Abstract
This meta-analysis sought to expand upon neurobiological models of mindfulness through investigation of inherent brain network connectivity outcomes, indexed via resting state functional connectivity (rsFC). We conducted a systematic review and meta-analysis of rsFC as an outcome of mindfulness training (MT) relative to control, with the hypothesis that MT would increase cross-network connectivity between nodes of the Default Mode Network (DMN), Salience Network (SN), and Frontoparietal Control Network (FPCN) as a mechanism of internally-oriented attentional control. Texts were identified from the databases: MEDLINE/PubMed, ERIC, PSYCINFO, ProQuest, Scopus, and Web of Sciences; and were screened for inclusion based on experimental/quasi-experimental trial design and use of mindfulness-based training interventions. RsFC effects were extracted from twelve studies (mindfulness n = 226; control n = 204). Voxel-based meta-analysis revealed significantly greater rsFC (MT > control) between the left middle cingulate (Hedge’s g = .234, p = 0.0288, I2 = 15.87), located within the SN, and the posterior cingulate cortex, a focal hub of the DMN. Egger’s test for publication bias was nonsignificant, bias = 2.17, p = 0.162. In support of our hypothesis, results suggest that MT targets internetwork (SN-DMN) connectivity implicated in the flexible control of internally-oriented attention.
Introduction
Human waking life contains many moments in which the mind is not engaged by external goals or tasks, but is instead absorbed in a state of stimulus-independent thought (SIT)1,2, commonly referred to as mind wandering (MW). The definition of MW is multidimensional, and has previously been described as thought that is task-unrelated3, spontaneously unfolding4, or perceptually decoupled5 (i.e., “internally-oriented” or self-reflective in nature6,7). The content and dynamics of MW have been shown to vary considerably between individuals8,9, with important implications for mental health. While MW appears to serve multiple adaptive functions, including self-regulation10, memory consolidation11 (but see Refs.12,13), and problem solving14,15, the resting mind can paradoxically become restless in nature when thoughts are negatively-oriented, repetitive, or intrusive5,16,17. Such perseverative cognitions reinforce maladaptive coping strategies endemic to affective disorders (e.g., anxiety and depressive conditions)18,19,20,21.
The disruption of repetitive negative thinking is notoriously challenging21,22; however, research suggests that cognitive training in mindful awareness may alter the resting mind so as to function more adaptively23,24. The effects of mindfulness training for the treatment of affective disorders and concomitant rumination are well-documented25,26,27,28; however, the mechanistic involvement of resting state activity is less well-understood. Prevailing theory posits that mindfulness training may alter such activity via attentional mechanisms, ostensibly reflected in the reorganization of neural circuitry24,29,30. Accordingly, recent research has begun to investigate resting state neural circuitry as an outcome of mindfulness training (e.g., Refs.31,32) and as a characteristic of dispositional mindfulness (e.g., Refs.33,34,35,36). Such studies support the involvement of candidate interacting brain networks—including the default mode network (DMN), salience network (SN), and the frontoparietal control network (FPCN)—through which mindfulness may facilitate the flexible allocation of attentional resources between introspective and perceptual processes37. However, there remains little consensus about how mindfulness alters functional connectivity within and between these networks. The current meta-analysis sought to update brain-based models of mindfulness by comprehensively examining mindfulness-driven resting state functional connectivity (rsFC) outcomes.
The neurocognitive features of mind wandering (MW)
When measured as functional connectivity, changes in neural circuitry reflect strengthened or weakened coordination between regions and, at a larger scale, between networks of interest38,39. Analogous to the spontaneous flow of thought characteristic of resting states, the brain’s activity at rest self-organizes into temporally-coherent neural networks detected by blood oxygen level dependent (BOLD) signal40,41. Such networks are commonly termed ‘resting-state’ or ‘intrinsic’ functional networks42. Although there is little consistency in the taxonomy of intrinsic functional networks (for review see Ref.43), it is generally accepted that a minimum of 10 networks are observable during the resting state44.
Among recognized large-scale networks, the DMN, SN, and FPCN have been frequently implicated in explanatory frameworks of MW. The DMN has received the most attention historically for its role in internally-directed mentation17,45. Indeed, prevailing evidence suggests that the primary function of the DMN may be the generation and maintenance of internally-oriented mental processes46,47. The DMN may be further parceled into three subnetworks, the midline-located core DMN, the medial temporal lobe subsystem (DMN-MTL), and the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex subsystem (DMN-dmPFC)48. While each subnetwork supports different functions of internally oriented mentation, the core DMN—encompassing the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) and ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC)—is most robustly associated with self-referential thought48,49,50,51. Hyperconnectivity within the core DMN has been reliably observed in psychopathologies characterized by self-focused perseverative cognition (i.e., depression)52,53,54; however, the role of the DMN in maladaptive MW is likely more complex. In support of this viewpoint, neural models of MW suggest that the regulation (or dysregulation) of internally-oriented mental states relies on inter-network coordination between the DMN and networks implicated in attention, namely the SN and FPCN37,55,56.
According to the dynamic framework of mind-wandering4, internal experiences—maintained by the DMN—may be deliberately or automatically constrained via coordination with the FPCN and SN, respectively. The FPCN, characterized by dorsolateral PFC and anterior inferior parietal structures43, is well-known for its integral role in cognitive control processes57. Studies combining experience sampling with neuroimaging suggest that the FPCN flexibly couples with the DMN to deliberately regulate internal mentation58, and that such coordination is used to inhibit internal thought when external attention is required59,60.
In contrast, the SN has been theorized to support automatic constraints on internal experiences through cognitively efficient cross-network signaling4. Key nodes of the SN include the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC)61 and anterior insula43, which jointly operate within a cortico-striato-thalamo-cortical loop to detect motivationally important stimuli, either internal or external62. Through its extensive connectivity with the DMN and FPCN, the SN facilitates “set-shifting” by automatically and flexibly directing attention to and from internal and external cues55,63. Interestingly, SN engagement has also been suggested as a putative mechanism of perseverative cognition64. According to this viewpoint, aberrant SN connectivity exacerbates interoceptive awareness of unpleasant sensations, and due to the high motivational value of ruminative thoughts, draws attentional resources to internally-directed mentation64. While this theory is indirectly supported by evidence of SN hyperconnectivity in high-rumination individuals65,66, it fails to explain why therapies aiming to enhance awareness of internal experiences (i.e., mindfulness) would ameliorate ruminative thought. Thus further research is warranted to elucidate how cognitive training, like mindfulness, alters the dynamics of internal experiences at both a subjective and neurobiological level.
Multi-method investigations suggest that variability in functional connectivity within and between networks likely reflects the content and dynamics of internal mentation6,67,68, and resting state functional connectivity (rsFC) has been associated with individual differences in symptoms of anxiety69, depression52,70, and trait rumination71,72. Thus reorganization of resting state neurocircuitry, as detected by rsFC protocols, has been suggested as a key mechanism of treatment for psychopathologies featuring repetitive negative thought73,74,75,76,77. Although the research is nascent, there is initial evidence that mindfulness training may improve regulation of mental states, and that rsFC indices may be used to better understand the mechanisms through which mindfulness extends its therapeutic effects (e.g., Ref.78).
Mechanisms of mindfulness: theory and empirical support
Mindfulness, commonly defined as the act of attending to present-moment thoughts, emotions, and sensations without judgment or appraisal79, is relatively unique as a treatment of maladaptive MW. Unlike popular cognitive behavioral therapies (CBTs), which enable the individual to challenge dysfunctional thoughts80, mindfulness instead targets one’s relationship to such thoughts81,82. This non-judgmental stance towards internal experiences may be promoted through a combination of neurocognitive mechanisms (for review see Refs.24,29,30,83,84,85,86). Mounting evidence indicates that mindfulness practice enhances attentional control, which may potentially support the deliberate constraint of maladaptive MW64. Foundational to the practice of mindfulness is the development of attentional control through a meditative technique called Focused Attention (FA). FA meditation trains the practitioner to focus on and maintain attention to a neutral sensory object (e.g., the breath), and direct attention back to that object when the mind begins to wander87. This recursive process of shifting and sustaining attention has previously been linked to enhanced functional connectivity within the FPCN of experienced meditators23, suggesting that FA improves top-down cognitive control needed to disengage from distracting thoughts and emotions.
Alternative models of mindfulness posit that mindfulness may indirectly regulate MW by promoting awareness of internal experiences85. According to this framework, the sustained concentration conferred by mindfulness facilitates awareness (or mindful meta-awareness), defined as the capacity to observe one’s mental patterns with a sense of equanimity and psychological distance87,88. This internal awareness thereby supports recognition of thoughts and feelings as discrete mental states, and in turn, improves flexible, adaptive responding87. The cultivation of mindful awareness has been theoretically attributed to enhanced functional cohesion of networks linked to self-awareness (e.g., default mode network) and attention monitoring (e.g., salience networks)23,30. However, support for this neural model draws largely from cross-sectional research (e.g., Refs.23,89,90), as well as correlational evidence33,34,35,36. Moreover, this neural model of mindful awareness does not fully account for the role of non-judgment, or acceptance, which may operate in tandem with awareness to reduce “experiential fusion” with one’s thoughts91,92. It has been speculated that mindfulness may dampen experiential fusion through DMN downregulation85,92; however, it is unclear how neural substrates of attention and awareness may mediate such effects.
Building on previous models of mindfulness meditation24,29,30,85, it is plausible that mindfulness training (MT) alters the resting state via reorganization of neural circuitry (i.e., intrinsic functional connectivity). Specifically, we posit that focused attention training recruits the FPCN, implicated in cognitive control and executive function. Given its documented role as a ‘hub’ of functional connectivity58, the FPCN may plausibly facilitate functional connections between other resting state networks—particularly between networks related to mind-wandering (i.e., default mode network) and internal awareness (i.e., salience network)—thus enabling the flexible regulation of mental states85. Coupled with improvements in executive functioning, such functional coordination between DMN and SN may likewise support meta-awareness skills needed to identify and disengage from maladaptive cognitive patterns. To extend this theoretical framework, the present study posed the question: Does mindfulness training alter functional connectivity between and within DMN, salience, and FPCN during rest?
Present study
The aim of this study was to determine if mindfulness training modifies intrinsic functional connectivity (IFC) observed during resting states. Specifically, this study sought to examine connectivity within and between the frontoparietal control network (FPCN), the default mode network (DMN), and the salience network (SN). To date, several studies have investigated the impact of mindfulness training on resting state functional connectivity (rsFC) using controlled, experimental designs78,89,93,94. Although insightful, such studies typically suffer from low statistical power, a limitation endemic to research relying on high-cost neuroimaging modalities such as fMRI. Addressing such concerns, meta-analytic approaches may be used to pool information from well-controlled studies while modeling convergence of effects across pooled samples. Thus, we conducted a systematic review and meta-analysis of rsFC outcomes of mindfulness training relative to structurally-equivalent programs (i.e., active controls). To test the neuroplastic changes associated with mindfulness skills—namely, executive functioning and meta-awareness—we hypothesized that (1) mindfulness training would enhance rsFC between the FPCN and DMN as an indicator of enhanced cognitive control; (2) mindfulness training would enhance rsFC between the DMN and SN, reflective of meta-awareness; and (3) mindfulness training would alter rsFC within such networks.
Results
Study characteristics and participant demographics are displayed in Table 1. Overall, studies used standardized mindfulness-based interventions ranging from 3 days to 8 weeks in length. All studies except for one featured designs with a structurally equivalent control intervention. Study samples varied in terms of age (M = 45.80; SD = 13.15) and clinical characteristics with the majority of studies recruiting healthy adults (see Table 1). The majority of individuals from the pooled sample identified as female (n = 286, 62.72%) followed by male (n = 170, 37.28%). No studies reported data from trans or non-binary participants. Of the 12 included studies, only 5 reported racial/ethnic demographic information. From this subsample of 152 participants, 100 (65.79%) identified as white, 31 (20.39%) as Black/African American, 12 (7.89%) as Mixed Race/Other, 5 as Hispanic/Latino (3.29%), 3 (1.97%) as Asian, and 1 as Southeast Asian (0.66%).
Seed regions were pooled according to standardized resting state network location. This process demonstrated that eligible studies used seed regions from four networks, the default mode network (DMN; n = 11), the midcingulo-insular network (M-CIN; n = 7), the dorsal attention network (DAN; n = 1), and the frontoparietal control network (FPCN; n = 2) (see Supplementary Table S1).
SDM meta-analysis was used to test for significant training condition effects. Meta-analysis results identified one significant cluster of 57 voxels loc ated in the paracingulate gyri (Table 2, Fig. 1), Hedge’s g = 0.234, uncorrected p = 0.0288, I2 = 15.87, suggesting that mindfulness training, relative to control training programs, increased connectivity to bilateral paracingulate gyri and the left anterior cingulate (non-significant cluster outcomes reported in Supplementary Table S2). Yeo’s cortical parcellation atlas147 places the peak coordinates of this cluster (0, 20, 34) in the ventral attention network (VAN), more recently taxonomized as the mid-cingulo insular network (within the anatomical domain) or salience network (within the cognitive domain)47. Egger’s test for publication bias was nonsignificant, bias = 2.17, p = 0.162, and funnel plots did not suggest the influence of small study effects (Fig. 2).
Examining the original articles revealed increased functional connectivity to the median cingulate via the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), a region canonically situated within the default mode network (DMN). Notably, studies within the sample exclusively reported increased connectivity between median cingulate effect regions and PCC seed regions. Therefore, the findings suggest that mindfulness training increased resting state functional connectivity between the default mode network and salience network.
Discussion
This meta-analysis is the first to systematically examine the effects of mindfulness-based training on resting state functional connectivity (rsFC), a neural marker of cognitive and emotion regulation. A systematic review of the literature revealed that rsFC has been sparsely investigated as a target of mindfulness training, with 12 studies meeting the eligibility criterion in the current review. Meta-analysis results partially supported our hypotheses, indicating that relative to mindfulness naive participants and participants trained in one or another structurally equivalent program, mindfulness trainees increased functional connectivity to the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC), and that such connectivity was seeded to the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC). Results did not support our first hypothesis, which predicted enhanced cross-network connectivity between the FPCN and DMN as a mechanism of cognitive control. In support of our second hypothesis, these results suggest that mindfulness training strengthened cross-network connectivity between regions associated with the default mode network (DMN) and salience network (SN). Implications for supported and null findings are explored in the following paragraphs.
Along with the frontoparietal control network (FPCN), the DMN and SN operate synergistically to support self-regulation, and aberrant coordination within and between these networks has been associated with emotional dysfunction, deficits in attentional control, and maladaptive mind wandering (MW) (e.g., rumination, worry, intrusive thought)78,95. It has been suggested that therapeutic outcomes of mindfulness are mediated by reorganization within and between such networks24,29,30,85; however, the precise neural targets of mindfulness are ill-defined, in no small part due to methodological differences between studies (e.g., in research designs, training protocols, and target populations)96. Among explanations of mindfulness’ neurocognitive mechanisms, the literature favors two competing theories, colloquially referred to as ‘top-down’ and ‘bottom-up’ models of mindful regulation. According to the top-down model, mindfulness recruits FPCN engagement to support the deliberate regulation of unwanted thoughts and emotions30,97, and—through dynamic cross-network coupling—inhibits task-incongruent DMN activity58. In contrast, bottom-up models suggest that mindfulness can operate automatically to modify thoughts and emotions without higher-level cognitive control98. Specifically, such bottom-up regulation may diminish the propensity for rumination by reducing within-network DMN connectivity99,100 or disrupting SN-DMN coordination associated with negative rumination64. We suggest that the effect of mindfulness training on dACC-PCC connectivity may partially align with models of both top-down and bottom-up regulation.
Contrary to our prediction, this meta-analysis did not reveal involvement of FPCN cross-network connectivity, a finding that is inconsistent with prior neural characterizations of long-term meditators101 and runs counter to theoretical explanations of the FPCN as a facilitator of mindful top-down regulation30,97. On the one hand, this null effect may derive from inadequate statistical power, given that only 2 of the included studies investigated FPCN-seeded rsFC. On the other hand, such findings do not necessarily rule out mechanisms of top-down cognitive control. It has previously been suggested that executive control processes may operate via dissociable networks with distinct temporal profiles64,102. According to this framework, the FPCN provides rapid, flexible feedback to lower-level systems, while the cingulo-opercular network (analogous to the salience network43) functions to maintain cognitive control over longer epochs102. Included within the cingulo-opercular network is the dACC, the functional connectivity target region identified from our analysis. Otherwise referred to as the midcingulate cortex61, the dACC is distinguished by specialty Von Economo neurons103,104. Von Economo neurons—or spindle neurons—are characterized by long-distance signal transmission and heterogenous dendritic/spinal structures105,106, two features which appear to support cross-network information integration103,104,107,108. Such extensive connectivity supports global monitoring of internal and external experiences109, and by extension, evaluates the degree of cognitive effort needed for a given situation110. In the early stages of mindfulness practice, when attentional focus is described as “effortful”30, dACC-PCC coupling may reflect the deliberate regulation of sympathetic arousal to maintain a state of alert focus. Such an explanation is plausible given that the studys’ participants included in this analysis were exposed to relatively brief training durations, with the majority ranging between 3 days to 8 weeks. It is possible that continued practice may induce neuroplastic changes indicative of “effortless” attention regulation as exhibited by long-term or expert practitioners109.
Alternatively, increased PCC-dACC functional connectivity may support indirect (i.e., bottom-up) regulation through the promotion of meta-awareness. Embedded within the DMN, the PCC has classically been associated with the maintenance of internally-oriented mentation17. However, the PCC may also play a critical role in attention regulation111 and the general maintenance of vigilance112,113. Evidence suggests that dorsal portions of the PCC may be involved in balancing internal and external attentional focus56 as facilitated through extensive cross-network connectivity111,114. In this vein, PCC connectivity with the dACC—a salience network hub—may serve to integrate information regarding the content, quality, and direction of attentional focus (i.e., meta-awareness). Thus, our finding of dACC-PCC connectivity may potentially underpin flexible cognitive faculties needed to observe internal states with open awareness.
Interpretation of the results reported here is limited by several factors. To date very few studies have examined rsFC outcomes of mindfulness, with even fewer implementing randomized controlled or quasi-experimental procedures. For this reason the meta-analysis pulls from a relatively small sample of 12 studies (MT n = 226; CT n = 224). Although such selectivity maintains the benefits of data quality and homogeneity of experimental conditions (i.e., mindfulness interventions), small samples also threaten generalizability as effects may be driven by only a few studies with shared experimental parameters115. The extracted studies draw from heterogeneous populations—in terms of demographic characteristics, gender, and age—features which have shown to vary in both gray matter and connectome characteristics116,117,118. Additionally, no study reported non-binary gender information and reports of racial/ethnic identity were omitted from 7 of the 12 studies included in our sample. The practice of demographic underreporting is a substantial challenge in the field of mindfulness research (and psychological research more broadly), with important implications for health inequities119. Future researchers should prioritize responsible reporting and recruitment practices to address such disparities119,120.
The duration and therapeutic focus of mindfulness interventions likewise warrant consideration. Although the majority of mindfulness interventions were standardized, these studies report a wide range of intervention durations (3 days to 16 weeks) with different degrees of intensity (e.g., retreat verses remote-delivered trainings) and therapeutic focus (e.g., PTSD, parent-focused stress reduction). There is currently little research examining the dose–response relation for mindfulness-based treatments; however, a recent meta-analysis of this topic suggests that brief trainings may be equally as effective as longer interventions for the treatment of stress, depression, and anxiety121. Nevertheless, the relation between mindfulness training dose and neuroplasticity remains poorly understood, a matter that is further complicated when applied to clinical and aging populations with atypical connectomes121,122,123.
Interpretation is likewise limited by a priori seed selection, which was requisite for all included studies (see protocol from Ref.52). This meta-analysis failed to detect significant FPCN cross-network connectivity, and while this null effect may stem from erroneous theoretical assumptions, it may instead be the consequence of preferencing seeds within the DMN. Behavioral neuroimaging studies have previously shown strong functional coupling between the FPCN and DMN across multiple tasks (e.g., Refs.59,124,125, and one study recently demonstrated how robust co-activation of FPCN and DMN regions may obscure detection of group effects126. Further research is needed to definitely determine if and how mindfulness training interacts with FPCN neurocircuitry during task-related and resting brain states.
Finally, it warrants noting that there is currently no standard classification system for large-scale functional networks. The lack of universal nomenclature presents a significant barrier to interpreting neural outcomes, especially given the multitude of naming schemes43 and inconsistent application of common labels (see meta-analysis of executive control network topography127). We use the functional network terms “frontoparietal control network”, “default mode network”, and “salience network” primarily due to their ubiquity in cognitive neuroscience43 and the mindfulness literature. Nevertheless, such naming conventions based on functional properties are problematic for the reasons described above. We recommend that future research move towards more transparent network taxonomies incorporating anatomical properties (see Ref.43).
This meta-analysis highlights the potential value of rsFC as a window into understanding the mechanisms of mindfulness. However, research on this topic is in its early stages, and considerably more research is necessary to qualify specific rsFC effects as mechanistic targets of mindfulness training. Without additional research, the effects of mindfulness on resting state cognition remains speculative. Researchers may consider novel sampling methods including lab-based phenomenological reporting (e.g., Ref.128) and ecological momentary assessment, a method used to capture day-to-day lived experiences129,130. Investigators should additionally consider examining multiple representations of connectivity—including effective connectivity, white matter connectivity, and dynamic connectivity—which probe different features of brain network organization (e.g., directional effects; temporal dynamics, etc.). The comparison of such representations has the potential to reduce ambiguity and improve interpretation of connectivity-based effects131.
Further research is also needed to determine the relevance of rsFC plasticity for different psychiatric conditions. The successful use of network neuroscience for clinical diagnosis and treatment is a lofty goal, which will require overcoming considerable methodological challenges (see Refs.132,133). Such research necessitates reliability and reproducibility; however, preprocessing and analysis procedures may vary significantly among studies based on individual research questions. Nevertheless, researchers can promote standardization of analysis techniques through commitment to open science practices in which scripted pipelines and nonthreshold brain images are publicly catalogued (for recommendations, see Ref.131).
Conclusion
Resting state neural indices have potential to elucidate the rich subtleties of internal experiences, with important implications for those suffering from rigid or negative inner dialogues. According to Buddhist perspectives, mindfulness offers a window into exploring the qualities of conscious experience and—through heightened awareness of such mental states—enables their adaptive transformation84. However, it remains ambiguous how mindfulness practice can be optimized to reduce suffering in heterogeneous populations, both healthy and clinical119,134,135. The current meta-analysis aimed to elucidate the nature of mindfulness training effects by focusing on neural indices of the resting mind. Results indicated strengthened dACC-PCC connectivity as a product of mindfulness. Although interpretation of this effect requires more experimental research, results may nevertheless advance the science of mindfulness and its impact on the resting brain.
Methods
Literature search
A comprehensive literature search was conducted using the keywords rest*(-ing), connect*(-ivity), default mode, mindfulness, meditation, MBSR [Mindfulness-based stress reduction], and MBCT [Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy] to search for matching literature from the following databases: MEDLINE/PubMed, ERIC, PSYCINFO, ProQuest, Scopus, and Web of Sciences (see Supplementary Table S4). Texts were screened by four reviewers and considered for inclusion if they reported resting state functional connectivity outcomes derived from functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) modalities. Further, all included studies were randomized controlled trials or used quasi-experimental (e.g., matched control) designs in which mindfulness-based training programs (as operationally defined by Ref.136) were compared to a control condition.
Studies were excluded if they met one or more of the following criteria: (1) experimental interventions predominantly featured training elements other than mindfulness meditation (e.g., yoga, transcendental meditation, loving-kindness or compassion meditation, tai chi; integrative body-mind training); (2) resting state functional connectivity (rsFC) group contrasts were not reported; (3) seed-based rsFC methods were not used or seed-based rsFC indices were not reported.
The systematic literature review identified 7041 records, from which 6093 were excluded upon abstract review (see PRISMA flow diagram in Fig. 3). Full-text examination of the remaining 21 studies for eligibility yielded a sample of 13 eligible publications from 12 unique studies32,78,89,94,137,138,139,140,141,142,143,144,145. The final sample of studies reported data from 226 participants assigned to mindfulness training and 204 participants assigned to an active control program.
Data extraction and coding
The meta-analysis was coordinate-based (for example see Ref.52), in which extracted coordinates reflected locations of significant group differences in resting state functional connectivity pre-post meditative training. Given that all studies used seed-based rsFC analyses, coordinates were categorized as belonging to either seed anatomy or effect anatomy. Using this coding scheme, 11 sets of seed coordinates were extracted, with coordinates reflecting each seed anatomy’s reported center of mass. In the instance that seed anatomy coordinates were not reported because the seed region was defined from a standardized atlas or subject-specific spatial map, the center of mass was estimated using meta-analytic maxima reported from the open source platform, neurosynth.org146. All studies reported peak coordinates of significant between-group effects, yielding a total of 65 effect coordinates. After extracting seed and effect coordinates, all coordinates were categorized into rsFC networks as defined from a standardized network cortical parcellation atlas147. Effects were likewise characterized by direction of effect with positive effects reflecting relatively stronger functional connectivity in the mindfulness group (MT > CT), and negative effects indicating relatively stronger functional connectivity in the control group (CT > MT). In addition to main effects, demographic data and intervention characteristics were extracted for review. Effects were extracted by four independent reviewers who collected data from independent reports.
Interrater reliability
We conducted a two-way random-effects ICC modeled with absolute agreement. Specifically, we tested for significant effects of rater ID on each quantitative measure. ICC estimates were within acceptable range (ICC > 0.6; p < 0.005), indicating a high degree of rater agreement. Next, unweighted Cohen’s kappa was calculated to examine reliability of extracted categorical variables. Results indicated substantial agreement148,149 between the raters’ judgements, k = 0.741 (95% CI 0.182 to 0.884). Finally, z statistics were converted to two-sample t-test statistics, assuming equal variances in both conditions. If records did not report z statistics or two-sample t-test statistics, p values were used to estimate t-test statistics used in the meta-analysis.
Voxel-based meta-analysis
While standard meta-analytic procedures require 3D statistical parametric maps, such images are often inaccessible in published fMRI reports. This limitation has prompted the development of coordinate-based meta-analytic (CBMA) approaches, which only require peak coordinates of significant clusters rather than 3D statistical images150. Nevertheless, CBMA procedures assume that voxels are independent and that the likelihood of false positives is equivalent among voxels151. Such assumptions may be overcome through voxel-based meta-analytic procedures, namely seed-based d mapping (SDM) with permutation of subject images (PSI)151. Unlike other (CBMA) approaches—namely, Activation Likelihood Estimation (ALE)152 and Multilevel Kernel Density Analysis (MKDA)153—which test for the presence or absence of peaks of statistical significance (i.e., null hypothesis testing), seed-based d mapping (SDM) uses multiple imputation to model effect sizes for each study before conducting a random-effects meta-analysis. By imputing effect sizes on 3D statistical maps, SDM has the advantage of both parametric mapping and coordinate-based approaches while controlling for familywise error rate (FWER)154 via threshold-free cluster enhancement (TFCE)155.
Thus, the current meta-analysis was conducted using SDM (SDM-PSI version 6.21) with permutation of subject images (PSI) CBMA algorithm156. SDM preprocessing was first used to convert t-values of peak coordinates into Hedge’s g effect sizes. Study-level images of upper and lower bounds of probable effects were then constructed for all voxels using multiple imputation156,157, in which anisotropic Gaussian kernels are convolved with reported effect sizes158. Using SDM we then calculated most likely effect size and standard error via multiple imputations of Maximum Likelihood Estimation (MLE) with a jackknife procedure. Finally, FWE corrections were performed via subject-based permutation testing and TFCE-corrected effect sizes were calculated to estimate group differences. Main analytic findings were scrutinized for small study effects and excess significance (i.e., publication bias) through Egger’s tests and examination of funnel plots. Finally, given that atypical functional connectivity is a transdiagnostic feature of pathological populations118, we conducted a meta-regression using random-effects general linear modeling to explore the potential confounding effect of clinical population status. Null effects are reported in Supplementary Table S3.
Data availability
The datasets generated during and/or analysed during the current study are available in the neurovault repository, https://identifiers.org/neurovault.image:768613. Review protocol is not registered and can only be accessed upon request.
References
Christoff, K., Ream, J. M. & Gabrieli, J. D. Neural basis of spontaneous thought processes. Cortex 40(4–5), 623–630 (2004).
Mason, M. F. et al. Wandering minds: The default network and stimulus-independent thought. Science 315(5810), 393–395 (2007).
Stawarczyk, D., Majerus, S., Maj, M., Van der Linden, M. & D’Argembeau, A. Mind-wandering: Phenomenology and function as assessed with a novel experience sampling method. Acta Physiol. (Oxf) 136(3), 370–381 (2011).
Christoff, K., Irving, Z. C., Fox, K. C., Spreng, R. N. & Andrews-Hanna, J. R. Mind-wandering as spontaneous thought: A dynamic framework. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 17(11), 718–731 (2016).
Smallwood, J. & Schooler, J. W. The restless mind. Psychol. Bull. 132(6), 946 (2006).
Diaz, B. A. et al. The Amsterdam Resting-State Questionnaire reveals multiple phenotypes of resting-state cognition. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7, 446 (2013).
Fox, K. C., Spreng, R. N., Ellamil, M., Andrews-Hanna, J. R. & Christoff, K. The wandering brain: Meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies of mind-wandering and related spontaneous thought processes. Neuroimage 111, 611–621 (2015).
Andrews-Hanna, J. R. et al. A penny for your thoughts: Dimensions of self-generated thought content and relationships with individual differences in emotional wellbeing. Front. Psychol. 4, 900 (2013).
Smallwood, J. & Schooler, J. W. The science of mind wandering: Empirically navigating the stream of consciousness. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 66, 487–518 (2015).
Morawetz, C. et al. Intrinsic functional connectivity underlying successful emotion regulation of angry faces. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 11(12), 1980–1991 (2016).
Wamsley, E. J. & Summer, T. Spontaneous entry into an “offline” state during wakefulness: A mechanism of memory consolidation?. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 32(9), 1714–1734 (2020).
Humiston, G. B., Tucker, M. A., Summer, T. & Wamsley, E. J. Resting states and memory consolidation: A preregistered replication and meta-analysis. Sci. Rep. 9(1), 1–9 (2019).
Blondé, P., Girardeau, J. C., Sperduti, M., & Piolino, P. A wandering mind is a forgetful mind: A systematic review on the influence of mind wandering on episodic memory encoding. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 132, 774–792 (2021).
Hearne, L. J., Cocchi, L., Zalesky, A. & Mattingley, J. B. Reconfiguration of brain network architectures between resting-state and complexity-dependent cognitive reasoning. J. Neurosci. 37(35), 8399–8411 (2017).
Ito, T. et al. Cognitive task information is transferred between brain regions via resting-state network topology. Nat. Commun. 8(1), 1–14 (2017).
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Wisco, B. E. & Lyubomirsky, S. Rethinking rumination. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 3(5), 400–424 (2008).
Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Smallwood, J. & Spreng, R. N. The default network and self-generated thought: component processes, dynamic control, and clinical relevance. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1316(1), 29 (2014).
Watkins, E. R. Constructive and unconstructive repetitive thought. Psychol. Bull. 134(2), 163 (2008).
Kaplan, D. M. et al. Maladaptive repetitive thought as a transdiagnostic phenomenon and treatment target: An integrative review. J. Clin. Psychol. 74(7), 1126–1136 (2018).
Spinhoven, P., van Hemert, A. M. & Penninx, B. W. Repetitive negative thinking as a mediator in prospective cross-disorder associations between anxiety and depression disorders and their symptoms. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 63, 6–11 (2019).
Watkins, E. R. & Roberts, H. Reflecting on rumination: Consequences, causes, mechanisms and treatment of rumination. Behav. Res. Ther. 127, 103573 (2020).
Mennin, D. S. & Fresco, D. M. What, me worry and ruminate about DSM-5 and RDoC? The importance of targeting negative self-referential processing. Clin. Psychol. Sci. Pract. 20(3), 258–267 (2013).
Hasenkamp, W. & Barsalou, L. W. Effects of meditation experience on functional connectivity of distributed brain networks. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6, 38 (2012).
Vago, D. R. & Zeidan, F. The brain on silent: Mind wandering, mindful awareness, and states of mental tranquility. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1373(1), 96 (2016).
Piet, J. & Hougaard, E. The effect of mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for prevention of relapse in recurrent major depressive disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 31(6), 1032–1040 (2011).
Vøllestad, J., Nielsen, M. B. & Nielsen, G. H. Mindfulness-and acceptance-based interventions for anxiety disorders: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 51(3), 239–260 (2012).
Gu, J., Strauss, C., Bond, R. & Cavanagh, K. How do mindfulness-based cognitive therapy and mindfulness-based stress reduction improve mental health and wellbeing? A systematic review and meta-analysis of mediation studies. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 37, 1–12 (2015).
Perestelo-Perez, L., Barraca, J., Penate, W., Rivero-Santana, A. & Alvarez-Perez, Y. Mindfulness-based interventions for the treatment of depressive rumination: Systematic review and meta-analysis. Int. J. Clin. Health Psychol. 17(3), 282–295 (2017).
Hölzel, B. K. et al. Mindfulness practice leads to increases in regional brain gray matter density. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 191(1), 36–43 (2011).
Tang, Y. Y., Hölzel, B. K. & Posner, M. I. The neuroscience of mindfulness meditation. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 16(4), 213–225 (2015).
Kilpatrick, L. A. et al. Impact of mindfulness-based stress reduction training on intrinsic brain connectivity. Neuroimage 56(1), 290–298 (2011).
Kral, T. R. et al. Mindfulness-based stress reduction-related changes in posterior cingulate resting brain connectivity. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 14(7), 777–787 (2019).
Doll, A., Hölzel, B. K., Boucard, C. C., Wohlschläger, A. M. & Sorg, C. Mindfulness is associated with intrinsic functional connectivity between default mode and salience networks. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9, 461 (2015).
Bilevicius, E., Kolesar, T. A., Smith, S. D., Trapnell, P. D. & Kornelsen, J. Trait emotional empathy and resting state functional connectivity in default mode, salience, and central executive networks. Brain Sci. 8(7), 128 (2018).
Harrison, R., Zeidan, F., Kitsaras, G., Ozcelik, D. & Salomons, T. V. Trait mindfulness is associated with lower pain reactivity and connectivity of the default mode network. J. Pain 20(6), 645–654 (2019).
Parkinson, T. D., Kornelsen, J. & Smith, S. D. Trait mindfulness and functional connectivity in cognitive and attentional resting state networks. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 13, 112 (2019).
Dixon, M. L. et al. Heterogeneity within the frontoparietal control network and its relationship to the default and dorsal attention networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 115(7), E1598–E1607 (2018).
Van Dijk, K. R. et al. Intrinsic functional connectivity as a tool for human connectomics: Theory, properties, and optimization. J. Neurophysiol. 103(1), 297–321 (2010).
Buckner, R. L., Krienen, F. M. & Yeo, B. T. Opportunities and limitations of intrinsic functional connectivity MRI. Nat. Neurosci. 16(7), 832–837 (2013).
Raichle, M. E. et al. A default mode of brain function. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 98(2), 676–682 (2001).
Fox, M. D. & Raichle, M. E. Spontaneous fluctuations in brain activity observed with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 8(9), 700–711 (2007).
Snyder, A. Z. & Raichle, M. E. A brief history of the resting state: The Washington University perspective. Neuroimage 62(2), 902–910 (2012).
Uddin, L. Q., Yeo, B. T. & Spreng, R. N. Towards a universal taxonomy of macro-scale functional human brain networks. Brain Topogr. 32(6), 926–942 (2019).
Damoiseaux, J. S. et al. Consistent resting-state networks across healthy subjects. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 103(37), 13848–13853 (2006).
Raichle, M. E. The brain’s default mode network. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 38, 433–447 (2015).
Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Reidler, J. S., Huang, C. & Buckner, R. L. Evidence for the default network’s role in spontaneous cognition. J. Neurophysiol. 104(1), 322–335 (2010).
Axelrod, V., Rees, G. & Bar, M. The default network and the combination of cognitive processes that mediate self-generated thought. Nat. Hum. Behav. 1(12), 896–910 (2017).
Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Reidler, J. S., Sepulcre, J., Poulin, R. & Buckner, R. L. Functional-anatomic fractionation of the brain’s default network. Neuron 65(4), 550–562 (2010).
Legrand, D. & Ruby, P. What is self-specific? Theoretical investigation and critical review of neuroimaging results. Psychol. Rev. 116(1), 252 (2009).
van Buuren, M., Gladwin, T. E., Zandbelt, B. B., Kahn, R. S. & Vink, M. Reduced functional coupling in the default-mode network during self-referential processing. Hum. Brain Mapp. 31(8), 1117–1127 (2010).
Denny, B. T., Kober, H., Wager, T. D. & Ochsner, K. N. A meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies of self-and other judgments reveals a spatial gradient for mentalizing in medial prefrontal cortex. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 24(8), 1742–1752 (2012).
Kaiser, R. H., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Wager, T. D. & Pizzagalli, D. A. Large-scale network dysfunction in major depressive disorder: A meta-analysis of resting-state functional connectivity. JAMA Psychiat. 72(6), 603–611 (2015).
Coutinho, J. F. et al. Default mode network dissociation in depressive and anxiety states. Brain Imaging Behav. 10(1), 147–157 (2016).
Lois, G. & Wessa, M. Differential association of default mode network connectivity and rumination in healthy individuals and remitted MDD patients. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 11(11), 1792–1801 (2016).
Sridharan, D., Levitin, D. J. & Menon, V. A critical role for the right fronto-insular cortex in switching between central-executive and default-mode networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 105(34), 12569–12574 (2008).
Leech, R. & Sharp, D. J. The role of the posterior cingulate cortex in cognition and disease. Brain 137(1), 12–32 (2014).
Zanto, T. P. & Gazzaley, A. Fronto-parietal network: Flexible hub of cognitive control. Trends Cogn. Sci. 17(12), 602–603 (2013).
Cole, M. W. et al. Multi-task connectivity reveals flexible hubs for adaptive task control. Nat. Neurosci. 16(9), 1348–1355 (2013).
Spreng, R. N., Stevens, W. D., Chamberlain, J. P., Gilmore, A. W. & Schacter, D. L. Default network activity, coupled with the frontoparietal control network, supports goal-directed cognition. Neuroimage 53(1), 303–317 (2010).
Gao, W. & Lin, W. Frontal parietal control network regulates the anti-correlated default and dorsal attention networks. Hum. Brain Mapp. 33(1), 192–202 (2012).
Allman, J. M., Hakeem, A., Erwin, J. M., Nimchinsky, E. & Hof, P. The anterior cingulate cortex: The evolution of an interface between emotion and cognition. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 935(1), 107–117 (2001).
Peters, S. K., Dunlop, K. & Downar, J. Cortico-striatal-thalamic loop circuits of the salience network: A central pathway in psychiatric disease and treatment. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 10, 104 (2016).
Seeley, W. W. et al. Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. J. Neurosci. 27(9), 2349–2356 (2007).
Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Christoff, K. & O’Connor, M.-F. Dynamic regulation of internal experience. In Neuroscience of Enduring Change: Implications for Psychotherapy. (eds. Lane, R.D., & Nadel, L.) (Oxford University Press, 2020).
Kühn, S., Vanderhasselt, M. A., De Raedt, R. & Gallinat, J. Why ruminators won’t stop: The structural and resting state correlates of rumination and its relation to depression. J. Affect. Disord. 141(2–3), 352–360 (2012).
Kühn, S., Vanderhasselt, M. A., De Raedt, R. & Gallinat, J. The neural basis of unwanted thoughts during resting state. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 9(9), 1320–1324 (2014).
Doucet, G. et al. Patterns of hemodynamic low-frequency oscillations in the brain are modulated by the nature of free thought during rest. Neuroimage 59(4), 3194–3200 (2012).
Marchetti, A. et al. Theory of mind and the whole brain functional connectivity: Behavioral and neural evidences with the Amsterdam Resting State Questionnaire. Front. Psychol. 6, 1855 (2015).
Liao, W. et al. Selective aberrant functional connectivity of resting state networks in social anxiety disorder. Neuroimage 52(4), 1549–1558 (2010).
Drysdale, A. T. et al. Resting-state connectivity biomarkers define neurophysiological subtypes of depression. Nat. Med. 23(1), 28–38 (2017).
Rosenbaum, D. et al. Aberrant functional connectivity in depression as an index of state and trait rumination. Sci. Rep. 7(1), 1–12 (2017).
Satyshur, M. D., Layden, E. A., Gowins, J. R., Buchanan, A. & Gollan, J. K. Functional connectivity of reflective and brooding rumination in depressed and healthy women. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 18(5), 884–901 (2018).
Broyd, S. J. et al. Default-mode brain dysfunction in mental disorders: A systematic review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 33(3), 279–296 (2009).
Dichter, G. S., Gibbs, D. & Smoski, M. J. A systematic review of relations between resting-state functional-MRI and treatment response in major depressive disorder. J. Affect. Disord. 172, 8–17 (2015).
Klumpp, H. & Fitzgerald, J. M. Neuroimaging predictors and mechanisms of treatment response in social anxiety disorder: An overview of the amygdala. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 20(10), 1–9 (2018).
Xu, J. et al. Anxious brain networks: A coordinate-based activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis of resting-state functional connectivity studies in anxiety. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 96, 21–30 (2019).
Feurer, C. et al. Resting state functional connectivity correlates of rumination and worry in internalizing psychopathologies. Depress. Anxiety 38(5), 488–497 (2021).
Creswell, J. D. et al. Alterations in resting-state functional connectivity link mindfulness meditation with reduced interleukin-6: A randomized controlled trial. Biol. Psychiatry. 80(1), 53–61 (2016).
Brown, K. W. & Ryan, R. M. The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 84(4), 822 (2003).
Beck, J. S. Cognitive Behavior Therapy: Basics and Beyond, 2nd ed. (The Guilford Press, 2012).
Kabat-Zinn, J. Wherever You Go, There You Are: Mindfulness Meditation in Everyday Life. (Hyperion, 1994).
Segal, Z. V., Williams, M., & Teasdale, J. Mindfulness-Based Cognitive Therapy for Depression, 2nd ed. (Guilford Publications, 2018).
Fox, K. C. et al. Is meditation associated with altered brain structure? A systematic review and meta-analysis of morphometric neuroimaging in meditation practitioners. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 43, 48–73 (2014).
Fox, K. C., & Cahn, B. R. Meditation and the brain in health and disease. In The Oxford Handbook of Meditation. (eds. Farias, M., Brazier, D. & Lalljee, M.) (Oxford University Press, 2018). https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780198808640.013.23.
Vago, D. R. & Silbersweig, D. A. Self-awareness, self-regulation, and self-transcendence (S-ART): A framework for understanding the neurobiological mechanisms of mindfulness. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6, 296 (2012).
Young, K. S. et al. The impact of mindfulness-based interventions on brain activity: A systematic review of functional magnetic resonance imaging studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 84, 424–433 (2018).
Lutz, A., Slagter, H. A., Dunne, J. D. & Davidson, R. J. Attention regulation and monitoring in meditation. Trends Cogn. Sci. 12(4), 163–169 (2008).
Dunne, J. D., Thompson, E. & Schooler, J. Mindful meta-awareness: Sustained and non-propositional. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 28, 307–311 (2019).
Brewer, J. A. et al. Meditation experience is associated with differences in default mode network activity and connectivity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 108(50), 20254–20259 (2011).
Farb, N. A., Segal, Z. V. & Anderson, A. K. Mindfulness meditation training alters cortical representations of interoceptive attention. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 8(1), 15–26 (2013).
Bernstein, A., Hadash, Y. & Fresco, D. M. Metacognitive processes model of decentering: Emerging methods and insights. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 28, 245–251 (2019).
King, A. P. & Fresco, D. M. A neurobehavioral account for decentering as the salve for the distressed mind. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 28, 285–293 (2019).
Froeliger, B. et al. Meditation-state functional connectivity (msFC): Strengthening of the dorsal attention network and beyond. Evid. Based Complement. Altern. Med. 2012, 1–9 (2012).
King, A. P. et al. Altered default mode network (DMN) resting state functional connectivity following a mindfulness-based exposure therapy for posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) in combat veterans of Afghanistan and Iraq. Depress. Anxiety 33(4), 289–299 (2016).
Menon, V. Mind the hype: A critical evaluation and prescriptive agenda for research on mindfulness and meditation. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15(10), 483–506 (2011).
Van Dam, N. T. et al. Mind the hype: A critical evaluation and prescriptive agenda for research on mindfulness and meditation. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 13(1), 36–61 (2018).
Chiesa, A., Serretti, A. & Jakobsen, J. C. Mindfulness: Top–down or bottom–up emotion regulation strategy?. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 33(1), 82–96 (2013).
Ochsner, K. N. et al. Bottom-up and top-down processes in emotion generation: Common and distinct neural mechanisms. Psychol. Sci. 20(11), 1322–1331 (2009).
Kucyi, A. et al. Enhanced medial prefrontal-default mode network functional connectivity in chronic pain and its association with pain rumination. J. Neurosci. 34(11), 3969–3975 (2014).
Hamilton, J. P., Farmer, M., Fogelman, P. & Gotlib, I. H. Depressive rumination, the default-mode network, and the dark matter of clinical neuroscience. Biol. Psychiat. 78(4), 224–230 (2015).
Kemmer, P. B., Guo, Y., Wang, Y. & Pagnoni, G. Network-based characterization of brain functional connectivity in Zen practitioners. Front. Psychol. 6, 603 (2015).
Dosenbach, N. U., Fair, D. A., Cohen, A. L., Schlaggar, B. L. & Petersen, S. E. A dual-networks architecture of top-down control. Trends Cogn. Sci. 12(3), 99–105 (2008).
Watson, K. K., Jones, T. K. & Allman, J. M. Dendritic architecture of the von Economo neurons. Neuroscience 141, 1107–1112. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.04.084 (2006).
Correa-Júnior, N. D., Renner, J., Fuentealba-Villarroel, F., Hilbig, A. & Rasia-Filho, A. A. Dendritic and spine heterogeneity of von Economo neurons in the human cingulate cortex. Front. Synaptic Neurosci. 12, 25 (2020).
Stevens, F. L., Hurley, R. A. & Taber, K. H. Anterior cingulate cortex: Unique role in cognition and emotion. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 23(2), 121–125 (2011).
Allman, J. M. et al. The von Economo neurons in frontoinsular and anterior cingulate cortex in great apes and humans. Brain Struct. Funct. 214(5), 495–517 (2010).
Posner, M. I., Rothbart, M. K., Sheese, B. E. & Tang, Y. The anterior cingulate gyrus and the mechanism of self-regulation. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 7(4), 391–395 (2007).
Sheth, S. A. et al. Human dorsal anterior cingulate cortex neurons mediate ongoing behavioural adaptation. Nature 488(7410), 218–221 (2012).
Tang, Y. Y., Tang, R., Posner, M. I. & Gross, J. J. Effortless training of attention and self-control: Mechanisms and applications. Trends Cogn. Sci. 26, 566–577 (2022).
Shenhav, A., Botvinick, M. M. & Cohen, J. D. The expected value of control: An integrative theory of anterior cingulate cortex function. Neuron 79(2), 217–240 (2013).
Leech, R., Kamourieh, S., Beckmann, C. F. & Sharp, D. J. Fractionating the default mode network: Distinct contributions of the ventral and dorsal posterior cingulate cortex to cognitive control. J. Neurosci. 31(9), 3217–3224 (2011).
Hahn, B., Ross, T. J. & Stein, E. A. Cingulate activation increases dynamically with response speed under stimulus unpredictability. Cereb. Cortex 17(7), 1664–1671 (2007).
Sämann, P. G. et al. Development of the brain’s default mode network from wakefulness to slow wave sleep. Cereb. Cortex 21(9), 2082–2093 (2011).
Margulies, D. S. et al. Precuneus shares intrinsic functional architecture in humans and monkeys. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 106(47), 20069–20074 (2009).
Müller, V. I. et al. Ten simple rules for neuroimaging meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 84, 151–161 (2018).
Campbell, K., Grigg, O., Saverino, C., Churchill, N. & Grady, C. Age differences in the intrinsic functional connectivity of default network subsystems. Front. Aging Neurosci. 5, 73 (2013).
Ingalhalikar, M. et al. Sex differences in the structural connectome of the human brain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 111(2), 823–828 (2014).
Sha, Z. et al. Meta-connectomic analysis reveals commonly disrupted functional architectures in network modules and connectors across brain disorders. Cereb. Cortex 28(12), 4179–4194 (2018).
Eichel, K. et al. A retrospective systematic review of diversity variables in mindfulness research, 2000–2016. Mindfulness 12, 1–20 (2021).
Sabik, N. J., Matsick, J. L., McCormick-Huhn, K. & Cole, E. R. Bringing an intersectional lens to “open” science: An analysis of representation in the reproducibility project. Psychol. Women Q. 45, 475–492 (2021).
Strohmaier, S. The relationship between doses of mindfulness-based programs and depression, anxiety, stress, and mindfulness: A dose-response meta-regression of randomized controlled trials. Mindfulness 11(6), 1315–1335 (2020).
Geerligs, L., Renken, R. J., Saliasi, E., Maurits, N. M. & Lorist, M. M. A brain-wide study of age-related changes in functional connectivity. Cereb. Cortex 25(7), 1987–1999 (2015).
Sha, Z., Wager, T. D., Mechelli, A. & He, Y. Common dysfunction of large-scale neurocognitive networks across psychiatric disorders. Biol. Psychiat. 85(5), 379–388 (2019).
Farb, N. A. et al. Attending to the present: Mindfulness meditation reveals distinct neural modes of self-reference. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2(4), 313–322 (2007).
Fleming, S. M. & Dolan, R. J. The neural basis of metacognitive ability. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 367(1594), 1338–1349 (2012).
Dixon, M. L. et al. Frontoparietal and default mode network contributions to self-referential processing in social anxiety disorder. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 22(1), 187–198 (2022).
Witt, S. T., van Ettinger-Veenstra, H., Salo, T., Riedel, M. C. & Laird, A. R. What executive function network is that? An image-based meta-analysis of network labels. Brain Topogr. 34, 1–10 (2021).
Hasenkamp, W., Wilson-Mendenhall, C. D., Duncan, E. & Barsalou, L. W. Mind wandering and attention during focused meditation: A fine-grained temporal analysis of fluctuating cognitive states. Neuroimage 59(1), 750–760 (2012).
Berkman, E. T. & Falk, E. B. Beyond brain mapping: Using neural measures to predict real-world outcomes. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 22(1), 45–50 (2013).
Gillan, C. M. & Rutledge, R. B. Smartphones and the neuroscience of mental health. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 44, 129–151 (2021).
Bijsterbosch, J. et al. Challenges and future directions for representations of functional brain organization. Nat. Neurosci. 23(12), 1484–1495 (2020).
Woo, C. W., Chang, L. J., Lindquist, M. A. & Wager, T. D. Building better biomarkers: Brain models in translational neuroimaging. Nat. Neurosci. 20(3), 365–377 (2017).
Douw, L. et al. The road ahead in clinical network neuroscience. Netw. Neurosci. 3(4), 969–993 (2019).
Britton, W. B. et al. Dismantling mindfulness-based cognitive therapy: Creation and validation of 8-week focused attention and open monitoring interventions within a 3-armed randomized controlled trial. Behav. Res. Ther. 101, 92–107 (2018).
Creswell, J. D. Mindfulness interventions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 68, 491–516 (2017).
Bishop, S. R. et al. Mindfulness: A proposed operational definition. Clin. Psychol. Sci. Pract. 11(3), 230 (2004).
Wells, R. E. et al. Meditation’s impact on default mode network and hippocampus in mild cognitive impairment: A pilot study. Neurosci. Lett. 556, 15–19 (2013).
Taren, A. A. Prefrontal Regulatory Mechanisms of Mindfulness and Stress Reduction and Links to Markers of Health (Doctoral dissertation, University of Pittsburgh) (2015).
Taren, A. A. et al. Mindfulness meditation training alters stress-related amygdala resting state functional connectivity: A randomized controlled trial. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 10(12), 1758–1768 (2015).
Shao, R., Keuper, K., Geng, X. & Lee, T. M. Pons to posterior cingulate functional projections predict affective processing changes in the elderly following eight weeks of meditation training. EBioMedicine 10, 236–248 (2016).
Kwak, S. et al. The immediate and sustained positive effects of meditation on resilience are mediated by changes in the resting brain. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 13, 101 (2019).
Turpyn, C. C. et al. Affective neural mechanisms of a parenting-focused mindfulness intervention. Mindfulness 12(2), 392–404 (2019).
Van der Gucht, K. et al. Effects of a mindfulness-based intervention on cancer-related cognitive impairment: Results of a randomized controlled functional magnetic resonance imaging pilot study. Cancer 126(18), 4246–4255 (2020).
Chumachenko, S. Y. et al. Keeping weight off: Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction alters amygdala functional connectivity during weight loss maintenance in a randomized control trial. PLoS One 16(1), e0244847 (2021).
Rahrig H., Bjork, J. M., Creswell, J. M., Lindsay, E. K., Brown, K. W. Mindfulness training alters default mode network connectivity. Manuscript in preparation.
Yarkoni, T., Poldrack, R. A., Nichols, T. E., Van Essen, D. C. & Wager, T. D. Large-scale automated synthesis of human functional neuroimaging data. Nat. Methods 8(8), 665–670 (2011).
Yeo, B. T. T. et al. The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J. Neurophysiol. 106(3), 1125–1165 (2011).
Altman, D. G. Practical Statistics for Medical Research (CRC Press, 1990).
Landis, J. R. & Koch, G. G. The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics 33, 159–174 (1977).
Radua, J. & Mataix-Cols, D. Meta-analytic methods for neuroimaging data explained. Biol. Mood Anxiety Disord. 2(1), 1–11 (2012).
Albajes-Eizagirre, A., Solanes, A., Vieta, E. & Radua, J. Voxel-based meta-analysis via permutation of subject images (PSI): Theory and implementation for SDM. Neuroimage 186, 174–184 (2019).
Eickhoff, S. B. et al. Coordinate-based activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis of neuroimaging data: A random-effects approach based on empirical estimates of spatial uncertainty. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30(9), 2907–2926 (2009).
Wager, T. D., Lindquist, M. & Kaplan, L. Meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging data: Current and future directions. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2(2), 150–158 (2007).
Winkler, A. M., Ridgway, G. R., Webster, M. A., Smith, S. M. & Nichols, T. E. Permutation inference for the general linear model. Neuroimage 92, 381–397 (2014).
Smith, S. M. & Nichols, T. E. Threshold-free cluster enhancement: Addressing problems of smoothing, threshold dependence and localisation in cluster inference. Neuroimage 44(1), 83–98 (2009).
Rubin, D. B. Multiple Imputation for Nonresponse in Surveys, vol. 81. (Wiley, 2004).
Albajes-Eizagirre, A., Solanes, A. & Radua, J. Meta-analysis of non-statistically significant unreported effects. Stat. Methods Med. Res. 28(12), 3741–3754 (2019).
Radua, J. et al. Anisotropic kernels for coordinate-based meta-analyses of neuroimaging studies. Front. Psychol. 5, 13 (2014).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
The authors confirm contribution to the paper as follows: study conception and design: H.R., K.W.B.; data extraction and curation: M.P., A.A., N.L.; analysis and interpretation of results: H.R.; K.W.B., D.R.V.; draft manuscript preparation: H.R., K.W.B., D.R.V.
Corresponding authors
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Rahrig, H., Vago, D.R., Passarelli, M.A. et al. Meta-analytic evidence that mindfulness training alters resting state default mode network connectivity. Sci Rep 12, 12260 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-15195-6
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-15195-6
Comments
By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.