Abstract
Canine cognitive dysfunction (CCD) is a neurodegenerative disease in aging dogs. It has been described previously in relatively small cohorts of dogs using multiple different rating scales. This study aimed to use a minimally modified CCD rating scale developed by previous researchers to describe the prevalence of CCD more thoroughly in a large, nationwide cohort of companion dogs participating in the Dog Aging Project (DAP) (nā=ā15,019). Associations between various canine characteristics, predicted lifespan quartiles, and CCD were examined using univariable and multivariable logistic regression models and receiver operating curve (ROC) analysis. When controlling for all other characteristics, the odds of CCD increased 52% with each additional year of age. Among dogs of the same age, health status, breed type, and sterilization status, odds of CCD were 6.47 times higher in dogs who were not active compared to those who were very active. When controlling for age, breed type, activity level, and other comorbidities, dogs with a history of neurological, eye, or ear disorders had higher odds of CCD. Lifespan quartile analysis showed excellent discriminating ability between CCD positive and negative dogs. Weight-based lifespan quartile estimation could therefore serve as a tool to inform CCD screening by veterinarians.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
Neurodegenerative diseases associated with aging have become increasingly prevalent among the aging American population. In 2020, approximately 5.8 million Americans were living with Alzheimerās disease (AD). It is the sixth leading cause of death in the country1. Despite this, the complex pathological pathways that lead to the development of AD are still not fully understood and can be difficult to study in vivo in early phases of disease progression. While transgenic animal models have been extensively used to study the pathophysiology of AD, limitations have been identified that have prompted investigation into non-transgenic animal models2.
The clinical and histological presentation of human AD and Canine Cognitive Dysfunction (CCD) in dogs have many similarities. As with humans, canine cognitive function declines throughout the course of the dogās lifespan3. Clinical signs of this decline appear to be related to learning and memory deficits, loss of spatial awareness, altered social interactions, and disrupted sleeping patterns4,5,6. Further, the presentation of human AD and CCD share certain neuropathological features such as amyloid-Ī² plaque deposition2,7,8,9,10.
The observed parallels between CCD and human AD suggest that dogs exhibiting CCD may offer researchers a valuable animal model in which to study characteristics of neurodegenerative diseases that are relevant to, but challenging to study in, human populations11. Further, dogs with CCD could serve as candidates for AD preventative and/or therapeutic strategies2. Finally, CCD is also a major health concern of dog owners and veterinarians. An increased understanding of CCD may help to advance treatment of cognitive disease in dogs. These potential benefits highlight the importance of accurate diagnosis of CCD in companion dogs.
A wide range of structured interviews and theory-driven scales for evaluating CCD have been developed within the last 20Ā years4,12,13,14. Studies of these scales have shown a correspondingly wide range of prevalence estimates that nonetheless appear to indicate an increased prevalence of cognitive impairment as a dog ages7,14,15. In order to gain a better understanding of CCD in aging dogs, researchers recently have developed assessment tools that are able to distinguish cognitively impaired aged dogs from those who are experiencing healthy aging11,14.
These scales provide veterinarians and researchers with tools to better identify the prevalence of CCD in aging dogs but have only been used to describe cognitive function in relatively small populations (<ā1000 dogs)11,14. The purpose of this study was to describe the range of cognitive function scores and prevalence of CCD in a very large, nationwide sample of companion dogs participating in the Dog Aging Project (DAP) (final nā=ā15,019). The DAP utilized the Canine Social and Learned Behavior (CSLB) survey, a minimally modified version of the validated canine cognitive dysfunction rating scale (CCDR) developed by Salvin et al.4. Further, we examined associations between weight-based lifespan quartile and CCD. Classifying a dogās lifespan into quartiles and quantifying the predictive ability of the CCDR in each quartile potentially allows for preventative healthcare measures to be taken by owners and veterinarians. The results could eventually lead to more timely detection and treatment of CCD16.
Methods
Study setting
Data were obtained from the DAP, a nationwide longitudinal study on aging initiated in 2018. One goal of the DAP is to better define aging in dogs based on comorbidities, frailty, and inflammaging17. Companion dog owners interested in enrolling their dogs complete multiple surveys throughout their involvement in the DAP. For the purposes of this report, these included the baseline Health and Life Experience Survey (HLES) and the CSLB survey completed during the first-year enrollment in DAP. The HLES is an extensive online survey that is distributed to all dog owners upon baseline enrollment, with sections on dog and owner household demographic characteristics, dog physical activity, and health status. The CSLB survey is a 13-item questionnaire based on Salvin et al.ās CCDR scale that aims to assess CCD4. It is described in further detail below.
Study design
This study analyzed baseline associations between selected characteristics collected through the HLES and dog cognitive characteristics reported in the CSLB. HLES data were provided by participants immediately upon enrollment, and CSLB data were provided by participants when that survey was administered. Dates of collection for HLES data used in the study ranged from December 23, 2019, to December 11, 2020. Dates of collection for CSLB data used in the study ranged from October 30, 2020, to December 31, 2020. The median time between completion of the two surveys was 5Ā weeks (range: 0ā50.3Ā weeks).
Study subjects
All dogs whose owners did not complete both the baseline HLES and CSLB surveys prior to December 31, 2020 were excluded from study. Further, all dogs whose owners indicated that they were not able to confidently report their dogās age, as assessed in the HLES, were also excluded. We began with a study sample of 27,542 dogs whose owners had completed the baseline HLES; 20,096 of those dogsā owners had also completed the CSLB survey. Of those 20,096 dogs, 15,019 owners reported being certain of their dogās age. The final sample for this study consisted of 15,019 dogs.
Data sources
The validated CCDR instrument, used by Salvin et al.4 for the identification of CCD, was minimally modified and renamed the CSLB (Canine Social and Learned Behavior Survey) for this study. In contrast to the original CCDR: (1) the instrument name was modified so as not to suggest dementia or cognitive dysfunction (identified in the term CCD) to the owners of enrolled dogs; and (2) it was programmed as an on-line digital tool with American English spelling. The CSLB thus consisted of 13 questions that assessed behaviors such as getting stuck behind objects, pacing, and failing to recognize familiar people. Responses for each question were scored based on frequency of the behavior (1ā=ānever, 2ā=āonce a month, 3ā=āonce a week, 4ā=āonce a day, 5ā=āmore than once a day). In addition, some questions asked owners to compare the severity of their dogās current behavior to the severity of the behavior 6Ā months prior. Numeric scores were summed from each ownerās responses. Certain questions had multipliers based on their clinical severity, as identified in CCD diagnosed dogs4. A minimum score of 16 and a maximum score of 80 were possible, with higher values indicating more severe levels of cognitive dysfunction. Age, sex, sterilization status, breed group, geographic region, physical activity level, comorbidities, and primary formal dog activity were assessed through the HLES.
For owner-reported purebred dogs, breed was elicited and assignedĀ to a breed group based on the eight groups defined by the American Kennel Club: herding, hound, toy, non-sporting, sporting, terrier, working, and miscellaneous/Foundation Stock Service18. Primary formal dog activity refers to a dogās main job or activity with the owner and was characterized as one of the following: companion animal or pet, obedience, show, breeding, agility, hunting, working, field trials, search and rescue, service dog, or assistance/therapy dog. Physical activity over the past year was classified as not active, moderately active, or very active. Major comorbidities considered were owner-reported in the HLES, including any history of cancer, endocrine disorders, kidney disorders, immune disorders, trauma, toxin consumption, infectious disease, hematologic disorders, neurological disorders, orthopedic disorders, reproductive disorders, liver disorders, gastrointestinal disorders, respiratory disorders, cardiac disorders, skin disorders, oral disorders, eye disorders, or ear disorders. Geographic region was determined based on the ownerās primary state of residence and categorized as West, Southwest, Midwest, Southeast, or Northeast. It is worth noting that the HLES collected hundreds of data points on participantsā behavior, lifestyle, diet and overall health. Although these variables were not considered to be within the scope of this study, future analyses intend to explore these associations further.
Data analyses
The primary outcome of interest was total CSLB score. Additionally, canine cognitive function was treated as a binary measure such that dogs with a CSLB scoreāā„ā50 were classified as having CCD4. Logistic regression models were fit to assess univariable associations between CCD and age, sex, sterilization status, breed group, geographic region, major comorbidities, and activity variables. Covariates that were determined a priori to be associated with CCD, informed by a directed acyclic graph (DAG) (Fig.Ā 2), were included in a multivariable logistic regression model.
Predicted lifespan quartile was then estimated for each subject. Projected mean life expectancies were calculated on a separate data set collected by Urfer et al. consisting of private U.S. veterinary hospital medical records for companion dogs19. Mean life expectancies were then assigned to each dog in the DAP data set as a function of their weight or projected weight class, sterilization status, and sex16,19. Each dog was assigned a predicted lifespan quartile that was equivalent to the proportion of their current life lived to their projected lifespan. Lifespan quartile was then evaluated for its ability to predict CCD using adjusted and unadjusted logistic regression models. The fitted models were used to construct a receiver operating curve (ROC) for two possible diagnostic thresholds (CSLB scoreāā„ā50, representative of previously defined thresholds, andā>ā37, which represented the highest quartile of scores in the data). Finally, area under the curve (AUC) was calculated in order to summarize predictive capacity of the fitted models. All statistical analyses were conducted in RStudio (Version 1.3)20. All methods and experimental protocols were carried out in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations. The University of Washington Institutional Review Board (IRB) reviewed the human subjects involvement for this study and determined that the limited dog owner demographic and lifestyle and anecdotal health information collected in the HLES is human subjects research that meets the qualifications for Exempt Status (Category 2). Given the Exempt status, the IRB did not require informed consent for completion of the surveys, but we nevertheless designed an informed consent form and process that meets the regulatory requirements laid down in U.S. Code of Federal Regulations, Title 45 Department of Health and Human Services Part 46, Protection of Human Subjects.
Results
Of the 15,019 dogs included in analyses, 19.5% were classified as being in their 4th quartile of life, 24.4% were classified as being in their 3rd quartile of life, 27.0% were in their 2nd quartile of life, and 29.1% of dogs in their 1st quartile of life. A total of 1.4% of dogs were classified as having CCD using the binary cut-off ofāā„ā50. There were similar distributions of sex, geographic region, primary role, and breed type among each lifespan quartile (Table 1). Sterilization status, history of each of 17 categories of health problems reported in HLES, purebred breed group, and activity level were substantially positively associated with weight-based projected lifespan quartile. Dogs in a higher weight-based projected lifespan quartile were more likely to be sterilized, more likely to have history of the above comorbidities, and were less active.
Associations between various canine characteristics and binary CCD assignment were assessed using univariable and multivariable logistic regression models (Table 2). When assessing age alone, the odds of being diagnosed with CCD increased almost 70% with each additional year of age (ORā=ā1.68, 95% CI 1.60, 1.77) (Fig.Ā 1).
A directed acyclic graph (DAG) was constructed to highlight relevant covariates to include in the multivariable logistic regression model assessing the association between age and CCD (Fig.Ā 2). Age, sterilization status, history of 15 categories of health problems, breed type, and activity level were selected for the final logistic regression model (Table 2). When controlling for all other characteristics, the odds of CCD increased 52% with each additional year of age (ORā=ā1.52, 95% CI 1.44, 1.61). Interestingly, among dogs of a given age, health status, breed type, and activity level, those who were intact had a higher odds of CCD, although this finding was not statistically significant (ORā=ā1.21, 95% CI 0.51ā2.51). Although attenuated, an inverse association between activity level and CCD was still found. Among dogs of the same age, health status, breed type, and sterilization status, odds of CCD were 6.47 times higher in dogs who were not active compared to those who were very active (ORā=ā6.47, 95% CI 2.93ā17.23). When controlling for age, breed type, activity level, and other comorbidities, dogs with a history of neurological, eye, or ear disorders had higher odds of CCD (ORā=ā1.84, 95% CI 1.26, 2.65; ORā=ā2.16, 95% CI 1.57, 2.98; ORā=ā1.96, 95% 1.42, 2.70, respectively).
Adjusted and unadjusted logistic regression models were then fit using the weight-based projected lifespan quartiles. Adjusted models included all covariates from the multivariable logistic regression model assessing the association between age and CCD (Table 2). These weight-based projected lifespan quartile models were evaluated for their ability to accurately classify CCD at two different diagnostic thresholds:āā„ā50, as cited in previous literature, andā>ā37, which captures the upper 4th quartile of CSLB scores. In total, four predictive models were assessed by evaluating the area under the curve (AUC) of an ROC curve (Fig.Ā 3). The diagnostic threshold of CSLB scoreāā„ā50 had the highest predictive capacity. The adjusted model that controlled for health status, breed type, sterilization status, and activity level provided a slightly improved predictive capacity over the unadjusted model (AUCā=ā0.892, 95% CI 0.854ā0.930 vs. AUCā=ā0.862, 95% CI 0.831ā0.893). The threshold of CSLB scoreā>ā37 had lower predictive capacity for both the adjusted and unadjusted models (AUCā=ā0.701, 95% CI 0.682ā0.721 vs. AUCā=ā0.644, 95% CI 0.624ā0.665).
Discussion
Results from this study indicate a positive association between age and CCD in companion dogs, even adjusting for other characteristics in a multivariable logistic regression model. There also existed associations between CCD and decreased activity level, as well as CCD and history of an eye, ear, or neurological disorder. The association between age and CCD aligns with the progressive nature of CCD and with previous dog research findings, which have shown an exponential increase in CCD prevalence with increasing age4.
An inverse association was seen in dogs whose owners indicated higher dog activity levels over the past year. Previous studies with rodent models have demonstrated that exercise can have protective effects against the development of biological markers and subsequent behavioral deficits characteristic of AD, and numerous observational human studies have consistently shown inverse associations between exercise and AD21,22,23,24,25. These observations may reflect a variety of biologic mechanisms, including a reduction of pro-inflammatory cytokines in the brain that otherwise contribute to neural damage and death, and an increase in neural plasticity24,25. The reduced odds of CCD among more active dogs in our cohort may be a result of these same mechanisms, but it is important to note that this correlation could also exist simply because of dogs exhibiting less activity due to their cognitive decline. Additionally, the presence of a third, unmeasured variable (such as owner interaction) could be amplifying this association.
The association we observed between dogs who have ever had a neurological disorder and their prevalence of CCD is expected. Dementia, a neurological disorder that could have been indicated by an owner in their HLES survey, is characterized by a decline in cognitive function. Seizure disorders, which also could have been indicated by the owner, have been found to occur more commonly in human AD patients than in comparable individuals without AD26.
Human studies have demonstrated potential links between eye and ear disorders and AD. Impairments in visual acuity and visual fields have been observed more frequently in AD patients than in similarly aged individuals without AD27,28. Further, the presence of ophthalmic conditions, such as cataracts and age-related macular degeneration, has been found to be associated with an increased risk of all-cause dementia and AD29. Amyloid deposits, which have been linked to the development of AD and have been associated with the acceleration of AD progression, have been found in the lens of the eye in some individuals with age-related cataracts, potentially indicating common neuropathological pathways leading to AD and cataracts30. Age-related macular degeneration has also been associated with an increased risk of all-cause dementia and AD30. Like the potential relationship between cataracts and AD, amyloid deposits have been detected in the small fatty protein deposits that accumulate under the eye of individuals with age-related macular degeneration31.
In addition to numerous studies identifying hearing loss as a possible risk factor for cognitive decline and all-cause dementia, a recent study found that individuals with dual sensory impairment (combined visual and hearing impairments) had a substantially increased risk of AD, possibly as a result of reduced neural resources needed for cognitive performance31,32,33,34. Further analysis of those with combined impairments may be used to investigate the possibility of this increased risk in our cohort.
Alternatively, the associations we observed between sensory impairment and CCD could be due at least in part to misclassification. The CSLB survey, which is closely derived from Salvin et al.ās CCDR, is intended to distinguish between behaviors associated with CCD as opposed to those that are part of normal dog aging4. However, some behaviors assessed by the rating scale may not arise solely from cognitive impairment4. Questions such as, āHow often does your dog walk into walls or doors?ā and āHow often does your dog have difficulty finding food dropped on the floor?ā were included in the CCDR because increased frequency of these behaviors is seen in dogs with CCD. However, sensory impairments could also cause increased frequency of these behaviors, resulting in high scores on the instrument.
We observed a dogās estimated lifespan quartile, which is a function of their age, weight, sex, and sterilization status, to have excellent discriminating ability between CCD positive and negative dogs (CSLB scoreāā„ā50). This improved only slightly in the model adjusted for age, sterilization status, history of 15 categories of health problems, breed type, and activity level. We also considered a lower diagnostic threshold for CCD assignment by using the top quartile of CSLB scores as a cut-off. However, that classification yielded much lower discriminating ability between CCD positive and negative dogs for lifespan quartile. Although the diagnostic threshold of 50 had high predictive capacity, it is important to note that this threshold between normal aging and CCD was chosen by previous researchers, and measures of sensitivity and specificity would be difficult to obtain4. It would therefore be beneficial to examine the validity of this diagnostic tool more closely in future studies. The CCDR scale (on which the CSLB was based) was chosen for this study due, in part, to its high reported diagnostic accuracy (98.9%) and its high re-test reliability4. However, it only identified CCD in 1.4% of our total canine population (mean ageā=ā6.9, SDā=ā4.2). Although this aligns well with previously reported prevalence of veterinary-diagnosed dementia, higher CCD prevalence has been reported in older dog cohorts (8ā+āyears) using the CCDR 14. This could be due to several reasons, including reliance on self-reported owner data as opposed to veterinary assessment, as well as wider age ranges in our study. Further sensitivity analyses of specific CSLB questions may be appropriate in future studies.
This study had several strengths, including a large sample size, standardized data collection methods, high participant response proportions within the cohort, and the inclusion of many potential confounders in analytic models. Important limitations did exist in this study that are worth addressing. In addition to potential concerns with the sensitivity and specificity of the diagnostic threshold chosen for CCD, and that this analysis considers only the first year of data and is not yet longitudinal, it is not possible to assess any prospective links between the various canine characteristics and CCD. While the HLES and CSLB surveys were administered to owners at different times (the maximum time interval between the completion of the HLES and CSLB surveys was 50.3Ā weeks), the median time between survey completion was only 5Ā weeks. 11,324 of the total 15,019 owners completed both surveys within 10Ā weeks. There was a low likelihood that this timeframe would result in any sort of meaningful changes in CCD status. Future studies using prospective DAP data will be able to explore potential risk factors for CCD as well as cognitive decline over time.
Another limitation is that all data used in this analysis were based on surveys completed by owners. Owner-reported information is potentially susceptible to multiple forms of bias. Questions requiring owners to recall behaviors and medical conditions that could have occurred from 6Ā months to many years prior could introduce either differential or non-differential misclassification, depending on whether the errors in recall were correlated with the dogās cognitive status. Non-differential misclassification could arise from social desirability if, for example, owners indicated a higher level of physical activity for their dog, or a lower level of impairment on the CSLB survey. Such errors would tend to dampen associations between physical activity and CCD. Additionally, the reliance on owner-reported behavioral survey information in this study did not allow for any further exploration into the pathological presentations of CCD. Future studies on this cohort intend to explore these relationships further.
It is also important to recognize the drastic changes that have taken place in many households due to the COVID-19 pandemic, and the possibility that these changes influenced our results. Depending on when an owner completed their HLES and CSLB surveys, their dogās activity level may have changed as a result of stay-at-home orders and/or the ownerās ability to work from home. Additionally, owners spending more time at home with their dog may have an impact on the dogās health, and it may increase the likelihood of observing specific health behaviors that would affect a dogās reported health status. These types of major lifestyle changes could have altered our data in ways that would be difficult to quantify. Finally, there is the potential for unmeasured confounders that were not captured by our surveys, as well as survey data that were not considered for analysis, which could result in residual confounding potentially adding bias to our results.
We identified a positive association between companion dog age and CCD. We also identified strong positive associations between history of a neurological, ear, or eye disorder as well as an association between decreased physical activity level and having a CSLB score of 50 or higher. Dogs were classified into their predicted quartile of life based on their age, weight, sex, and sterilization status. Using a binary diagnostic threshold ofāā„ā50, a dogās lifespan quartile showed excellent discriminating ability between CCD positive and negative dogs. This quartile estimation could potentially serve as a useful tool to inform whether a dog should be screened for CCD by their veterinarian. Finally, given increasing evidence of the parallels between canine and human cognitive disease, accurate CCD diagnosis in dogs may provide researchers with more suitable animal models in which to study aging in human populations. Additional studies that further explore factors that will provide a better understanding of canine cognitive function are needed.
Data availability
The datasets generated during and/or analyzed during the current study are available through Terra in the Dog Aging Project 2020 Curated Workspace.
Change history
30 May 2023
A Correction to this paper has been published: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-023-34875-5
References
Alzheimerās Association. Alzheimerās Association 2020 Facts and Figures Report. Alzheimerās Assoc. (2020). https://www.alz.org/alzheimers-dementia/facts-figures;https://www.alz.org/media/Documents/alzheimers-facts-and-figures_1.pdf. Accessed February 21, 2021.
SchĆ¼tt, T. et al. Dogs with cognitive dysfunction as a spontaneous model for early Alzheimerās disease: A translational study of neuropathological and inflammatory markers. J. Alzheimerās Dis. 52(2), 433ā449. https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-151085 (2016).
Adams, B., Chan, A., Callahan, H. & Milgram, N. W. The canine as a model of human cognitive aging: Recent developments. Prog. Neuro-Psychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 24(5), 675ā692. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0278-5846(00)00101-9 (2000).
Salvin, H. E., McGreevy, P. D., Sachdev, P. S. & Valenzuela, M. J. The canine cognitive dysfunction rating scale (CCDR): A data-driven and ecologically relevant assessment tool. Vet. J. 188(3), 331ā336. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tvjl.2010.05.014 (2011).
Ruehl, W. W. & Hart, B. L. Canine cognitive dysfunction. In Psychopharmacology of Animal Behavior Disorders (eds Dodman, N. & Shuster, L.) 283ā304 (Blackwell Scientific Publications, 1998).
Ruehl, W. W. et al. Canine cognitive dysfunction as a model for human age-related cognitive decline, dementia and Alzheimerās disease: Clinical presentation, cognitive testing, pathology and response to l-deprenyl therapy. Prog. Brain Res. 106, 217ā225 (1995).
Azkona, G. et al. Prevalence and risk factors of behavioural changes associated with age-related cognitive impairment in geriatric dogs: PAPER. J. Small Anim. Pract. 50(2), 87ā91. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1748-5827.2008.00718.x (2009).
Cummings, B. et al. Ī²-Amyloid accumulation in aged canine brain. A model of early plaque formation in Alzheimerās disease. Neurobiol. Aging 14, 547ā560 (1993).
Cummings, B. J. et al. Diffuse plaques contain C-terminal AĪ²42 and not AĪ²40: Evidence from cats and dogs. Neurobiol. Aging 17, 653ā659 (1996).
Cummings, B. J. et al. The canine as an animal model of human aging and dementia. Neurobiol. Aging 17, 259ā268 (1996).
Madari, A. et al. Assessment of severity and progression of canine cognitive dysfunction syndrome using the CAnine DEmentia Scale (CADES). Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 171, 138ā145. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2015.08.034 (2015).
Colle, M.-A. et al. Vascular and parenchymal A-beta deposition in the aging dog: Correlation with behavior. Neurobiol. Aging 21(5), 695ā704. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0197-4580(00)00113-5 (2000).
Pugliese, M. et al. Severe cognitive impairment correlates with higher cerebrospinal fluid levels of lactate and pyruvate in a canine model of senile dementia. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 29(4), 603ā610. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pnpbp.2005.01.017 (2005).
Salvin, H. E., McGreevy, P. D., Sachdev, P. S. & Valenzuela, M. J. Under diagnosis of canine cognitive dysfunction: A cross-sectional survey of older companion dogs. Vet. J. 184(3), 277ā281. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tvjl.2009.11.007 (2010).
Neilson, J. C., Hart, B. L., Cliff, K. D. & Ruehl, W. W. Prevalence of behavioral changes associated with age-related cognitive impairment in dogs. J. Am. Vet. Med. Assoc. 218(11), 1787ā1791. https://doi.org/10.2460/javma.2001.218.1787 (2001).
Creevy, K. E. et al. 2019 AAHA canine life stage guidelines. J. Am. Anim. Hosp. Assoc. 55(6), 267ā290. https://doi.org/10.5326/JAAHA-MS-6999 (2019).
Alexander, J. E., Colyer, A., Haydock, R. M., Hayek, M. G. & Park, J. Understanding how dogs age: Longitudinal analysis of markers of inflammation, immune function, and oxidative stress. J. Gerontol. Ser. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 73(6), 720ā728. https://doi.org/10.1093/gerona/glx182 (2018).
The American Kennel Club. List of Breeds by Group. https://www.akc.org/public-education/resources/general-tips-information/dog-breeds-sorted-groups/ (2020). Accessed November 12, 2020.
Urfer, S. R., Kaeberlein, M., Promislow, D. E. L. & Creevy, K. E. Lifespan of companion dogs seen in three independent primary care veterinary clinics in the United States. Canine Med. Genet. 7(1), 1ā14. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40575-020-00086-8 (2020).
R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, 2020). https://www.R-project.org/
Lindsay, J. et al. Risk factors for Alzheimerās disease: A prospective analysis from the Canadian study of health and aging. Am. J. Epidemiol. 156(5), 445ā453. https://doi.org/10.1093/aje/kwf074 (2002).
Richards, M., Hardy, R. & Wadsworth, M. E. Does active leisure protect cognition? Evidence from a national birth cohort. Soc. Sci. Med. 56(4), 785ā792. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0277-9536(02)00075-8 (2003).
Larson, E. B. et al. Exercise is associated with reduced risk for incident dementia among persons 65Ā years of age and older. Ann. Intern. Med. 144(2), 73ā81. https://doi.org/10.7326/0003-4819-144-2-200601170-00004 (2006).
Nascimento, C. M. et al. Physical exercise in MCI elderly promotes reduction of pro-inflammatory cytokines and improvements on cognition and BDNF peripheral levels. Curr. Alzheimer Res. 11(8), 799ā805. https://doi.org/10.2174/156720501108140910122849 (2014).
Duzel, E., van Praag, H. & Sendtner, M. Can physical exercise in old age improve memory and hippocampal function?. Brain 139(3), 662ā673. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awv407 (2016).
Amatniek, J. C. et al. Incidence and predictors of seizures in patients with Alzheimerās disease. Epilepsia 47(5), 867ā872. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1528-1167.2006.00554.x (2006).
Sadun, A. A., Borchert, M., DeVita, E., Hinton, D. R. & Bassi, C. J. Assessment of visual impairment in patients with Alzheimerās disease. Am. J. Ophthalmol. 104(2), 113ā120. https://doi.org/10.1016/0002-9394(87)90001-8 (1987).
Lakshminarayanan, V., Lagrave, J., Kean, M. L., Dick, M. & Shankle, R. Vision in dementia: Contrast effects. Neurol. Res. 18(1), 9ā15. https://doi.org/10.1080/01616412.1996.11740369 (1996).
Hwang, P. H. et al. Ophthalmic conditions associated with dementia risk: The Cardiovascular Health Study. Alzheimerās Dement. https://doi.org/10.1002/alz.12313 (2021).
Wang, S., Mims, P. N., Roman, R. J. & Fan, F. Is Beta-Amyloid accumulation a cause or consequence of Alzheimerās disease?. J. Alzheimerās Parkinsonism Dement. 1(2), 007 (2016).
Loughrey, D. G., Kelly, M. E., Kelley, G. A., Brennan, S. & Lawlor, B. A. Association of age-related hearing loss with cognitive function, cognitive impairment, and dementia: A systematic review and meta-analysis [published correction appears in JAMA Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. 2018 Feb 1;144(2):176]. JAMA Otolaryngol. Head Neck Surg. 144(2), 115ā126. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamaoto.2017.2513 (2018).
Lin, F. R. et al. Hearing loss and incident dementia. Arch. Neurol. 68(2), 214ā220. https://doi.org/10.1001/archneurol.2010.362 (2011).
Johnson, J. C. S. et al. Hearing and dementia: From ears to brain. Brain 144(2), 391ā401. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awaa429 (2021).
Hwang, P. H. et al. Dual sensory impairment in older adults and risk of dementia from the GEM study. Alzheimerās Dement. (Amst) 12(1), e12054. https://doi.org/10.1002/dad2.12054 (2020).
Acknowledgments
The Dog Aging Project thanks study participants, their dogs, and community veterinarians for their important contributions.Ā
Funding
The Dog Aging Project is supported by U19 grant AG057377 from the National Institute on Aging, a part of the National Institutes of Health, and by private donations.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Consortia
Contributions
S.Y. performed statistical analysis and contributed to the drafting and editing of the manuscript. A.F., S.M.S., K.C., and A.R. provided statistical advice and contributed to the drafting and editing of the manuscript. V.W., M.K., and D.P. provided scientific advice and contributed to the drafting and editing of the manuscript. The authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
The original online version of this Article was revised: The original version of this Article contained errors in the Dog Aging Project Consortium list. Full information regarding the corrections made can be found in the correction for this Article.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Yarborough, S., Fitzpatrick, A., Schwartz, S.M. et al. Evaluation of cognitive function in the Dog Aging Project: associations with baseline canine characteristics. Sci Rep 12, 13316 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-15837-9
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-15837-9
This article is cited by
-
Ocular biomarkers of cognitive decline based on deep-learning retinal vessel segmentation
BMC Geriatrics (2024)
-
A randomized, controlled clinical trial demonstrates improved owner-assessed cognitive function in senior dogs receiving a senolytic and NAD+ precursor combination
Scientific Reports (2024)
-
A preliminary analysis of the effect of individual differences on cognitive performance in young companion dogs
Animal Cognition (2024)
-
Comparative neurogenetics of dog behavior complements efforts towards human neuropsychiatric genetics
Human Genetics (2023)
-
Associations between physical activity and cognitive dysfunction in older companion dogs: results from the Dog Aging Project
GeroScience (2023)