Abstract
Among animal species, the songs of male humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae) are a rare example of social learning between entire populations. Understanding fine-scale similarity in song patterns and structural features will better clarify how accurately songs are learned during inter-population transmission. Here, six distinct song types (2009–2015) transmitted from the east Australian to New Caledonian populations were quantitatively analysed using fine-scale song features. Results found that New Caledonian whales learned each song type with high accuracy regardless of the pattern’s complexity. However, there were rare instances of themes (stereotyped patterns of sound units) only sung by a single population. These occurred more often in progressively changing ‘evolutionary’ songs compared to rapidly changing ‘revolutionary’ songs. Our results suggest that populations do not need to reduce complexity to accurately learn song patterns. Populations may also incorporate changes and embellishments into songs in the form of themes which are suggested to be learnt as distinct segments. Maintaining complex song patterns with such accuracy suggests significant acoustic contact, supporting the hypothesis that song learning may occur on shared feeding grounds or migration routes. This study improves the understanding of inter-population mechanisms for large-scale cultural transmission in animals.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
Culture, once thought to be uniquely human, is found in a wide range of animal species. Individuals acquire a specific behaviour or trait through contact with another individual or their products, known as social learning1. Cultural transmission of these behaviours can occur between related individuals (e.g., tool use in a matriline of bottlenose dolphins [Tursiops truncatus]2), social groups (e.g., sweet potato washing in a tribe of Japanese macaques [Macaca fuscata]3), or populations (e.g., geographically distinct birdsong dialects4). Primates and cetaceans possess a varied and complex set of cultural traits, surpassed only by those found in humans5,6. Studies across these taxa groups allow for a comparative exploration of the common evolutionary drivers that lead to cultural traditions and their transmission7. Importantly, studies on cetaceans broaden the scope of such comparisons by including species from a non-primate lineage. One exemplary model species to contribute to the discussion of animal cultural exchange is the humpback whale (Megaptera novaeangliae) due to the presence of cultural traits in multiple aspects of their ecology. These include novel foraging strategies8, maternally directed site fidelity9,10,11, and a complex song display12,13,14.
A clear example of broad-scale cultural exchange among multiple populations is the transmission of humpback whale song within the South Pacific meta-population14. Song patterns are transmitted eastward from the west Australian population first to east Australia, then to New Caledonia, Tonga, and American Samoa, and then on to the Cook Islands, and French Polynesia14. However, the mechanism(s) for this transmission are not fully understood. Payne and Guinee15 proposed three possible vectors for song transmission between populations: (1) inter-population movement of individuals within a season, (2) inter-population movement of individuals between seasons, or (3) acoustic contact along migratory routes or feeding grounds shared among populations. The first vector, inter-population movement within a season (June-November which encompasses both the migratory and breeding seasons in the South Pacific) is relatively rare in the South Pacific region, though it has been documented10,16,17. More commonly reported are inter-population movements between seasons10,18, shared migratory routes19,20, and singing on the Antarctic feeding grounds21, presenting the second and third vectors as more plausible mechanisms of transmission for the South Pacific. However, not all populations necessarily use the same mechanisms of song exchange. Movement of entire song patterns across multiple populations, as observed in the South Pacific, has not yet been documented in any other location worldwide14,22, or any other species except humans.
Humpback whale song is a long, complex vocal display produced solely by males13. Individual sounds called ‘units’ are arranged in a sequence, which is termed a ‘phrase’13,23. Phrases are repeated multiple times to create a ‘theme’. Themes are then sung in a consistent order without repetition, creating a ‘song’. The song evolves over time through small, progressive changes, which all singers adopt through social learning24. These changes result in each year’s song containing a slightly different arrangement, known as a ‘song type’. In the east Australian population, small, progressive changes to songs (known as ‘evolutions’14,24,25) tend to increase the song pattern’s complexity (quantified using ‘complexity scores’26). For example, songs increase in duration, new themes are added, and a wider variety of units are used as songs evolve. Following these evolutionary changes, songs may also undergo a radical population-wide change known as a ‘revolution’, where a different song type introduced from the west Australian population entirely replaces the existing song12. Revolutionary songs tend to have lower complexity than the songs they replace, possibly as a result of limitations in learning such a large amount of novel material26.
Much of what is known about humpback whale song learning and fine-scale song structure comes from the songs of the east Australian population12,14,27,28. There is typically a consistent one-year delay in song transmission from east Australia to its closest neighbouring population, New Caledonia14,29. Although the same eastward one-year transmission in song also occurs from the west Australian population to east Australia, this is intermittent and has only been documented in ‘revolution’ years30. Songs are transmitted with strong song similarity on a broad scale (i.e., the sequences and occurrences of the themes present)14. Understanding fine-scale similarity (i.e., the sequences and occurrences of individual sound units within each theme) in song patterns and structural features will better clarify how accurately songs are learned during inter-population transmission.
Here, we analysed six separate song types, four revolutions and two evolutions, that were first recorded in the east Australian population and subsequently transmitted to the New Caledonian population the following year. This allowed for a direct and fine-scale quantitative comparison of song features using the same song patterns sung by two separate populations, both in situations where a known song is modified with moderate amounts of novel material (‘evolutionary’) and where the song is unknown and entirely novel (‘revolutionary’). Complexity scores were calculated using fine-scale song features including both the total and unique number of units, phrase and song duration, and individual theme complexity level. Identifying fine-scale commonalities and differences across populations will complement previous broad-scale works and improve our understanding of how inter-population song learning occurs.
Results
Song sharing between populations
Of the 40 total themes identified from the six song types (Purple, Light Purple, Brown, Light Brown, Teal, Orange, spectrograms in Figs. S1–6), most (29 themes) were shared between populations and a few (11 themes) were only recorded in a single population (Table 1). However, unique themes were rarely sung (across song types, unique themes only comprised 0–12.7% of all phrase repetitions, Figs. S1–6), precluding any consistent population-distinct versions of the respective song types. Unique themes occurred more frequently in the two evolutionary song types (Light Purple and Light Brown, Fig. 1) and were also more common in the New Caledonian population (Table 1). Neither shared nor unique themes had defining acoustic characteristics. Both types of themes contained units that covered the spectrum of acoustic features such as frequency (i.e., low- and high-pitched calls), duration (i.e., short and long calls), and modulation (i.e., flat and fluctuating calls). Such a range of call types suggests that acoustic features were not a driving factor in whether a theme was transmitted from one population to another.
Complexity
Complexity scores were calculated using a Principal Component Analysis (PCA) of variables representing song pattern at both the song (average total units, average unique units, average song length) and theme (average number of themes, average phrase length, average theme complexity) levels of the song (Allen, et al.26). These scores serve as a reliable indicator of relative complexity within a song arrangement26,31,32. There were no significant differences in complexity scores of song types between songs recorded in the east Australian and songs recorded in the New Caledonian populations (Fig. 2, Mann–Whitney/Wilcoxon test, W = 28, p = 0.132). This indicates that if a song type was particularly simple or complex in the east Australian population, it remained so after transmission to New Caledonia. The only clear exception to this was the Light Purple song type, in which the complexity scores diverged sharply due to a single complex theme unique to the east Australian song (Theme 6), which was consistently present in all song cycles. Overall, complexity scores were lower in New Caledonia for all song types compared to east Australia. Further examination of the song sequences revealed that this was likely due to five of six east Australian song types having more total units per song cycle than New Caledonia. Despite these fine-scale differences, the overall song type was clearly retained between the east Australian and New Caledonia populations (see Methods: Verification of theme and song classifications).
There were no significant differences in the complexity scores of individual themes between populations for any song types (Fig. 1, Mann–Whitney/Wilcoxon, p > 0.4 for all song types). In most themes that were shared between populations, complexity was consistent relative to the other themes. In other words, themes that were simple (i.e., low complexity) in the east Australian population were also simple in the New Caledonian population. While shared themes had a range of complexity levels, themes that were unique to a single population tended to have consistently low complexity. The exceptions to this were Theme 6 from the Light Purple song type, and Theme 28 from the Light Brown song type (Fig. 1). Each of these themes had high complexity levels and were found in evolutionary song types.
Discussion
Singers in New Caledonia learned the fine-scale unit arrangements of each song type with a high degree of fidelity. This demonstrates that song transmission from east Australia to New Caledonia is consistent at a fine-scale level of unit sequences as well as at the broad-scale level of theme sequences14. The high degrees of song similarity prevented analyses from distinguishing between populations, even with the occasional presence of themes unique to one population. High-fidelity copying was further supported by the comparable complexity levels for each song type and each of the shared themes between the populations. While east Australian song complexity was always higher, this difference was not significant and suggests that relative song complexity levels were largely maintained across song types. This was the case regardless of whether the song type itself was highly complex or not. Both the simplest song type (Purple) and the most complex song type (Light Brown) maintained similar complexity levels during transmission. The exception to these trends, found in the Light Purple song type, resulted from the presence of a common but single uniquely east Australian theme that had particularly high complexity (Fig. 1b), skewing the overall song type’s complexity level due to its consistent presence in all song cycles. Ultimately the lack of consistent differences in theme and unit arrangements demonstrates a lack of distinct “east Australian” or “New Caledonian” versions of each song type. Inter-population transmission therefore appears to be as robust as intra-population transmission, with similar degrees of within and between population variability. This strengthens the argument that song transmission is therefore likely to occur through close acoustic contact.
Themes that were shared by both populations contained units which covered the spectrum of acoustic features such as frequency, duration, and contour shape. Of note was the retention of sequences containing high frequency unit types (e.g., “whistles” or “squeaks”) during song transmission. Typically, high frequency signals do not propagate as far as low frequency signals and thus can be lost over distance33,34. Recent work by Girola, et al.34 also found that source levels (i.e., the sound level (dB re 1 μPa) at the acoustic source) in humpback whale song decrease with increasing frequency. The results found here highlight that the mechanism of song exchange between the populations allows for sufficient acoustic contact and high fidelity copying for all sequences to be learned (including those containing high frequency units). It further suggests that content-related factors such as novelty or learning ability might be more of a driving force in theme transmission as opposed to practical drivers such as an ability to hear certain elements of a theme.
The close acoustic contact could occur on the Antarctic feeding grounds21 or through the New Zealand migration corridor shared by both populations19,20. Recent work by Warren, et al.35 lends further strength to both of these as transmission mechanisms for these populations. Their detection of two song types on the Cook Strait migration corridor presents the possibility of song exchange with the required acoustic proximity suggested by our results. Additionally, song themes heard on east Australia’s southward migration were subsequently recorded in New Caledonia the following year, suggesting the additional exchange possibly of the shared Balleny Island feeding grounds. The limited individual exchange documented thus far between east Australia and New Caledonia18 suggests the movement of individuals either within or between seasons is a less likely mechanism for song transmission compared to feeding ground and/or migratory exchange, but cannot be ruled out. The one-year delay between the population is also not absolute, as Garland, et al.14 identified both populations singing the same song type at the same time in five separate years. When combined with the equivalent support for migratory and feeding ground transmission mechanisms, it is possible that there is flexibility in the transmission mechanisms used based on either ecological or environmental factors.
Themes that were unique to a specific population primarily occurred in evolving song types (Light Purple, Light Brown), which maintain part of their pattern from the previous year’s song. Incorporating larger sections of novel material (e.g., themes) might therefore only be attempted in songs with which singers are already at least partially familiar. Revolutionary song types are entirely novel material, which Allen, et al.26 suggest may be too demanding a learning task to allow for additional embellishments to be added at the time of learning. Given that the baseline learning requirements for an evolving song pattern is less or is built on prior learning, evolutions may either provide more opportunity to improvise/embellish or allow for copy error to occur more frequently as pattern complexity increases. The presence of population-specific themes suggests that these progressive song evolutions are indeed made through insertions and deletions at the theme level36, and spread among individuals within the population. Similar instances of population-specific themes have been previous identified across South Pacific populations, including both those in east Australia and New Caledonia37. These results support the proposed hypothesis that themes are the fundamental “building blocks” of song learning38,39. This corresponds with similar segmented learning mechanisms in other complex vocal communication systems such as bird song and human language. For example, some songbirds learn their song displays in smaller segments delineated by pauses40, while humans tend to use small components such as words or short phrases when learning language41. This further parallels other structural similarities between humpback whale song and both bird song and human communication, such as the underlying small-world structure found in all three taxa which is thought to facilitate learning42. A segmented learning mechanism may therefore be another key element to the cultural evolution and transmission of complex communication displays across taxa38,43.
More complex theme arrangements do not appear to be more difficult to learn, as both simple and complex shared themes retained their complexity levels during transmission with high levels of similarity (Figs. 1, S1–6). This corresponds with previous studies which have shown that more complex patterns do not contain more information and are likely to be embellishments or artifacts of the song learning process26. Such novel variations are thought to be driven by the changes of individual singers44, which are then learned by all singers through song conformity. However, many of the unit-level population differences in shared themes were substitutions of unit types with similar acoustic properties. Additionally, east Australia’s higher song-level complexity appears to be due to an increase in the total number of units present in the song rather than the number of unique units present. Rather than meaningful changes, it is more likely that these embellishments may be copy errors, flexibility in unit repetition, or unit types that singers may use interchangeably. Instead, a key source of novel material seems to be through the insertion of new themes, further supporting segmented learning.
These novel themes were disproportionately found in the New Caledonian population, suggesting that changes or embellishments of the song pattern were more common there. If complexity and novelty are indeed indications of learning capacity, as suggested in both songbirds32,45,46 and humpback whales12,26, then both maintaining complexity and adding novel material in a song arrangement are arguably more important in a breeding ground such as New Caledonia compared to the east Australian migratory corridor. More plausibly, the east Australian population may add their own novel themes when learning the song from west Australia. The New Caledonian population then learns the song with high fidelity (including most of east Australia’s added themes), and then attempts to incorporate their own novel themes as the song continues to evolve. If this is the case, then adding novel content in the form of these themes occurs during or after acquiring the new song. To test this, a fine-scale comparison of inter-population song transmission similar to those performed in this study should be conducted between the west and east Australian populations. More broadly, similar studies in other populations with the potential for acoustic contact (such as the Brazilian and Southwest African populations of the South Atlantic47) should be conducted to determine if the learning mechanisms identified here are species-wide.
Despite the prevalence of unique themes to New Caledonia, these unique themes tended to be simple while those unique to east Australia were often more complex. The much larger population in east Australia (~ 25,00048) has more singers and therefore may provide more sources for novelty for any given singer44. Conversely, New Caledonia (~ 120049) has a smaller pool of individuals from which novelty may be drawn, as well as fewer individuals to spread the song among the population. Therefore, while novelty and embellishment could be more important in the breeding stock of New Caledonia, the small population size may restrict the complexity of the novel material that can be successfully introduced and incorporated into the population-wide song. This could also explain why east Australian songs as a whole tended to be more complex than New Caledonia, albeit not significantly. The role of these disparate population sizes is already hypothesized to drive the consistently eastward transmission of these songs across the South Pacific22,50, as the west Australian population is the largest and eastward populations get subsequently smaller51. Population size has further been shown to influence song learning in other species as well, such as the critically endangered honeyeater whose population decline is linked with a stark loss of vocal culture52.
Conclusions
The consistent, directional song transmission between the east Australian and New Caledonian populations provides a unique opportunity to examine the product of song learning at an inter-population level. These populations are part of a broader case of horizontal cultural transmission on a large scale, the only example that has been documented in a non-human species14. We have quantitatively shown consistency in fine-scale structural features across populations to complement broad-scale analyses14,22. Complexity within these song types remained consistent during transmission, suggesting that constraints on song learning may not impact inter-population transmission. Although learning cannot be directly observed, song patterns provide indirect evidence regarding the mechanisms of song exchange in use. The results found here suggest a segmented learning mechanism with close acoustic contact (such as shared feeding grounds or migration routes), allowing for high fidelity copying of complex song arrangements between populations. Given that cultural exchange between populations is rarely documented in species other than humans, humpback whale song provides a model by which the evolution of cultural transmission in both animals and humans can be further understood.
Methods
Song collection and transcription
Data were collected from two sites (Fig. 3): Peregian Beach on the coast of southeast Queensland, Australia (26°30’ S, 153°05’ E), and the southern lagoon of New Caledonia (22°28’ S, 166°56’ E). Passive acoustic recordings in east Australia were collected in 2009–2014 using two autonomous loggers (Acousonde 3A with external battery housings, Greenridge Sciences) and a fixed five-buoy hydrophone array (detailed in Allen, et al.27). New Caledonian recordings were made in the Southern Lagoon using boat-based hydrophones (2010–2013, HiTech) and Zoom recorder (16 bit, sampling rate of 44 kHz, WAV files), and a single passive acoustic recorder (2014–2015, SM2M + Whalesong recorder, Wildlife Acoustics with a sampling rate of 22 kHz). The years selected for analyses were based on the availability of high-quality recordings and resources for the subsequent transcription of recordings. All data were collected with approval from the University of Queensland Research and Innovation Animal Ethics (certificate approval numbers by year—2009: SVS/299/08/ACAMMS, 2010–2012: SVS/230/10/(NF), 2012–2013: SVS/403/12/EPSML. 2013–2014: CURTIN/SVS/283/1, 2014–2015: SVS/103/14). Data collection protocols were in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations.
Spectrograms were generated in Raven Pro 1.6 (www.birds.cornell.edu/raven) and evaluated for quality following the methods outlined in Allen, et al.27. Song patterns were transcribed at the individual sound unit level using an acoustic dictionary developed from a subsample of measured units from each song type in each population (following Allen, et al.27). 353 complete song cycles (10–36 per year per population) were taken from 89 recordings, with each recording treated as a distinct singer following previous studies42,53. Each year song cycles were transcribed from a minimum of six individuals (excepting New Caledonia 2010, n = 4, due to a lack of high-quality recordings) which is considered to be sufficiently representative of the general population song pattern14. Although a small portion of singers in New Caledonia were recorded singing the 2013 song type in 2014, they were excluded from this study to focus on fine-scale comparisons between the same song pattern sung by the separate populations.
Verification of theme and song classifications
Qualitative theme assignments were quantitatively verified for each population separately using the Levenshtein distance similarity index (LSI) analysis26,28,54. The calculation was weighted (β = 1) based on the acoustic feature similarity of units26,27,55. The LSI values were calculated between every pair of phrases (each phrase is composed of a sequence of units). The songs from each population in each year were first evaluated separately to identify the themes present and assign these to a song type (Figs. S1–6). The same song types from each population were then grouped together for comparison and assigned a colour name following previous work on these populations14 (Table 1). While transitional themes were eliminated due to their rarity and lack of stereotypy, all themes with at least two phrase repetitions in a song cycle were included.
This allowed themes to be assigned to each song type irrespective of population. Once themes had been verified, an unweighted LSI analysis was applied to the theme sequences of each song cycle to validate the qualitative song types (N = 10–36 song cycles per year per population, total N = 353 song cycles, Figs. S7 and 8). Theme sequences were used to account for the skewing that tends to occur in LSI analyses based on length55, following similar studies14,37,56. To identify population-level differences within each song type, separate similarity analyses were also conducted between every pair of song cycles per song type (Purple: N = 46, Light Purple: N = 54, Brown: N = 61, Light Brown: N = 48, Teal: N = 72, Orange: N = 72, Figs. S7 and 8. The inclusion of all song cycles ensured that both intra- and inter-individual variability could be accounted for26. The LSI matrix was clustered using average-linkage hierarchical clustering and bootstrapping using pvclust and hclust packages in R57 and custom written code (available at https://github.com/ellengarland/leven). Resulting dendrograms were assessed using the cophenetic correlation coefficient (CCC) to determine how well they represented associations within the data, with CCC > 0.8 indicating good representation58.
Based on their respective patterns, LSI grouped the song types (Purple, Light Purple, Brown, Light Brown, Teal, Orange) onto separate major dendrogram branches, and were clearly distinguished from one another (Figs. S7 and 8). On each major branch (i.e., each song type), the LSI further grouped each ‘song cycle’ (i.e., one set of specific themes sung in a stereotyped order13,23) regardless of the recording’s population of origin. Shared themes, which occurred in both populations, predominantly clustered together on the same branch (Figs. S1–6). This demonstrates that individual themes consistently retained their unit sequence patterns across populations in an accurate and recognisable way, providing quantitative evidence to confirm previous qualitative work14. Dendrograms for all song types had a cophenetic correlation coefficient (CCC) score of > 0.9, indicating a good representation of the associations in the data.
Complexity scores
Song complexity was evaluated using ‘complexity scores’ generated for each year’s song type following the methods presented in Allen, et al.26, modified from Boogert, et al.32 and Templeton, et al.31. Complexity scores were generated by reducing positively correlated variables to a single principal component using principal component analysis (PCA) with the princomp function in R (Version 4.0.559). Song level variables represented the full sequence of units in the song cycle, including all phrase repetitions, using the following variables: (1) number of units per song cycle, (2) number of unit types per song cycle, and (3) duration of each song cycle (s). Theme level variables accounted for the presence of separate themes using the following variables: (1) number of themes per song cycle, (2) mean phrase duration per song cycle (s), and (3) mean individual theme complexity score per song cycle (calculated following Allen, et al.26). For each song type, these six variables were combined and condensed into a single composite score by the PCA, with this score represented by the first principal component score value. Scores have a direct, positive relationship with complexity: higher scores indicate higher relative complexity within the song’s pattern. To further evaluate population-level differences, a set of complexity scores was also calculated for each individual theme per population. Differences in separate theme complexities between the two populations were then evaluated using a non-parametric Mann–Whitney/Wilcoxon test. Statistical analyses were run in R (Version 4.0.559).
Data availability
All datasets generated during and/or analysed during this study are available on the Dryad Digital Repository [doi: https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.9p8cz8wk1]. Please seek prior written permission from JAA to reuse data in any form except to confirm the results of this study.
References
Rendell, L. & Whitehead, H. Culture in whales and dolphins. Behav. Brain Sci. 24, 309–324 (2001).
Krützen, M. et al. Cultural transmission of tool use in bottlenose dolphins. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 102, 8939–8943 (2005).
Kawai, M. Newly-acquired pre-cultural behavior of the natural troop of Japanese monkeys on Koshima Islet. Primates 6, 1–30 (1965).
Slater, P. The cultural transmission of bird song. Trends Ecol. Evol. 1, 94–97 (1986).
Whitehead, H. & Rendell, L. The cultural lives of whales and dolphins. (University of Chicago Press, 2014).
Whiten, A. The identification and differentiation of culture in chimpanzees and other animals: from natural history to diffusion experiments. The question of animal culture, 99–124 (2009).
Allen, J. A. Community through culture: from insects to whales: How social learning and culture manifest across diverse animal communities. BioEssays 41, 1900060 (2019).
Allen, J., Weinrich, M., Hoppitt, W. & Rendell, L. Network-based diffusion analysis reveals cultural transmission of lobtail feeding in humpback whales. Science 340, 485–488 (2013).
Baker, C. Migratory movement and population structure of humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae) in the central and eastern North Pacific. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 31, 105–119 (1986).
Garrigue, C. et al. Movement of individual humpback whales between wintering grounds of Oceania (South Pacific), 1999 to 2004. J. Cetacean Res. Manage 3, 275–281 (2011).
Rosenbaum, H. C. et al. First circumglobal assessment of Southern Hemisphere humpback whale mitochondrial genetic variation and implications for management. Endang. Spec. Res. 32, 551–567 (2017).
Noad, M. J., Cato, D. H., Bryden, M. M., Jenner, M. N. & Jenner, K. C. Cultural revolution in whale songs. Nature 408, 537. https://doi.org/10.1038/35046199 (2000).
Payne, R. S. & McVay, S. Songs of humpback whales. Science 173, 585–597 (1971).
Garland, E. C. et al. Dynamic horizontal cultural transmission of humpback whale song at the ocean basin scale. Curr. Biol. 21, 687–691. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2011.03.019 (2011).
Payne, R. & Guinee, L. N. Humpback whale (Megaptera novaeangliae) songs as an indicator of “stocks”. Communication and behavior of whales, 333–358 (1983).
Garrigue, C. et al. Movements of humpback whales in Oceania, South Pacific. J. Cetac. Res. Manage. 4, 255–260 (2002).
Derville, S., Torres, L. G., Zerbini, A. N., Oremus, M. & Garrigue, C. Horizontal and vertical movements of humpback whales inform the use of critical pelagic habitats in the western South Pacific. Sci. Rep. 10, 1–13 (2020).
Garrigue, C. et al. First assessment of interchange of humpback whales between Oceania and the East coast of Australia. J. Cetac. Res. Manage. 3, 269–274 (2011).
Steel, D. et al. Migratory connections between humpback whales from South Pacific breeding grounds and Antarctic feeding areas based on genotype matching. Int. Whal. Comm. (2008).
Constantine, R., Russell, K., Gibbs, N., Childerhouse, S. & Baker, C. S. Photo-identification of humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae) in New Zealand waters and their migratory connections to breeding grounds of Oceania. Mar. Mam. Sci. 23, 715–720 (2007).
Garland, E. C. et al. Humpback whale song on the southern ocean feeding grounds: implications for cultural transmission. PLoS ONE 8, e79422 (2013).
Garland, E. C. et al. Population structure of humpback whales in the western and central South Pacific Ocean as determined by vocal exchange among populations. Conserv. Biol. 29, 1198–1207 (2015).
Cholewiak, D. M., Sousa-Lima, R. S. & Cerchio, S. Humpback whale song hierarchical structure: Historical context and discussion of current classification issues. Mar. Mam. Sci. 29, E312–E332. https://doi.org/10.1111/mms.12005 (2013).
Payne, K., Tyack, P. & Payne, R. Progressive changes in the songs of humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae): a detailed analysis of two seasons in Hawaii. Communication and behavior of whales, 9–57 (1983).
Payne, K. & Payne, R. Large scale changes over 19 years in songs of humpback whales in Bermuda. Ethology 68, 89–114 (1985).
Allen, J. A., Garland, E. C., Dunlop, R. A. & Noad, M. J. Cultural revolutions reduce complexity in the songs of humpback whales. Proc. R. Soc. B: Biol. Sci. 285, 20182088. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2018.2088 (2018).
Allen, J. A., Garland, E. C., Murray, A., Noad, M. J. & Dunlop, R. Using self-organizing maps to classify humpback whale song units and quantify their similarity. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 142, 1943–1952 (2017).
Murray, A., Dunlop, R. A., Noad, M. J. & Goldizen, A. W. Stereotypic and complex phrase types provide structural evidence for a multi-message display in humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae). J Acoust Soc Am. 143, 980–994 (2018).
Garland, E. C. et al. Redefining western and central South Pacific humpback whale population structure based on vocal cultural exchange. (2013).
Rekdahl, M. Humpback whale vocal communication: Use and stability of social calls and revolutions in the songs of east Australian whales. (2012).
Templeton, C. N., Laland, K. N. & Boogert, N. J. Does song complexity correlate with problem-solving performance in flocks of zebra finches?. Anim. Behav. 92, 63–71 (2014).
Boogert, N. J., Giraldeau, L.-A. & Lefebvre, L. Song complexity correlates with learning ability in zebra finch males. Anim. Behav. 76, 1735–1741 (2008).
Winn, H. & Winn, L. The song of the humpback whale Megaptera novaeangliae in the West Indies. Mar. Biol. 47, 97–114 (1978).
Girola, E., Noad, M. J., Dunlop, R. A. & Cato, D. H. Source levels of humpback whales decrease with frequency suggesting an air-filled resonator is used in sound production. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America (In Review).
Warren, V. E., Constantine, R., Noad, M., Garrigue, C. & Garland, E. C. Migratory insights from singing humpback whales recorded around central New Zealand. R. Soc. Open Sci. 7, 201084 (2020).
Garland, E. C. et al. Quantifying humpback whale song sequences to understand the dynamics of song exchange at the ocean basin scale. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 133, 560–569. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.4770232 (2013).
Owen, C. et al. Migratory convergence facilitates cultural transmission of humpback whale song. R. Soc. Open Sci. 6, 190337 (2019).
Garland, E. C., Rendell, L., Lamoni, L., Poole, M. M. & Noad, M. Song hybridization events during revolutionary song change provide insights into cultural transmission in humpback whales. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 114, 7822–7829 (2017).
Noad, M. & Cato, D. A combined acoustic and visual survey of humpback whales off southeast Queensland. Mem. Qld. Mus. 47, 507–523 (2001).
Spierings, M., de Weger, A. & Ten Cate, C. Pauses enhance chunk recognition in song element strings by zebra finches. Anim. Cogn. 18, 867–874 (2015).
Doupe, A. J. & Kuhl, P. K. Birdsong and human speech: common themes and mechanisms. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 22, 567–631 (1999).
Allen, J. A., Garland, E. C., Dunlop, R. A. & Noad, M. J. Network analysis reveals underlying syntactic features in a vocally learnt mammalian display, humpback whale song. Proc. R. Soc. B 286, 20192014 (2019).
Barón Birchenall, L. Animal communication and human language: An overview. International Journal of Comparative Psychology 29 (2016).
Noad, M. J. The use of song by humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae) during migration off the east coast of Australia (doctoral dissertation) Doctor of Philosophy thesis, University of Sydney, (2002).
Catchpole, C. Song and female choice: good genes and big brains?. Trends Ecol. Evol. 11, 358–360. https://doi.org/10.1016/0169-5347(96)30042-6 (1996).
Nowicki, S., Hasselquist, D., Bensch, S. & Peters, S. Nestling growth and song repertoire size in great reed warblers: evidence for song learning as an indicator mechanism in mate choice. Proc. R. Soc. London B: Biol. Sci. 267, 2419–2424 (2000).
NOAA. Vol. 81 (ed National Marine Fisheries Service) 62260–62320 (Department of Commerce, Federal Register, 2016).
Noad, M. J., Kniest, E. & Dunlop, R. A. Boom to bust? Implications for the continued rapid growth of the eastern Australian humpback whale population despite recovery. Popul. Ecol. 61(2), 198–209 (2019).
Garrigue, C., Albertson, R. & Jackson, J. A. An anomalous increase in the New Caledonia humpback whales breeding sub-stock E2. Scientific Committee of the International Whaling Commission, Paper (2012).
Garland, E. C. & McGregor, P. K. Cultural transmission, evolution, and revolution in vocal displays: Insights from bird and whale song. Front. Psychol. 11, 2387 (2020).
Zandberg, L., Lachlan, R. F., Lamoni, L. & Garland, E. C. Global cultural evolutionary model of humpback whale song. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 376, 20200242 (2021).
Crates, R. et al. Loss of vocal culture and fitness costs in a critically endangered songbird. Proc. R. Soc. B 288, 20210225 (2021).
Garland, E. C., Garrigue, C. & Noad, M. J. When does cultural evolution become cumulative culture? A case study of humpback whale song. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 377, 20200313 (2022).
Garland, E. C. et al. Improved versions of the Levenshtein distance method for comparing sequence information in animals’ vocalisations: tests using humpback whale song. Behaviour 149, 1413–1441 (2012).
Garland, E. C. et al. The devil is in the detail: quantifying vocal variation in a complex, multileveled, and rapidly evolving display. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 142, 460–472 (2017).
Rekdahl, M. L. et al. Culturally transmitted song exchange between humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae) in the southeast Atlantic and southwest Indian Ocean basins. R. Soc. Open Sci. 5, 172305 (2018).
Suzuki, R. & Shimodaira, H. Pvclust: an R package for assessing the uncertainty in hierarchical clustering. Bioinformatics 22, 1540–1542 (2006).
Sokal, R. R. & Rohlf, F. J. The comparison of dendrograms by objective methods. Taxon 11(2), 33–40 (1962).
R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. 2015. URL http (R Core Development Team, 2016).
Acknowledgements
We would like to acknowledge the Quandamooka People on who’s country this research was conducted. We thank the staff and volunteers of the Cetacean Ecology Group for data collection, maintenance, and storage over the course of the study. East Australian data were collected during the HARC project (2009) and the BRAHSS project (2010–2012 and 2014). Thanks to Douglas Cato for his significant role in instigating and participating in all these field programmes. New Caledonian data were collected through Opération Cétacés with the assistance of the Province Sud Gardes Nature. We are grateful to Patrice Plichon, Veronique Pérard, and Remi Dodemont for their roles in this field data collection. We thank Adam Pack and Christopher Templeton for providing comments on a previous version of the manuscript. The American Australian Association and the Winifred Violet Scott Trust provided additional funding to J.A.A. for this study. E.C.G. is funded by a Royal Society University Research Fellowship (Grant No. UF160081).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
J.A.A., E.C.G., and M.J.N. developed the research concepts; R.A.D. and M.J.N. provided extensive raw east Australian song data; C.G. provided extensive raw New Caledonian song data (2010–2013); J.A.A., M.J.N., and C.G. collected raw New Caledonian song data (2014–2015); J.A.A. transcribed all song recordings, conducted all data analysis, and led final manuscript writing; E.C.G., M.J.N., C.G., and R.A.D. reviewed and edited the final manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Allen, J.A., Garland, E.C., Garrigue, C. et al. Song complexity is maintained during inter-population cultural transmission of humpback whale songs. Sci Rep 12, 8999 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-12784-3
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-12784-3
This article is cited by
-
Principal component analysis
Nature Reviews Methods Primers (2022)
Comments
By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.