the presence of another male and increased in the presence of a mated (0.49 ± 0.46 s) or virgin female (0.49 ± 0.46 s) or solitary males (0.33 ± 0.06 s). Kruskal–Wallis test showed that there was a significant difference between treatments (H = 11.879, df = 3, P = 0.0078) and Mann–Whitney post-hoc test for pairwise comparisons showed that IPI was of shorter duration in male–male pairs than in male–female pairs (mated or virgin) (Table 3).

Discussion

In our work, we present a for the first time a high-quality digital audio recording of this male-produced sound of B. oleae and describe its temporal and spectral characteristics. The sound produced by wing vibration/stridulation is different than that of the flight wingbeat and consists of trains of pulses of variable duration and fundamental frequency (FF) that can vary from 300 to 390 Hz, as it was observed for B. cucurbitae40. In addition, higher harmonics are also present at multiples of the FF, as it was reported by Feron and Andrieu9 and Rolli10. Interestingly, male flies were engaging in wing vibration/stridulation even without the presence of female flies in their proximity.

Sexual communication in Tephritidae is multimodal and the auditory stimuli alone did not prove enough to elicit a behavioral response. In our experiments no phonotaxis to the broadcasts was observed. However, it has recently been recognized that communication by substrate-borne vibrations is the most widespread channel of communication in arthropods25. In parasitic Hymenoptera, wing fanning performed by males may act as a vibrational stimulus to quieten the female and triggering sexual receptivity41. Pea leafminers (Liriomyza huidobrensis) produce vibrational signals that convey efficient information to the opposite sex and result in pair formations on substrates, like plant leaves42. Also, recent studies on Drosophila melanogaster and its two sibling species, revealed that males quiver their abdomens and generate substrate-borne signals to induce female immobility and to enhance the receptivity of females to copulation43. For the olive fruit fly, it is possible that during courtship displays on leaves, where attraction could involve visual, olfactory and acoustic cues, vibrations are transmitted through plant surfaces and detected by the legs44. It would therefore be interesting to assess whether the signal of male wing vibration that we tested can be attractive to male and female olive fruit flies by using techniques of Biotremology45.

The temporal characteristics of produced sound vary depending on the presence of conspecific flies and the pulse duration was from 0.10 to 1.58 s. According to Miyatake and Kanmiya13 longer pulse durations are related to male pheromone spraying after its release on the body surface. Similarly, in the braconid parasitoid wasp P. concolor, pulse durations were longer in the presence of conspecific females18. The variation in the male pulse duration of B. oleae in the presence of conspecific individuals could be related to the increased effort exerted by the male when it is near a potential partner.

The sound produced by the wing vibration/stridulation of males may be related to the unique anatomy of their wings compared to females. Earlier studies by Benelli et al7 have shown that the total wing size is larger in males than females. By contrast, our results show that the total size of the wings is larger in females than in males, and perhaps this is a regional peculiarity. However, the posterior part of the male wings is larger than the respective part in female wings which may facilitate sound production. This posterior part of the wings is actively involved in sound production by stridulation of microtrichia present on the A1 + CuA2 vein on the pecten of the abdominal tergite (see Supplementary Fig. S1 in Supporting information). Females lack wing microtrichia and abdominal pecten and do not produce any sounds by wing vibration. On the other hand, females are heavier than males and their larger wings may facilitate their flying.

Females of B. oleae have immature oocytes in their ovaries in the first 3 to 5 days of their adult life and during this period do not mate. Mating occurs after this period and reaches a maximum on the 10th day of their age46. Virgin females produce a multi-component sex pheromone containing four constituents with a synergistic action: 1,7-dioxaspiro[5.5]undecane (DSU) and methyl dodecanoate, a-pinene and nonanal. However, among these compounds, DSU is reported as the most abundant component and exhibits the highest biological activity towards males. Interestingly, young B. oleae males also produce DSU in their rectal glands and production of DSU starts from the 1st day after adult emergence, reaches a maximum when gonad maturation is complete, and then decreases to zero by the 11th day. When olive fruit fly males become sexually mature, they start to produce (Z)-9-tricosene, a compound unique to males, which is able to selectively attract females during the close-range phase only1,47,48. Here, we show that males begin to perform wing vibration after the first 5 days of their adult life which corresponds with females becoming receptive to mating. Therefore, we consider that the sound produced by male wing vibration is linked to mating. This hypothesis is supported by the fact that wing vibration and mating occur only in the last 4 h of the photophase30. In addition, in the presence of females (either virgin or mated), the percentage of males exhibiting wing vibration increases to about 30%. This increase in the percentage of males showing wing vibration in the presence of females was also indicated by Benelli et al7 and supports our hypothesis concerning the role of wing vibration in mating.

We found that the fundamental frequency of the pulses varies and is higher in the presence of another male and lower in the absence of another conspecific of either sex. Yet, a higher proportion of males performed the wing vibration when they were together with another male than alone in the cage. This could be due, among other reasons, to an intragroup stimulation to perform the same behavior, as was demonstrated in C. capitata49. In B. cucurbitae50 83% of the males observed in the field were seen wing vibrating when other calling males were in proximity. Males exert more effort for wing vibration and stridulation when they are in groups and this increased wingbeat results in higher fundamental frequencies of the produced sound. In addition, we should not exclude the possibility that this particular sound production, with specific pulse characteristics in the presence of another male, may be related to a territoriality behavior between males. In B. curcubitae, when a calling male encounters another male that also engages in wing vibration, each male continues to vibrate the wings before engaging in additional actions (blows with mid and fore legs, head thrusts, attempting mounts)50. For some Bactrocera species, Keiser et al.51 speculated that males may display wing vibration as a territorial behavior during mating. In olive fruit flies, aggressions between males could serve to maintain territories in which each male fly can perform courtship displays. Aggressive behaviors have been also observed and described between olive fruit fly females on oviposition sites52. On the other hand, when in solitary conditions, males demonstrate their courtship behavior with reduced effort, perhaps as a form of training, since in the absence of a conspecific in the proximity, there is a lack of stimuli to provoke this type of behavior. This is in accordance with our results that wing vibration occurs in higher numbers and the fundamental frequency of the produced sound is higher in males maintained in male–male pairs than in single ones.

In conclusion, our research has shown that the sound produced by male wing vibration/stridulation of microtrichia in the abdominal pecten has specific quality characteristics and is quite different than the flight sound. Stridulations and wing vibrations have different spectrogram patterns. The spectrogram in Fig. 1 suggests that most of the acoustic energy in the B. oleae signal is concentrated around the fundamental frequency of the wing vibration (300 Hz) and the multiples of the wingbeat harmonics (600 Hz, 900 Hz, 1200 Hz) and decreases rapidly away from the harmonic frequencies. Stridulations, in contrast, have most energy at the harmonics of tooth impact but the energy is fairly uniformly distributed in between (see e.g., the spectrograms of calls from two Neoscapteriscus mole cricket species in Rohde et al53). The difference in energy output suggests that the microtrichia and abdominal pecten probably do not contribute significantly to the acoustic signal.

Further experiments are now running to consider the ecological significance of this behavior, which, as in other tephritid flies, may be mainly related to the involvement of the lekking system in mating54,55,56. However, the role of the presence of conspecific males and their positive effect on sound production remains unknown. Future research in Tephritidae should focus on the communication via substrate vibrations, as it is recently becoming clear that flies make use of substrate-borne vibrational signals43,44.

Materials and methods

Insect rearing

Our laboratory colony was established in autumn from field-infested olive fruit collected around Thessaloniki, in northern Greece. Colony flies were kept in wooden cages (30 × 30 × 30 cm) with three sides of metal net and the front side of glass. Insects used in the experiments were reared in the laboratory in olive fruit for no more than three generations. A liquid diet of yeast hydrolysate (MP Biomedicals®), white granulated sugar and tap water (1:4:5 ratio) was provided daily to the stock adult flies.

For all the experiments where virgin flies were used, adult female and male flies soon after their emergence were transferred and maintained separately in plexiglass cages (20 × 20 × 20 cm, 40 flies per cage). In the experiment concerning the male flies’ behavior in the presence of mated females, we used female flies from the stock colony, that were presumed mated, as they had been kept in mixed sex groups since their emergence.

General observations

Experiments were carried out in laboratory rooms at 25 ± 2 °C and 55 ± 10% relative humidity (RH) and LD 14:10. For the experiments concerning the male wing vibration behavior in relation to age, time of day and the presence of conspecifics, the flies were maintained either individually or in pairs in transparent plastic cups of 400 ml volume as described by Kouloussis et al57.

Test 1: Audio recording of male wing vibration/stridulation and flight sounds

The aim of this test was to obtain a high quality, low-noise recording of the sound produced by the male wing vibration/stridulation to be used for (a) spectral analysis and (b) playback bioassay in test 2, where a possible attraction and arrestment effect on olive fruit flies was investigated. Also, the flight sounds of both sexes were to be compared.

Recording sessions took place in the sound studio of the Laboratory of Electroacoustics, at the Faculty of Electrical Engineering of the Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, Greece. A wooden cage with the experimental flies was placed inside a sound booth with 28 dBA ambient noise level and very low reverberation time (down to 0,4 s). A super gun hyper-cardioid condenser microphone (Seinheiser M67 with K6 powering, frequency range 40–20,000 Hz ± 2.5 dB, Sensitivity 50 mV/Pa ± 2.5 dB) was used for the recording and a precision sound level meter was used for calibration. Both outputs were connected to Digital Audio Workstation with Pro Tools HD environment. Two independent recording tracks were used at 24 bit/48 kHz sampling rate.

A wooden cage (30 × 30 × 30 cm) with metal net sides, containing approximately 70 virgin male and female flies 2–3 days old was placed under the microphone at 10 cm distance from the top side. Both sexes were present in this cage to better imitate natural conditions of courtship during which the male wing vibration is observed. Flies usually rest and perform wing vibration on the topmost side of the cage, due to the proximity to ventilation and light. Recording was continuous for five days, until the male flies started to engage in wing vibration. In addition, we recorded flight sounds of virgin male and female flies maintained separately.

The parameters to be specified were the fundamental frequency and harmonics in the spectrum of the recorded sounds.

Test 2: Response of olive fruit flies to broadcasts of recorded wing vibration

The aim of this test was to investigate if the broadcasted sound recorded at test 1 had an attraction and arrestment effect on both sexes of the olive fruit fly.

We placed an omnidirectional loudspeaker (M5 Beoplay by Bang and Olufsen, 16.5 cm diameter × 18.5 cm height, Frequency range 37–22,000 Hz, Speaker configuration 1 × 5″ woofer 1 × 1.5″ midrange 3 × 3⁄4″ tweeters, https://www.bang-olufsen.com/en/gr/speakers/beoplay-m5) at one end of a cylindrical net cage placed horizontally (0.5 m diameter × 1.5 m length). At the opposite end of the cylindrical cage a mimic silent control was placed, and the experimental flies were released at the center of the cylinder. The experimental arena was placed parallel to a window in a laboratory room with natural daylight. Experiments took place between 15:00 and 17:00 h in the months of December and January (sun-setting time at approximately 17:15). The loudspeaker broadcasted the recorded sound continuously for 1 h. We chose a high sound intensity level (96–98 dB at 10 cm from the source), because according to Mankin et al15, behavioral effects of sound are observed at intensities higher than 93 dB. We released 20 virgin female olive fruit flies (12–16 days old) from an opening at the center of the cylindrical arena and after 1 h we noted the position of the flies in the arena. The same bioassay took place for virgin males of the same age. Each day 2 repetitions were made and we made 20 repetitions for each sex. A total of 400 female and 400 male flies were used.

The experimental arena was divided into three virtual compartments: near the sound source, the middle area and opposite the sound source which serves as control. The number of flies that remained after 1 h of the bioassay in the compartment near the sound source and the opposite compartment were compared with a two-tailed t-test (unpaired data).

Test 3: Wing sexual dimorphism

The aim of this test was to study if there are morphological differences in the wings of male and female flies, that could account for possible differences in flight sounds of the sexes.

The right wings of 50 male and 50 female olive fruit flies were removed with a razor blade and photographed with a camera (Jenoptic Gryphax Naos) connected to a stereoscope (Leica M28). Using the camera software (GRYPHAX version 2.1.0.724), we measured the following parameters: the length, width, total area of the wings and the Cu1 cell curvature index (see Supplementary Fig. S2 in Supporting Information). Wing length and width were estimated using landmarks according to Benelli et al7.

Test 4: Observations of male wing vibration/stridulation behavior and mating percentage in relation to age and time of day

This was a preliminary test aiming to determine from which age and time of the day the male flies begin the wing vibration/stridulation behavior and to associate it with their sexual maturity and courtship rituals which are also related to age and time of the day.

We maintained two virgin male flies in each of 50 individual cages for their first 12 days of age and we observed and scored wing vibration behavior. To observe the behavior of each male fly, they were painted with a different color (non-toxic watercolor was used) on the thorax. Observations were carried out every ten minutes during a 2-h period in the morning, afternoon, and evening at 10:00–12:00, 14:00–16:00 and 18:00–20:00. The daylight period was from 07:00 until 21:00. We measured the proportion of individual male flies that were observed to wing vibrate to the total number of male flies observed at each day of observation.

For the experiments concerning the determination of mating percentages in relation to the age of the flies, ten virgin males and ten virgin females of the same age were maintained in each of 10 plexiglass cages (15 × 15 × 15 cm) and observed from 15:00 until 21:00 for mating. There were ten repetitions for each day of bioassay (from 1st until 12th day of age) and new sets of virgin flies were used every day. When a successful mating occurred, the pair was removed from the cage and the time of the beginning of mating was scored. We determined the proportion of mated couples to the total number of pairs that were maintained in the cages at the beginning of the bioassay.

Test 5: Male wing vibration parameters and behaviors in different social situations

The aim of this group of experiments was to observe the behavior of virgin males in the presence of another virgin male, virgin female and mated female, focusing on the wing vibration/stridulation frequencies and associated behaviors. We recorded the male wing vibration and observed the behavior of individuals in the presence of a conspecific.

For the recording of the male wing vibration in different social situations, male flies were transferred 1 day after emergence into a laboratory room (temperature 25 ± 2 °C and 55 ± 10% relative humidity (RH) and LD 14:10), where no females were present and kept in plexiglass cages (20 × 20 × 20 cm). On the 8th day of age, they were transferred and maintained inside plastic cups of 400 ml volume (as described in General observations) either individually or in male–male pairs (50 cups for each treatment). In another laboratory room, with the same temperature and photoperiod conditions, virgin male-virgin female pairs and virgin male-mated female pairs were maintained in each plastic cup (50 pairs for each treatment). All virgin flies were 8 days old at the beginning of the experiment whereas mated females were 11–12 days old and had been kept since their emergence in a stock colony cage.

In all treatments, observations were carried out during the last 4 h of the photophase and sounds produced by males vibrating their wings were recorded with the help of a directional condenser microphone (VideoMicMe, Røde, Australia, frequency range 100 Hz–20 kHz, sensitivity 33.0 dB re 1 Volt/Pascal (22.00 mV @ 94 dB SPL) + /− 2 dB @ 1 kHz, https://www.rode.com/microphones/videomicme) connected to a smartphone. When a mating occurred in virgin male-virgin female pairs, individuals were replaced with new ones of the same age and treatment and observations continued the following day. During the observation period, we obtained n = 10 useful recordings of males in solitary conditions, n = 10 in male-mated female pairs, n = 16 in male-virgin female pairs and n = 13 in male–male pairs. Recordings had to be discarded when they were of short duration and of low signal-to-noise ratio.

For the behavioral bioassay, we observed the flies every 10 min during the last 2 h of the photophase. We observed and scored each individual for the following behaviors: (1) wing vibration/stridulation, (2) fighting, i.e. boxing and pouncing, (3) attempted copulation and (4) wing waving according to Benelli32. Fighting engaged both individuals, so the parameter measured was the number of pairs observed in a fight. Behaviors of wing vibration, wing waving and attempts of copulation were scored for each individual fly.

In the presence of a female (virgin or mated) the male flies were observed every 10 min for wing vibration and the time spent signaling during the 2-h observation period was scored with a stopwatch. We made the same observations for male flies maintained in the cages without the presence of a conspecific. We observed 50 pairs for each treatment for two consecutive days. Different flies were used each day.

Calculation of sound temporal and spectral parameters

Recordings were analyzed using the software Praat-doing phonetics by computer v.6.1.3958 for defining the pulse duration (PD), inter-pulse interval (IPI) and fundamental frequency (FF). Adobe Audition 3.0 was used for frequency analysis graphs and spectrographs. Fast Fourier transformations (FFT) were calculated on 2048-point time-slices of the waveforms using a Hamming window. In accordance with Joyce et al59 the second, middle and second to last pulses were used for measuring PD, IPI and FF and then averaged. Spectral analysis diagrams were computed in Adobe Audition version 3.0 and the figures were made with GIMP 2.10.22.

Data analysis

Normality and homogeneity were tested with the Shapiro–Wilk test and Levene’s test, respectively. In wing morphology and flight sounds data, the number of samples was n > 30 and comparisons were made with the t-test (two-tailed, unpaired data). The acoustic parameters of male wing vibration in the presence of conspecifics were compared with one-way Anova and Tukey post-hoc test when assumptions for normality and homogeneity were met. When they were violated, the non-parametric Kruskal Wallis test was used with the Mann–Whitney post-hoc test for pairwise comparisons.

Proportions were compared with Pearson’s chi-square test. For all tests, the level of significance was α = 0.05. All statistical tests were performed with JMP 14.1.060.

Consent for publication

All authors read and approved the final manuscript.