Abstract
The work of McCloskey and Cohen popularized the concept of catastrophic interference. They used a neural network that tried to learn addition using two groups of examples as two different tasks. In their case, learning the second task rapidly deteriorated the acquired knowledge about the previous one. We hypothesize that this could be a symptom of a fundamental problem: addition is an algorithmic task that should not be learned through pattern recognition. Therefore, other model architectures better suited for this task would avoid catastrophic forgetting. We use a neural network with a different architecture that can be trained to recover the correct algorithm for the addition of binary numbers. This neural network includes conditional clauses that are naturally treated within the back-propagation algorithm. We test it in the setting proposed by McCloskey and Cohen and training on random additions one by one. The neural network not only does not suffer from catastrophic forgetting but it improves its predictive power on unseen pairs of numbers as training progresses. We also show that this is a robust effect, also present when averaging many simulations. This work emphasizes the importance that neural network architecture has for the emergence of catastrophic forgetting and introduces a neural network that is able to learn an algorithm.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
Catastrophic forgetting or catastrophic interference is today a central paradigm of modern machine learning. It was introduced in 1989 by McCloskey and Cohen1 when they systematically showed how learning new information by a fully connected neural network leads to the rapid disruption of old knowledge. This work was followed by many others that confirmed this phenomenology using a myriad of different models and tasks2,3,4,5,6,7,8,9.
In this context, a neural network (NN) represents a nonlinear mapping between input and output spaces. This nonlinear mapping depends on many parameters (the weights of the NN) that can be tuned to achieve the desired behavior, e.g. that a specific image (input) maps to the correct class in the output. In supervised classification tasks, many labeled examples are used to build a loss function that quantifies the performance of the NN. Training the NN consists on minimizing this loss function with respect to the weights, so that the system moves in parameter space to a region where it correctly classifies most of the training data. In the classic work by McCloskey and Cohen they used a NN that took as input two numbers and output another one. They used this architecture to learn two different tasks: the NN was first trained to output the correct answer to the “ones addition facts” (\(1+1\) through \(9+1\) and \(1+1\) through \(1+9\)). Once the network learned the first task, they trained it on the “twos addition facts” (\(1+2\) through \(9+2\) and \(2+1\) through \(2+9\)). Although they changed many components of their setup, they always found that training on the second task erased any knowledge about the previous one. In other words, when the system tried to minimize the loss function corresponding to the second task, it moved to a region of parameter space where it was not able to solve the first task. Inspired by human and animal learning10,11,12,13,14,15,16 or even by the behavior of physical systems17,18,19,20,21,22,23,24, modern approaches to this problem try to constrain the change of the parameters during training of the second task to avoid forgetting the first one25,26,27,28,29,30,31,32,33,34,35.
But the definition of “task” can be subtle. In the case of McCloskey and Cohen, should not the addition of any two numbers be a unique task? We would definitely claim that a child has learned addition only if it is able to sum any two numbers (not only the ones already shown to them). The fact that learning a subset of additions interfered with a previously learned group of sums indicates that their NN was trying to memorize (as in pattern recognition) the results of the operations. We hypothesize that, if a different NN was able to learn the algorithm of addition (instead of learning to recognize particular examples), showing new data would lead to better performance (positive transfer) instead of catastrophic interference. In the rest of this paper, we aim to create a neural network architecture that can learn the rules of addition to avoid catastrophic forgetting. We hope that this work will motivate machine learning practitioners working on different areas to consider new architectures that can transform catastrophic forgetting into positive transfer in other frameworks.
The mathematical model
Addition of binary numbers
Computers are already programmed to perform the addition of binary numbers, we would like to define a NN that can change its internal weights to find an equivalent algorithm. We can recast the algorithm of addition of binary numbers in a way that resembles the typical structure of NNs. Figure 1 represents the algorithm for the addition of two binary numbers (\(N_1\) and \(N_2\)). It outputs the correct result \(N_4\) and it also creates an additional array, \(N_3\), keeping track of the ones that are carried to the next step. Each of these binary numbers (\(N_m\)) are composed of binary digits (\(\{1, 0\}\)) that we notate \(N_m^n\), where n is an index indicating the position of the binary digit.
To add two binary numbers—see Fig. 1—, the operator \(U_{ijkl}\) acts secuencially on the input arrays (\(N_1^n\), \(N_2^n\) and \(N_3^n\)) and outputs two numbers at each step, \(U_{N_3^nN_2^nN_1^nl}\), that correspond to \(N_3^{n+1}\) and \(N_4^n\), where \(N_4\) is the result of the sum. In this way, \(U_{ijkl}\) acts similarly to a convolutional filter, with two main differences:
-
Instead of performing a weighted average of \(N_1^n\), \(N_2^n\) and \(N_3^n\), it uses their value to choose one option (it is performing three conditional clauses).
-
One component of the output (\(U_{N_3^nN_2^nN_1^n0}\)) goes into the next line of the input (\(N_3^{n+1}\)). There is information from the previous step that goes into the next one.
See Fig. 1 for an example. We identify \(N_3^n\), \(N_2^n\) and \(N_1^n\) as the ijk indices in \(U_{ijkl}\). In the first step, \(N_1^0=1\), \(N_2^0=1\) and \(N_3^0=0\), and the output is \(U_{011l}=(1,0)\), where (1, 0) correspond to the next empty spaces of \(N_3\) and \(N_4\) (\(N_3^{1}\) and \(N_4^0\)). Iteratively applying this operator we get the correct values for \(N_3\) and \(N_4\). If \(N_1\) and \(N_2\) have Z digits, \(N_3\) and \(N_4\) will have \(Z+1\) digits and in the last line we just identify \(N_4^{Z+1}=N_3^{Z+1}\). In the example shown in Fig. 1 this algorithm gets the correct answer for \(29+43=72\).
Building an algorithmic neural network
We can define now the architecture of our neural network. We would like to have \(U_{ijkl}\) as a specific (learned) state in its parameter space. For simplicity, we define our NN using a modified operator, \({\hat{U}}_{ijkl}\):
where S() stands for the sigmoid operator,
and \(a_{ijkl}\), \(i,j,k,l \in \{0,1\}\) are 16 parameters that have to be learned. Our neural network takes the operator \({\hat{U}}_{ijkl}\) and applies it to the input numbers (\(N_1\) and \(N_2\)) as shown in Fig. 1 for \(U_{ijkl}\). In this process the network creates two new arrays corresponding to \(N_3\) and \(N_4\). The predicted answer to the input addition is \(N_4\), then let us use the notation \(N_4 = \mathscr{F}(N_1,N_2,a_{ijkl})\) to highlight that our neural network is a nonlinear funtion of inputs \(N_1\) and \(N_2\) and of the parameters \(a_{ijkl}\). There is a fundamental difference with the previous case, now the digits of \(N_3\) and \(N_4\) will be real numbers between 0 and 1 and we have to decide how to apply \({\hat{U}}_{ijkl}\) when \(N_3^n\) is different from \(\{0,1\}\).
If we apply the operator \({\hat{U}}_{ijkl}\) to the same example shown in Fig. 1, the first line is again \(N_1^0=1\), \(N_2^0=1\) and \(N_3^0=0\) and the output now will be \(N_3^1=S(a_{0110})\) and \(N_4^0=S(a_{0111})\). In the second line we find now \(N_1^1=0\), \(N_2^1=1\) and \(N_3^1=S(a_{0110})\), since \(S(a_{0110})\) is a real number between 0 and 1, we compute the output of this line as a combination of \({\hat{U}}_{010l}\) and \({\hat{U}}_{110l}\) in the following way:
where we use the dot product, \(\cdot\), for clarity. In this way, if \(N_3^1=S(a_{0110})\) is exactly equal to 0 or 1 we recover the theoretical algorithm shown in Fig. 1, and when \(S(a_{0110}) \in (0,1)\) the output is a combination of both options weighted by \(S(a_{0110})\). Applying this operator iteratively, the output of our neural network is computed, \(N_4 = \mathscr{F}(N_1,N_2,a_{ijkl})\).
To define a learning process we create a loss function. In the simplest case, we would like to learn the addition of two specific numbers (\(N_1 + N_2\)), we can define the loss function as:
where \({\tilde{N}}_4\) is the correct result of the sum \(N_1 + N_2\), and \({\tilde{N}}_4^n\) and \(\mathscr{F}(N_1,N_2,a_{ijkl})^n\) are the nth binary digit of \({\tilde{N}}_4\) and \(\mathscr{F}(N_1,N_2,a_{ijkl})\), respectively. To compute \(\mathscr{F}(N_1,N_2,a_{ijkl})\) our model uses sums, multiplications and a nonlinear function, S(), resembling the standard convolutional filters extensively used in deep neural networks. Finally, we can differentiate \(\mathscr{L}\) with respect to the parameters of the neural network (\(a_{ijkl}\)) using standard methods36. Training the NN consists of changing the value of the parameters (\(a_{ijkl}\)) following \(-\frac{\partial \mathscr{L}}{\partial a_{ijkl}}\), such that the loss function is minimized.
Results
Learning the ones and twos addition facts: the problem proposed by McCloskey and Cohen
In the problem proposed by McCloskey and Cohen, a neural network is trained using two tasks: the “ones addition facts” (all the additions of 1 with another digit, \(1+1=2\) through \(9+1=10\) and \(1+1=2\) through \(1+9=10\)) and the “twos addition facts” (\(1+2=3\) through \(9+2=11\) and \(2+1=3\) through \(2+9=11\)). In their work, the neural network catastrophically forgets the first task when training on the second one. We aim to show that this was due to an inadequate choice of model architecture and that a different architecture, such as the one proposed in this work, will not display catastrophic forgetting when training on these tasks.
We create two datasets, one for the ones and another for the twos addition facts. We will train the model using the ones addition facts first, then we will continue training the same model using the twos addition facts. We create a loss function for each of these datasets, for each pair of numbers \(N_1\) and \(N_2\) we compute the correct result \(N_1 + N_2 = {\tilde{N}}_4\) and compute their corresponding loss, using equation (5). The total loss for each of the tasks is the average value of Eq. (5) evaluated for each pair of numbers in the task (e.g. \(1+2=3\) through \(9+2=11\) and \(2+1=3\) through \(2+9=11\)). We train the network using gradient descent with learning rate equal to 1. We train for 2000 steps using the “ones addition facts” loss and for another 2000 steps using the “twos addition facts” loss. In Fig. 2 we plot both losses during the learning process to quantify the performance of the model for both tasks. Figure 2a shows that the loss functions corresponding to both tasks greatly decrease when training on the “ones addition facts”: learning the “ones addition facts” has a positive transfer to the “twos addition facts”. Similarly, both loss functions keep decreasing when training on the “twos addition facts”: there is no catastrophic forgetting and the model shows positive backward transfer from the new task to the previous one. Figure 2b shows the evolution of the parameters of the network. At initialization, the parameters of the network (\(a_{ijkl}\)) are real random numbers between \(-1\) and 1, what leads to \(S(a_{ijkl})\) being randomly distributed within \(1/(1+e)\sim 0.27\) and \(e/(1+e)\sim 0.73\). To recover the correct addition algorithm, \(U_{ijkl}\), the black continuous lines should saturate to one whereas the red dash lines should go to zero as learning progresses. Up to step \(\sim 90\) in the minimization process, we observe some lines performing non-monotonic behaviors until all of them (except for two black lines) tend to the correct values, we will term this region the non-trivial learning regime. For minimization steps larger than \(\sim 100\) (including when training switches to the “twos addition facts”) learning continues smoothly and all the parameters (except two) approach their asymptotic vales, what we will term the trivial learning regime. The two black lines that do not approach 1 correspond to \(S(a_{111l})\). The reason for this behavior is that the tasks used here do not contain any addition that would require these parameters, only used when \(N_1^n=N_2^n=N_3^n=1\) (for a specific n), for example in \(3+3\).
Learning to sum, one example at a time
We now perform a second experiment where we train on one sample at a time. Each sample consists on two integers chosen at random (\(N_1 + N_2 = {\tilde{N}}_4\)). This is more challenging than the example of the previous section. Before, the system was trained on a group of additions, the loss function was imposing more constraints on the parameters, and training led to the correct values of \(a_{ijkl}\). Now, the model is trained using one sample at a time, which gives more freedom to the parameters. We train on each sample during 50 steps using gradient descent on Eq. (5). Figure 3 shows the process of training the model for 200 different samples (one after another, 10000 steps in total). Test loss is computed every step as the average loss for 100 samples that are not included in the training dataset. During training we observe that the training loss (blue line in Fig. 3) decreases for 50 steps every time that a new example is shown, this indicates that the operator \({\hat{U}}_{ijkl}\) is changing (learning) to correctly add this particular pair of numbers. When we switch to the next sample there is a sudden increment of the loss, followed by a decrease for another 50 steps. However, the test loss (contrary to the previous section) increases when training on the first samples, indicating the presence of overfitting: the NN is learning the addition of a pair of numbers, but the rules it is learning do not generalize to the rest of samples. From step \(\sim 400\) forward, the test loss shows a steady decrease. Panel (b) of Fig. 3 shows the value of the parameters \(S(a_{ijkl})\) at all times. Similarly to the previous section, there is a non-trivial regime where parameters show non-monotonic behavior. In this case, they show more sudden transitions due to the change in training data every 50 steps. After step \(\sim 400\) all parameters approach their correct values leading to a trivial learning regime.
One could wonder how reproducible are the results of Fig. 3, or if the learning process could get stuck in the non-trivial regime, preventing the system to reach the trivial regime and correctly learn addition. To study the robustness of this behavior we perform now 70 different simulations in the same conditions of Fig. 3 (training on one example at a time for 50 minimization steps). For each simulation we use a new random initialization and different training samples picked at random, the test dataset is the same for all the simulations. We plot the average values of the training and test loss functions in Fig. 4. The inset shows the same data but in a semi-log scale, including shaded areas around the mean values that correspond to one standard deviation of the data. Training on each sample leads to a decreasing training loss and an increasing test loss (overfitting). The average values show a final regime (from step \(\sim 5000\) forward) where all simulations collapse (note vanishing standard deviation in the inset). A fit to this regime, dashed line in Fig. 4, shows a power law behavior with an exponent \(\sim -1\). Fig. 3 is a good representative of the behavior of most simulations contained in Fig. 4, most of then reach the trivial regime around step \(\sim 1000\). However, some outliers take much longer to find the trivial learning regime, leading to the plateau observed in the test loss between step \(\sim 1000\) and \(\sim 5000\), when the last outlier reaches the trivial learning regime. These results prove that this model architecture shows positive transfer, changing “tasks” leads to better performance in previous and future tasks instead of catastrophic interference. When we stop training, our NN has learned the rules of addition, it will be able to sum any pair of numbers whether they were included in the training data or not.
Analysis of the results
In the cases shown in Figs. 3 and 4, we use binary numbers of dimension 5 for \(N_1\) and \(N_2\). The largest number considered in the additions is 11111, 31 in decimal form. Therefore, there are \((31 \cdot 31) = 961\) possible combinations to create our training and test datasets of additions. As we have seen in Figs. 3 and 4 it was enough to take around 500 minimization steps (\(\sim 10\) different samples) to correctly learn addition and enter the trivial learning regime. After this, all the parameters start to asymptotically approach their theoretical value and the loss displays a power law decay with an exponent \(\sim -1\).
Figure 2 also shows how learning in our model is divided in non-trivial and trivial learning regimes. In the former case, the evolution of the parameters (\(a_{ijkl}\)) are coupled to each other, displaying non-monotonic behavior. This regime is characterized by a non-trivial exponent 0.2, for which we do not have an analytical derivation. In the later regime, the coefficients asymptotically approach their theoretical values \(a_{ijkl} \rightarrow \pm \infty\) such that \(S(a_{ijkl}) \rightarrow 0, 1\). The loss function that the system minimizes is a sum of terms that compare each binary digit of the correct result with the number predicted by the network, \(\left( {\tilde{N}}_4^n-\mathscr{F}(N_1,N_2,a_{ijkl})^n\right) ^2\). In this regime, the dynamics of the parameters of the network are uncoupled and all behave in an equivalent manner. For simplicity, let us study the evolution of one of the parameters that we term a, in this regime there are two possibilities \(a \rightarrow \pm \infty\),
Keeping up to leading order in \(e^{-|a|}\), the terms appearing in the loss function take the following forms:
All the terms included in the loss function are then proportional to the ones in equation (7) and we can study the evolution of one of them. Let us take the case \(e^{2a}, \ a \rightarrow -\infty\). Our dynamics are discrete, but we assume that the continuous limit is a good approximation in the trivial learning regime. If we term t the minimization time, the parameter a evolves as
we can integrate this equation as,
where C is an additive constant that would have the information about the initial condition of a. This constant can be neglected in the limit \(t \rightarrow \infty\). Solving (9) for a we find,
indicating that, in the trivial learning regime, the parameters \(a_{ijkl}\) tend to \(\pm \infty\) in a logarithmic manner. Finally, since the loss if a sum of terms proportional to the ones in equation (7), using again the case \(a\rightarrow -\infty\), we get,
Equation (11) recovers the scaling observed numerically in the trivial regime of Figs. 2 and 4, \(\mathscr{L}(t) \sim t^{-1}\).
Discussion
The NN defined in this work is able to learn to sum any two numbers when trained on a finite set of examples. But should we still consider the addition of different pairs of numbers as different tasks? It probably depends on the NN that is being used. If the NN is only able to perform some version of pattern recognition, it will not be able to extract any common rules and training on different samples will lead to catastrophic forgetting. However, if the NN has the necessary set of tools (as shown in this work) the addition of different pairs of numbers constitute different examples of the same task, and training on all of them has a positive effect. This is similar to humans that can transfer previous knowledge when they have a deep, rather than superficial, understanding12.
We have defined a NN that is algorithmically aligned37,38 with the correct algorithm for the sum of binary numbers. This NN was able to learn the correct parameters through gradient descent, when different examples of additions were used as training data. To define this NN we have created a layer that operates similarly to a traditional convolutional layer but with three important differences:
-
It performs different operations based on the input (equivalent to “if” clauses), instead of performing a weighted average of the input values (filter).
-
At every step it passes information to the next line of the input (\(N_3^{n+1}\)).
-
If the input (\(N_3^n\)) is not 0 or 1 the network combines both options of the corresponding “if” clause weighting them with \(N_3^n\).
The mathematical operations required to apply our model to the input data, and to build the loss function, are just sums, multiplications and the sigmoid nonlinear function, similar to standard convolutional filters. This allows us to use standard back-propagation algorithms. Since our model has 16 parameters, we have not perfommed a systematic study of the runtimes. This could be necessary if this model were to be combined with standard deep learning layers.
In future work we would like to increase the complexity of this algorithmic neural network, which is able to perform different tasks depending on the input. It should be possible to combine this NN with other types of neural networks to create a complete model with the capacity to learn an algorithm at the same time that takes advantage of the power of other NNs (e.g. convolutional NNs). Hopefully, this could help “building causal models of the world that support explanation and understanding, rather than merely solving pattern recognition problems”39. Training these complete models can lead to a non-convex optimization problem that can benefit from changing the topography of the loss function landscape40, an effective way of doing this in machine learning is the use of dynamical loss functions41.
In the work of McCloskey and Cohen the addition of two groups of numbers were considered as two different tasks. When training on the second task the previous knowledge was erased. However, this could have been just a symptom of using a pattern-recognition neural network for the detection of seemingly different tasks. Addition is the paradigm of algorithmic tasks, which should not be learned through the memorization of a large number of examples but through finding/learning the correct algorithm. We hope these results will inspire practitioners to explore new model architectures when encountering catastrophic forgetting within standard frameworks.
References
McCloskey, M. & Cohen, N. J. Catastrophic interference in connectionist networks: The sequential learning problem. In Psychology of Learning and Motivation, vol. 24, 109–165 (Elsevier, 1989).
Ratcliff, R. Connectionist models of recognition memory: Constraints imposed by learning and forgetting functions. Psychol. Rev. 97, 285 (1990).
Lewandowsky, S. & Li, S.-C. Catastrophic interference in neural networks: Causes, solutions, and data. In Interference and Inhibition in Cognition, 329–361 (Elsevier, 1995).
French, R. M. Catastrophic forgetting in connectionist networks. Trends Cogn. Sci. 3, 128–135 (1999).
Goodfellow, I. J., Mirza, M., Xiao, D., Courville, A. & Bengio, Y. An empirical investigation of catastrophic forgetting in gradient-based neural networks. arXiv preprint arXiv:1312.6211 (2013).
Srivastava, R. K., Masci, J., Kazerounian, S., Gomez, F. J. & Schmidhuber, J. Compete to compute. In NIPS, 2310–2318 (Citeseer, 2013).
Nguyen, C. V. et al. Toward understanding catastrophic forgetting in continual learning. arXiv preprint arXiv:1908.01091 (2019).
Mirzadeh, S. I., Farajtabar, M., Pascanu, R. & Ghasemzadeh, H. Understanding the role of training regimes in continual learning. arXiv preprint arXiv:2006.06958 (2020).
Lee, S., Goldt, S. & Saxe, A. Continual learning in the teacher-student setup: Impact of task similarity. In International Conference on Machine Learning, 6109–6119 (PMLR, 2021).
McClelland, J. L., McNaughton, B. L. & O’Reilly, R. C. Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: Insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory. Psychol. Rev. 102, 419 (1995).
Braun, C. et al. Dynamic organization of the somatosensory cortex induced by motor activity. Brain 124, 2259–2267 (2001).
Barnett, S. M. & Ceci, S. J. When and where do we apply what we learn?: A taxonomy for far transfer. Psychol. Bull. 128, 612 (2002).
Yang, G. et al. Sleep promotes branch-specific formation of dendritic spines after learning. Science 344, 1173–1178 (2014).
Cichon, J. & Gan, W.-B. Branch-specific dendritic Ca2+ spikes cause persistent synaptic plasticity. Nature 520, 180–185 (2015).
Musslick, S. et al. Multitasking Capability Versus Learning Efficiency in Neural Network Architectures (Cognitive Science Society, 2017).
Flesch, T., Balaguer, J., Dekker, R., Nili, H. & Summerfield, C. Comparing continual task learning in minds and machines. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 115, E10313–E10322 (2018).
Pine, D. J., Gollub, J. P., Brady, J. F. & Leshansky, A. M. Chaos and threshold for irreversibility in sheared suspensions. Nature 438, 997–1000 (2005).
Keim, N. C. & Nagel, S. R. Generic transient memory formation in disordered systems with noise. Phys. Rev. Lett. 107, 010603 (2011).
Keim, N. C. & Arratia, P. E. Mechanical and microscopic properties of the reversible plastic regime in a 2D jammed material. Phys. Rev. Lett. 112, 028302 (2014).
Hexner, D., Liu, A. J. & Nagel, S. R. Periodic training of creeping solids. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 117, 31690–31695 (2020).
Sachdeva, V., Husain, K., Sheng, J., Wang, S. & Murugan, A. Tuning environmental timescales to evolve and maintain generalists. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 117, 12693–12699 (2020).
Stern, M., Arinze, C., Perez, L., Palmer, S. E. & Murugan, A. Supervised learning through physical changes in a mechanical system. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 117, 14843–14850 (2020).
Stern, M., Pinson, M. B. & Murugan, A. Continual learning of multiple memories in mechanical networks. Phys. Rev. X 10, 031044 (2020).
Dillavou, S., Stern, M., Liu, A. J. & Durian, D. J. Demonstration of decentralized, physics-driven learning. arXiv preprint arXiv:2108.00275 (2021).
Rusu, A. A. et al. Progressive neural networks. arXiv preprint arXiv:1606.04671 (2016).
Kirkpatrick, J. et al. Overcoming catastrophic forgetting in neural networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 114, 3521–3526 (2017).
Zenke, F., Poole, B. & Ganguli, S. Continual learning through synaptic intelligence. In International Conference on Machine Learning, 3987–3995 (PMLR, 2017).
Parisi, G. I., Tani, J., Weber, C. & Wermter, S. Lifelong learning of human actions with deep neural network self-organization. Neural Netw. 96, 137–149 (2017).
Lopez-Paz, D. & Ranzato, M. Gradient episodic memory for continual learning. Adv. Neural. Inf. Process. Syst. 30, 6467–6476 (2017).
Shin, H., Lee, J. K., Kim, J. & Kim, J. Continual learning with deep generative replay. arXiv preprint arXiv:1705.08690 (2017).
Lee, S.-W., Kim, J.-H., Jun, J., Ha, J.-W. & Zhang, B.-T. Overcoming catastrophic forgetting by incremental moment matching. arXiv preprint arXiv:1703.08475 (2017).
Riemer, M. et al. Learning to learn without forgetting by maximizing transfer and minimizing interference. arXiv preprint arXiv:1810.11910 (2018).
De Lange, M. et al. Continual learning: A comparative study on how to defy forgetting in classification tasks. arXiv preprint arXiv:1909.083832 (2019).
Farajtabar, M., Azizan, N., Mott, A. & Li, A. Orthogonal gradient descent for continual learning. In International Conference on Artificial Intelligence and Statistics, 3762–3773 (PMLR, 2020).
Doan, T., Bennani, M. A., Mazoure, B., Rabusseau, G. & Alquier, P. A theoretical analysis of catastrophic forgetting through the ntk overlap matrix. In International Conference on Artificial Intelligence and Statistics, 1072–1080 (PMLR, 2021).
Code reproducing our main results can be found at: https://github.com/miguel-rg/learning_addition.
Xu, K. et al. How neural networks extrapolate: From feedforward to graph neural networks. arXiv preprint arXiv:2009.11848 (2020).
Xu, K. et al. What can neural networks reason about? arXiv preprint arXiv:1905.13211 (2019).
Lake, B. M., Ullman, T. D., Tenenbaum, J. B. & Gershman, S. J. Building machines that learn and think like people. Behavioral and brain sciences40 (2017).
Ruiz-García, M., Liu, A. J. & Katifori, E. Tuning and jamming reduced to their minima. Phys. Rev. E 100, 052608 (2019).
Ruiz-Garcia, M., Zhang, G., Schoenholz, S. S. & Liu, A. J. Tilting the playing field: Dynamical loss functions for machine learning. In International Conference on Machine Learning (pp. 9157–9167). PMLR. (2021)
Bradbury, J. et al. JAX: Composable transformations of Python+NumPy programs (2018).
Acknowledgements
This work was motivated by a talk given by Irina Rish as part of ICML2021. We thank Rahul Chacko, Farshid Jafarpour, Stefano Sarao Mannelli, Andrew Saxe, Markus Spitzer and Nachi Stern for stimulating discussions. We have used Google JAX42 to perform our experiments. We acknowledge support from the CONEX-Plus programme funded by Universidad Carlos III de Madrid and the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme under the Marie Sklodowska-Curie grant agreement No. 801538.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
M.R.-G. carried out all the research presented in this work.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The author declares no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Ruiz-Garcia, M. Model architecture can transform catastrophic forgetting into positive transfer. Sci Rep 12, 10736 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-14348-x
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-14348-x
Comments
By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.