Abstract
Endangered animals in captivity may display reduced brain sizes due to captive conditions and limited genetic diversity. Captive diets, for example, may differ in nutrition and texture, altering cranial musculature and alleviating constraints on cranial shape development. Changes in brain size are associated with biological fitness, which may limit reintroduction success. Little is known about how changes in brain size progress in highly managed carnivoran populations and whether such traits are retained among reintroduced populations. Here, we measured the endocranial volume of preserved Mexican wolf skulls across captive generations and between captive, wild, and reintroduced populations and assessed endocranial volume dependence on inbreeding and cranial musculature. Endocranial volume increased across captive generations. However, we did not detect a difference among captive, wild, and reintroduced groups, perhaps due to the variability across captive generations. We did not find a relationship between endocranial volume and either inbreeding or cranial musculature, although the captive population displayed an increase in the cross-sectional area of the masseter muscle. We hypothesize that the increase in endocranial volume observed across captive generations may be related to the high-quality nutrition provided in captivity.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
The relative endocranial volume of the vertebrate skull varies within and between populations of a species1,2,3. Given its intricate relationship with brain size and structure, endocranial volume has been linked to differences in behavior, performance, and fitness2,4. Changes in endocranial volume may have significant consequences for the viability of a population1,5,6. Within captive populations, endocranial volumes have been documented to increase [e.g.,7], decrease [e.g.,7,8,9], and in some cases show no discernable difference [e.g.,10,11]. Most frequently, the reported changes occurring in captivity have been related to a reduction in endocranial volume [e.g.,7,8,9], which has been attributed to improper diets, socially and environmentally depauperate enclosures, or lack of stimulating enrichment activities5,12,13. The nature of this relationship has been poorly documented in carnivoran species, particularly those that are intensively managed and bred for reintroduction initiatives.
Measurements of endocranial volume provide a useful and widely used proxy for brain size14,15. These measurements are often correlated with social behaviors and other traits related to learning, memory, problem-solving, and behavioral flexibility1,4,16 and may also influence traits directly related to fitness, including longevity and fecundity6,8. Changes in endocranial volume are not necessarily proportional across all brain structures16,17. For example, domestic dogs show disproportionate decreases to the neocortex and olfactory bulb associated with a smaller endocranial volume (roughly 30%) compared to their wild counterparts [18; although see19]. Disproportionate changes in endocranial volume may significantly influence cognition and important sensory functions16,20.
Within wild vertebrate populations, environmental variables have been linked to intraspecific variation in brain size, including seasonality, environmental severity, habitat complexity, and urbanization3,15,21. However, brain tissue is metabolically expensive to maintain; therefore, intraspecific increases in brain size are primarily thought to occur under intense selection pressure or when ample resources are available, which can enable increased development without risking other aspects of fitness3,5,8. Resource limitations are thought to constrain brain size in wild populations3,8. However, in managed populations, where ample nutrient-rich diets may be readily and consistently available, and where energetically expensive activities are eliminated (e.g. foraging costs or predator avoidance), enhanced endocranial development may be achievable. However, the relative impact of changes to endocranial volume occurring in captivity has yet to be explored in populations that have been reintroduced to wild conditions.
Previous studies have demonstrated a change in brain volume occurring in captivity, including a decrease in brain volume among captive fish8,12,22, waterfowl23, and a variety of mammalian taxa including rodents16,24,25, artiodactyls26,27, perissodactyls18,28,29 and carnivorans7,30, as well as an increase in brain volume associated with captive Asiatic lions (although the opposite trend was documented among sub-Saharan lions in this study)7. Such changes have been attributed to small population sizes (e.g., inbreeding depression and/or genetic drift), pleiotropy associated with breeding for docility, and relaxed selection, where maintaining a large relative brain size may be quickly lost in the absence of selection pressures in natural environments8,17,31. While brain size is generally heritable6,15, plastic changes related to captive husbandry may arise within a single captive generation8,12,14.
The shape of bone structures is constrained, at least partially, by the musculature of a given anatomical region32,33. For example, braincase morphology is determined in part by cranial musculature involved in food intake and vocalization33,34. This influence of muscle tissue on underlying bone structures may have a considerable impact on the phenotypic evolution of dietary specialists. In particular, large hypercarnivorous mammals (whose diet is composed of at least 70% vertebrate prey)35,36 evolved enhanced cranial musculature, which enables them to restrain and kill their prey and process mechanically tough dietary items33,34 alongside distinct skull morphologies37,38,39. However, captive carnivores frequently consume a comparatively soft diet compared to what is available in the wild40,41, which may lead to changes in skull shape and functionality42,43,44 and may potentially alleviate constraints on internal cranial structures as a byproduct. Although the discussion of a captive diet often focuses on nutritional quality, nutritional quality may not be sufficient to ensure morphological change does not occur in captive individuals40,42,45. Likewise, the development of brain tissue is facilitated by high-nutrient diets, where malnourished animals may experience poor brain development and decreased endocranial volume46,47. The differential nutritional quality available in captivity may therefore lead to differences in brain development.
The diets of captive Mexican wolves (Canis lupus baileyi), for example, may vary between facilities; however, husbandry manuals for the species suggest that 90–95% of their captive diet should be composed of a high-grade meat-based dry domestic dog food (or a similar dry food formulated for exotic canids) and the remaining 5–10% of their diet may include supplemental meat for enrichment48,49; whereas, up to 93% of their wild diet is composed of elk (Cervus elaphus) and other large ungulates50,51. Although dry dog food diets are likely to accommodate the nutritional needs of the animals, the composition of such food is quite different from their wild prey, due in part to the large percentage of cereals other grains often found in dry dog foods52,53. This may lead to changes in cranial musculature and bite force, resulting in observable shifts in cranial morphology43,54. For example, when comparing captive and wild canid populations, hypercarnivorous species, including Mexican wolves, display a greater change in cranial morphology than less carnivorous species, potentially suggesting a shift in the cranial musculature of captive hypercarnivorous canids55.
Here, we investigated the relative endocranial volume of Mexican wolves across captive generations and among captive, wild, and reintroduced populations. Given the small founding size of their captive population (only 7 individuals) and the population bottleneck the species experienced prior to the formation of the captive population56,57 as well as the unique conditions provided in captivity (e.g., enclosures, nutrition, enrichment)8,15,46,47, we expected to see a cumulative change in endocranial volume across captive generations. In particular, we expected to find a strong relationship between inbreeding coefficients associated with the captive population and a change in brain size. Likewise, given the difference in diet texture provided in captivity, we expected to find a change in cranial musculature between captive, wild, and reintroduced wolves and across captive generations. By quantifying the endocranial volume of a managed population and comparing it to wild and reintroduced populations, this study explores the long-term viability of captive-breeding and helps illuminate the intraspecific variation that may be available to populations that are released from resource scarcity.
Methods
Mexican wolves (Canis lupus baileyi) are a highly managed, endangered population57. These animals are endemic to Mexico and the southwestern United States, although they were hunted to near extinction by 197656,58. Mexican wolves were successfully bred within zoos and wildlife sanctuaries from a founding population of just 7 individuals56,57. By 1998, individuals from this captive-bred population were reintroduced to portions of its native range in Arizona and New Mexico56,57.
We assessed the endocranial volume of 53 captive, 22 wild, and 12 reintroduced Mexican wolves (Appendix A). We distinguished among these groups using the Mexican wolf studbook57 and museum records. Individuals from the historic wild population were collected between 1900 and 1950, prior to any reintroduction attempts (c. 1998). Captive individuals included only animals that spent the majority (at least 60%) of their first year in the captive environment (either a zoo or enclosed wildlife sanctuary). Reintroduced individuals included animals that had a captive-lineage but spent the majority of their first year (at least 60%) in the wild (as assessed from the studbook). Given that the brain and the cranium do not fully develop until seven months of age in canids59, we used exclusively adult specimens for all analyses. Maturity was established via the absence of cranial sutures and deciduous teeth and verified by the captive studbook when possible. To avoid improper estimates of endocranial volume, we only used specimens that were completely intact and had no discernable cranial damage.
To measure endocranial volume, we poured glass beads, 2 mm in diameter, into the foramen magnum of preserved specimens until the beads reached the occipital condyles of the skull, at which point, we gently tapped the skull several times to ensure that the cranial cavity was completely filled. This method is frequently used, easily accessible, and delivers comparable estimates of endocranial volume as measures derived from a cranial CT scan14. We used a funnel to remove the beads and recorded their weight in grams to the nearest one hundredth using an electronic balance. To ensure the closest possible measurement, this process was repeated three times for each skull. Based on the mean measure of the three estimates, we calculated a measurement error of less than 1%. We converted the mean mass estimate into volume by developing a calibration curve. Beads were measured into precise volumes of 0, 25, 75, 100, 150, and 200 mL and weighed (to the nearest one hundredth of a gram) resulting in a regression line (y = 0.6692x + 0.7927) used to estimate the volume of each endocranial skull measure.
We assessed the dataset for allometry associated with endocranial volume by regressing it against skull length, a proxy for body size in canids35,60 that is frequently used in analyses of brain-to-body size in mammals7,14. Each skull was photographed in the ventral view by the same individual to maintain consistent specimen orientation. The length of each skull, from the prosthion to the inion, was measured (in one hundredths of a millimeter) using TPSdig232 software61. Using a simple linear regression, we detected allometry (r2 = 0.15, F = 15.62, p = 1.6e−4; P-value significance: 0.01–0.05*, 0.001–0.01**, 0–0.001***). In an effort to minimize the influence of allometry, we extracted the regression residual for each specimen in the dataset and used these values in the remaining analyses.
Given that captive Mexican wolves are known to display a significant difference in cranial length and width when compared to wild populations62, we assessed allometrically corrected residuals of endocranial volume (hereafter relative endocranial volume) relative to the skull centroid size, which accounts for both skull length and width. To calculate the centroid size, we photographed the ventral view of the cranium for each specimen and applied 36 homologous landmarks that capture the entire length and width of the skull (Figure S1) using TPSdig23261. Based on this landmark scheme, the centroid size of each cranium was calculated as the square root of the sum of squared differences between each point and the geometric center (centroid) of the landmarks63. To test whether there are differences in relative endocranial volume among captive, wild, and reintroduced populations, we performed ANOVAs and Tukey’s Honest Significant Difference (HSD) to evaluate pairwise comparisons.
All reintroduced individuals and most captive individuals in the dataset were associated with a recorded studbook number, which allowed us to extract data regarding the specimen’s age, housing history, and lineage (Appendix A). To assess whether relative endocranial volume had any age-related effects, we extracted the age of each captive specimen (in days) and performed a simple linear regression of age to relative endocranial volume. There was no clear statistical relationship between ages and relative endocranial volume within our sample (r2 = − 0.01, F = 0.15, p > 0.05), therefore, all specimens (seven months and older) were pooled for the remaining analyses. Using a Welch two sample t-test of male and female specimens, we did not detect any statistically clear sexual dimorphism associated with the relative endocranial volume (t = 1.48, p > 0.05), similar to other canid species [19; although see59]; therefore, the sexes were pooled in all remaining analyses. Using the pedigree package64, we calculated the inbreeding coefficients (F) associated with captive individuals from the data provided in the studbook. We used the inbreeding coefficients as independent variables in simple linear regressions to explore the relative impact of inbreeding upon endocranial volume among captive individuals.
For each captive specimen, we also estimated the number of captive generations (generations removed from the wild) in their family tree. We calculated captive generations by adding one to the mean value of the generations experienced by both the sire and the dam65. In total, our dataset included individuals from 1.5 to 6.6 captive generations (Appendix A). We analyzed the statistical power of our sample size (within captive generations) using leave-one-out rarefaction. During 1,000 iterations of the model, stabilization was achieved in both the slope and R2 at a sample of size of 10, suggesting that our sample should be robust enough to detect any possible signal within our data. To evaluate changes in relative endocranial volume across captive generations, we performed a simple linear regression. We calculated the relative difference between the wild population and each individual captive generation using an ANOVA and post-hoc Tukey’s HSD. Captive generations were rounded to whole numbers (including generations 2, 3, 4, 5, and 6) for all ANOVA analyses, therefore the ANOVA analyses do not include captive generation 1. Whereas captive generations remained continuous (from captive generation 1.5 to captive generation 6.6) and were not rounded to whole numbers for linear regression analyses (Appendix A).
Given that the endocranium and the musculature of the skull are not independent of each other, we calculated the bite force of each specimen using Thomason’s dry method (Figure S2), which estimates skull musculature relative to body mass35,66. Measurements of the cross-sectional areas, in-lever, and out-lever moment arms of the masseter and temporalis muscles were taken on photos of the dorsal, ventral, and lateral views of the cranium and lateral view of the mandible via Fiji67. Specimens that lacked any of these views (n = 2) were not included in the bite force analyses. We assessed the difference between the cross-sectional areas of the masseter and temporalis muscles as well as the overall bite forces of the wild, captive, and reintroduced specimens using an ANOVA and Tukey’s HSD. We also investigated the relative difference in the cross-sectional areas of the masseter and temporalis muscles and in the overall bite force across captive generations using a simple linear regression. Unless otherwise noted, we conducted all analyses in R version 3.6.368.
Results
The average absolute endocranial volume (+/− 1 standard deviation) of captive, wild, and reintroduced Mexican wolves was 134.12 mL (+/− 9.83 mL), 132.01 mL (+/− 9.39 mL), and 130.31 mL (± 8.49 mL), respectively (Table 1). Captive individuals displayed the greatest overall endocranial volume, which was 1.59% greater than the wild wolves and 2.83% greater than the reintroduced wolves. We did not detect a clear statistical difference in skull length (F = 1.46; df = 2; p = 0.24; Table 1) or endocranial volume (F = 0.95; df = 2; p = 0.39) among captive, wild, and reintroduced groups. Thus, we used a common allometric relationship associated with skull length and endocranial volume (r2 = 0.15, F = 15.62, p = 1.6e−4; Figure S3) and conducted our analyses using the residual values from this regression. All results were similar regardless of whether the endocranial volumes were calculated relative to skull length or to skull centroid size (which also estimates size by skull length and width measures).
We observed a statistical difference in endocranial volumes across captive generations (r2 = 0.13, F = 6.9, p = 0.01*), where generations further removed from the wild population displayed higher mean values of endocranial volume relative to skull length (Fig. 1). This increase peaked at generation 5 (Fig. 1), which was 8.40% greater than the first captive generation recorded in our dataset (captive generation 2, where captive generations were rounded to whole numbers) and was 4.29% greater than the overall mean value for the captive population. Captive generation 2 displayed a smaller endocranial volume than any other captive generation included in this analysis and was 3.94% smaller than the overall mean value for the captive population and 2.30% smaller than the mean value for the wild population. The average length of the skull also decreased across captive generations, where the skull length of the first captive generation (21.17 mm ± 1.11 mm) was 4.40% longer than average length of captive generation 6 (20.23 mm ± 1.13), however; this trend did not result is a statistical difference (r2 = 0.04, F = 2.5, p = 0.12).
We detected a statistical difference between the captive generations and the reintroduced population (F = 2.48; DF = 5; p = 0.04*; Fig. 2). In particular, the endocranial volume of captive generation 5 was 6.84% greater than the reintroduced population; however, the Tukey HSD analysis did not detect a pairwise difference between any of the captive generations and the reintroduced group, including comparisons with the largest overall captive generation (captive generation 5; t = − 2.32; p = 0.20). We also detected a significant difference between the wild group and the captive generations (F = 2.95; DF = 5; p = 0.02*), where captive generation 5 was 5.96% greater than the overall wild population average. However, Tukey HSD analysis did not detect any pairwise differences between the captive generations and the wild group (Fig. 2).
We did not detect a difference in estimated bite force either across captive generations (r2 = 0.05, F = 3.75, p = 5.8e−2) or between the captive, wild, and reintroduced populations (F = 2.03, Df = 2, p = 0.14). Although the reintroduced population showed the weakest overall bite force, which was 8.34% weaker than the captive population and 11.18% weaker than the historic wild population. We detected a difference in the cross-sectional area of the masseter relative to the jaw length across captive generations (r2 = 0.08, F = 5.66, p = 0.02*), associated with a decrease over captive generations (Figure S4). We also detected a difference in the masseter cross-sectional area between the captive, wild, and reintroduced populations (F = 4.76, Df = 2, p = 0.01*). Tukey’s HSD detected a pairwise difference between the masseter cross-sectional area of captive and reintroduced specimens (t = − 2.82, 0.02*), where captive individuals displayed the greatest cross-sectional area overall (Figure S5). We did not detect a difference in the cross-sectional area of the temporalis muscle either across captive generations (r2 = − 2.8e−3, F = 0.86, p = 0.36) or between captive, wild, and reintroduced populations (F = 2.39, Df = 2, p = 0.10).
We did not detect a relationship between endocranial volume and bite force (r2 = − 0.01, F = 0.18, p = 0.67) nor did we detect a relationship between endocranial volume and the cross-sectional areas of either the masseter or the temporalis muscles (r2 = 3.9e−3, F = 1.32, p = 0.25 and r2 = − 3.6e−3, F = 0.71, p = 0.40, respectively). Similarly, we did not detect a relationship between the relative endocranial volume and the inbreeding coefficients of the captive population (r2 = − 0.01, F = 0.71, p = 0.41).
Discussion
In direct opposition to the expected trend, we show an increase in the endocranial volume of Mexican wolves across captive generations. Given the results of previous studies regarding captive-bred animals, which have typically either reported a decrease in endocranial volume in captivity [e.g., 7,12,23] or no discernable difference in captive populations10,69, we expected to find similar results for captive Mexican wolves. This trend toward an increasing endocranial volume may suggest that the relationship between captivity and brain size is more complicated and less consistent than previously thought and will require a more nuanced approach in future analyses.
Species that have undergone significant population bottlenecks may be particularly prone to a reduction in endocranial volume12,31. Given that the captive population of Mexican wolves was founded with just seven animals56,57, we anticipated a correlation between relative endocranial volume and inbreeding; however, we detected no such relationship. It is important to note that inbreeding coefficients calculated exclusively from studbook data may underestimate the full extent of trends as they cannot account for inbreeding that occurred prior to captivity70. However, the inbreeding coefficients calculated here have been previously validated against inbreeding estimates calculated from genetic data62,71. Regardless, an association with inbreeding seems unlikely in this case given the increase in endocranial volume across captive generations. Notably, however, the first captive generation in our dataset displayed the smallest overall endocranial volume, which was not only smaller than any other captive generation, it was also smaller than both the wild and reintroduced population means. This may reinforce the idea that the founding captive population experienced poor overall health due to the bottleneck they experience prior to forming the captive population.
We did not detect a clear difference in bite forces either among captive, wild, and reintroduced Mexican wolves or across captive generations, although we did detect a difference associated with the cross-sectional area of the masseter muscle, which suggests that the masseter may be stronger within the captive population. This change may be unsurprising given that the primary attachment site for the masseter muscle occurs from the mandibular masseteric fossa to the zygomatic arches72 and captive Mexican wolves display comparatively wide skulls62, providing additional space for muscle attachment. However, if the trend toward increasing endocranial volume across captive generations was related to the constraints of cranial musculature upon cranial shape, as hypothesized here, we might expect a weaker temporalis muscle associated with the captive population given that the primary attachment site of the temporalis occurs on the braincase72. The fact that we did not detect a difference in the temporalis muscle either among captive, wild, and reintroduced groups or across captive generations suggests that either the temporalis muscle has not changed in conjunction with cranial morphology or that the measurement technique used here may be too coarse to detect the changes. We encourage future studies to explore this relationship more closely using soft tissue analyses.
The literature exploring shifts in endocranial volume of captive animals has mostly focused on fish [e.g.,8,12,22] or non-carnivorous mammals [e.g.,10,69]. These studies use animals reared in captive situations where animal husbandry (e.g., enrichment) may be a lower priority, such as lab-reared species [e.g.,24,25], or those with a captive history that extends to a time prior to the establishment of strict husbandry protocols [e.g.,7,30]. However, captive husbandry has improved significantly over the past century with the oversight of accrediting organizations, which monitor aspects of animal welfare including nutrition, enrichment, and enclosure design73. The more recent captive history represented by the Mexican wolves in this study (which only date back to 1975) may suggest that modern efforts to improve animal welfare may have led to an increase in brain size. Even by modern standards, captive Mexican wolves may receive above average care given their status as a remnant population of an endangered species bred specifically for conservation purposes.
Captive management may have a strong effect on endocranial volume. The development of brain tissue, for example, is facilitated by high-nutrient diets, where malnourished animals may experience poor brain development47,74. While high-quality diets are generally provided on a daily basis in captivity, the availability of consistently high-quality diets may be difficult to come by in the wild. The differential nutrition available in captivity may therefore suggest that captive animals should be expected to display an increase in endocranial volume compared to their wild counterparts, particularly among more recently established captive populations. For example, Hecht and colleagues (2021)75 found that regardless of whether they were bred for aggression or docility, silver foxes (Vulpes vulpes) maintained in a provisioned captive environment developed larger brain sizes than their farm-bred counterparts.
Other aspects of captive management, including enclosure design and the availability of enrichment, have been linked to changes in brain size22,40. For example, rats maintained in enriched enclosures display significantly larger brain sizes than those without enrichment76,77. Similar findings have also been associated with a lack of appropriate social stimulation8,78. Given that they were bred to initiate a reintroduction program, captive Mexican wolves may have avoided some of these drawbacks as special care was likely taken to breed healthy animals that maintained species-appropriate behaviors. Future studies should explore the nature of these trends in terms of captive husbandry and management, particularly nutrition, enclosure design, and enrichment.
Although we detected a significant increase in endocranial volume across captive generations, we did not detect a significant difference between captive, wild, and reintroduced groups. This may be related to the variation in endocranial volume associated with the captive population. Although captive generation five displayed the largest endocranial volume overall, the first captive generation in our dataset displayed a lower mean endocranial volume than either the wild or reintroduced groups, potentially obscuring some of the trends in the data. The endocranial volume of captive generation five was 6.84% larger than the reintroduced population and 5.96% larger than the wild population, suggesting that the peak endocranial volume achieved in captivity may have been quickly lost after the animals were reintroduced to their wild habitat.
Our results suggest an unexpected trend in the endocranial volume of captive Mexican wolves in which brain size increases over successive captive generations. Given that this increase appears to be absent from reintroduced populations of these wolves, these trends are likely to be related to the enhanced provisions accessible in captivity including consistently high-quality nutrition. These results suggest that a loss of brain volume in captivity is no longer a clear expectation and that with proper captive management and careful husbandry decisions, brain volumes may be maintained or actually increase. Intensive captive management of endangered species is common and may be fraught with a variety of unintended negative consequences; however, the results of this study suggest that a loss of brain volume need not be a foregone conclusion.
Data availability
All data analyzed during this study are included in this published article (and its appendix file).
References
Sol, D., Bacher, S., Reader, S. M. & Lefebvre, L. Brain size predicts the success of mammal species introduced into novel environments. Am. Nat. 172(Suppl. 1), S63–S71 (2008).
González-Lagos, C., Sol, D. & Reader, S. M. Large-brained mammals live longer. J. Evol. Biol. 23, 1064–1074 (2010).
Gonda, A., Herczeg, G. & Merilä, J. Evolutionary ecology of intraspecific brain size variation: A review. Ecol. Evol. 3(8), 2751–2764 (2013).
Benson-Amram, S., Dantzer, B., Stricker, G., Swanson, E. M. & Holekamp, K. E. Brain size predicts problem-solving ability in mammalian carnivores. PNAS 113(9), 2532–2537 (2016).
Näslund, J., Aarestrup, K., Thomassen, S. T. & Johnsson, J. I. Early enrichment effects on brain development in hatchery-reared Atlantic salmon (Salmo salar): No evidence for a critical period. Can. J. Fish. Aquat. Sci. 69(9), 1481–1490 (2012).
Logan, C. J., Kruuk, L. E. B., Stanley, R., Thompson, A. M. & Clutton-Brock, T. H. Endocranial volume is heritable and is associated with longevity and fitness in a wild mammal. R. Soc. Open Sci. 3(12), 160622 (2016).
Yamaguchi, N., Kitchener, A. C., Gilissen, E. & MacDonald, D. W. Brain size of the lion (Panthera leo) and the tiger (P. tigris): Implications for intrageneric phylogeny, intraspecific differences and the effects of captivity. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 98, 85–93 (2009).
Turschwell, M. P. & White, C. R. The effects of laboratory housing and spatial enrichment on brain size and metabolic rate in the eastern mosquitofish Gambusia holbrooki. Biol. Open. 5(3), 205–210 (2016).
Welniak-Kaminska, M. et al. Volumes of brain structures in captive wild-type and laboratory rats: 7T magnetic resonance in vivo automatic atlas-based study. PLoS ONE 14(4), e0215348 (2019).
Guay, P. J., Parrott, M. & Selwood, L. Captive breeding does not alter brain volume in a marsupial over a few generations. Zoo Biol. 31, 82–86 (2012).
Isler, K. et al. Endocranial volumes of primate species: Scaling analyses using a comprehensive and reliable data set. J. Hum. Evol. 55(6), 967–978 (2008).
Burns, J. G., Saravanan, A. & Rodd, F. H. Rearing environment affects the brain size of guppies: Lab-reared guppies have smaller brains than wild-caught guppies. Ethol. 115(2), 122–133 (2009).
Kruska, D. On the evolutionary significance of encephalization in some eutherian mammals: Effects of adaptive radiation, domestication, and feralization. Brain Behav. Evol. 65(2), 73–108 (2005).
Logan, C. J. & Clutton-Brock, T. H. Validating methods for estimating endocranial volume in individual red deer (Cervus elaphus). Behav. Processes. 92, 143–146 (2013).
Colby, A. E., Kimock, C. M. & Higham, J. P. Endocranial volume is variable and heritable, but not related to fitness, in a free-ranging primate. Sci. Rep. 11, 1–11 (2021).
Stuermer, I. W. & Wetzel, W. Early experience and domestication affect auditory discrimination learning, open field behaviour and brain size in wild Mongolian gerbils and domesticated Laboratory gerbils (Meriones unguiculatus forma domestica). Behav. Brain Res. 173, 11–21 (2006).
Agnvall, B., Bélteky, J. & Jensen, P. Brain size is reduced by selection for tameness in red junglefowl-correlated effects in vital organs. Sci. Rep. 7(3306), 1–7 (2017).
Röhrs, M. & Ebinger, P. Wild is not really wild: Brain weight of wild and domestic mammals. Berl. Munch. Tierarztliche Wochenschrift. 112(6–7), 234–238 (1999).
Smith, B. P., Lucas, T. A., Norris, R. M. & Henneberg, M. Brain size/body weight in the dingo (Canis dingo): Comparisons with domestic and wild canids. Aust. J. Zool. 65(5), 292–301 (2017).
Roberts, T., McGreevy, P. & Valenzuela, M. Human induced rotation and reorganization of the brain of domestic dogs. PLoS ONE 5(7), e11946 (2010).
Pollen, A. A. et al. Environmental complexity and social organization sculpt the brain in Lake Tanganyikan cichlid fish. Brain Behav. Evol. 70, 21–39 (2007).
Kihslinger, R. L., Lema, S. C. & Nevitt, G. A. Environmental rearing conditions produce forebrain differences in wild Chinook salmon Oncorhynchus tshawytscha. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. 145(2), 145–151 (2006).
Guay, P. J. & Iwaniuk, A. N. Captive breeding reduces brain volume in waterfowl (Anseriformes). Condor 110(2), 276–284 (2008).
Diamond, M. C., Ingham, C. A., Johnson, R. E., Bennett, E. L. & Rosenzweig, M. R. Effects of environment on morphology of rat cerebral cortex and hippocampus. J. Neurobiol. 7, 75–85 (1976).
Courtney Jones, S. K., Munn, A. J. & Byrne, P. G. Effect of captivity on morphology: Negligible changes in external morphology mask significant changes in internal morphology. R. Soc. Open Sci. 5(5), 1–13 (2018).
Kruska, D. & Röhrs, M. Comparative-quantitative investigations on brains of feral pigs from the Galapagos Islands and of European domestic pigs. Z. Anat. Entwicklungsgesch. 144(1), 61–73 (1974).
Kruska, D. Changes of brain size in Tylopoda during phylogeny and caused by domestication. Verh. Dtsch. Zool. Ges. 75, 173–183 (1982).
Groves, C. P. Skull-changes due to captivity in certain Equidae. Z. Säugetierkd. 31, 44–46 (1966).
Groves, C. P. The skulls of Asian rhinoceroses: Wild and captive. Zoo Biol. 1, 251–261 (1982).
Hollister, N. Some effects of environment and habit on captive lions. Proc. US. Natl. Mus. 53, 177–193 (1917).
Price, E. O. Behavioral development in animals undergoing domestication. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 65(3), 245–271 (1999).
Wolff, J. Das Gesetz der Transformation der Knochen (A. Hirchwild, 1892).
Herring, S. W. Formation of the vertebrate face: Epigenetic and functional influences. Am. Zool. 33, 472–483 (1993).
Wroe, S. & Milne, N. Convergence and remarkably consistent constraint in the evolution of carnivore skull shape. Evol. 61(5), 1251–1260 (2007).
Damasceno, E. M., Hingst-Zaher, E. & Astúa, D. Bite force and encephalization in the Canidae (Mammalia: Carnivora). J. Zool. 290(4), 246–254 (2013).
Van Valkenburgh, B. Deja vu: the evolution of feeding morphologies in the Carnivora. Integr. Comp. Biol. 47, 147–163 (2007).
Van Valkenburgh, B. Carnivore dental adaptations and diet: A study of trophic diversity within guilds in Carnivore behavior, ecology, and evolution (ed. Gittleman, J. L.) 410–436 (Springer Science & Business Media, 1989).
Slater, G. J., Dumont, E. R. & Van Valkenburgh, B. Implications of predatory specialization for cranial form and function in canids. J. Zool. 278(3), 181–188 (2009).
Michaud, M., Veron, G. & Fabre, A. C. Phenotypic integration in feliform carnivores: Covariation patterns and disparity in hypercarnivores versus generalists. Evol. 74(12), 2681–2702 (2020).
O’Regan, H. J. & Kitchener, A. C. The effects of captivity on the morphology of captive, domesticated and feral mammals. Mamm. Rev. 35, 215–230 (2005).
Kapoor, V., Antonelli, T., Parkinson, J. A. & Hartstone-Rose, A. Oral health correlates of captivity. Res. Vet. Sci. 107, 213–219 (2016).
Mitchell, D. R., Wroe, S., Ravosa, M. J. & Menegaz, R. A. More challenging diets sustain feeding performance: Applications toward the captive rearing of wildlife. Integr. Org. Biol. 3, 1–13 (2021).
Curtis, A. A., Orke, M., Tetradis, S. & Van Valkenburgh, B. Diet-related differences in craniodental morphology between captive-reared and wild coyotes, Canis latrans (Carnivora: Canidae). Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 123(3), 677–693 (2018).
Siciliano-Martina, L., Light, J. E. & Lawing, A. M. Cranial morphology of captive mammals: A meta-analysis. Front. Zool. 18(4), 1–13 (2021).
Corruccini, R. S. & Beecher, R. M. Occlusal variation related to soft diet in a nonhuman primate. Science 218, 74–75 (1982).
Ramirez Rozzi, F. V., González-José, R. & Pucciarelli, H. M. Cranial growth in normal and low-protein-fed Saimiri An environmental heterochrony. J. Hum. Evol. 49(4), 515–535 (2005).
Taylor, A. B. & van Schaik, C. P. Variation in brain size and ecology in Pongo. J. Hum. Evol. 52, 59–71 (2007).
AZA Canid TAG. Large Canid (Canidae) Care Manual. (Association of Zoos and Aquariums, 2012).
Mexican Wolf Species Survival Plan. Mexican Gray Wolf Husbandry Manual: Guidelines for Captive Management (2009 edition). (Mexican Wolf Species Survival Plan and U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service, 2009).
Carrera, R. et al. Comparison of Mexican wolf and coyote diets in Arizona and New Mexico. The J. Wildl. Manag. 72(2), 376–381 (2008).
Reed, J. E. et al. Diets of free-ranging Mexican gray wolves in Arizona and New Mexico. Wildl. Soc. Bull. 34(4), 1127–1133 (2006).
Kazimierska, K., Biel, W. & Witkowicz, R. Mineral composition of cereal and cereal-free dry dog foods versus nutritional guidelines. Molecules 25(21), 1–24 (2020).
Pezzali, J. G. & Aldrich, C. G. Effect of ancient grains and grain-free carbohydrate sources on extrusion parameters and nutrient utilization by dogs. J. Anim. Sci. 98(2), 3758–3767 (2019).
Hartstone-Rose, A., Selvey, H., Villari, J. R., Atwell, M. & Schmidt, T. The three-dimensional morphological effects of captivity. PLoS ONE 9(11), 1–15 (2014).
Siciliano-Martina, L., Light, J. E. & Lawing, A. M. Changes in canid cranial morphology induced by captivity and conservation implications. Biol. Conserv. 257, 109143 (2021).
Hedrick, P. W. & Fredrickson, R. Genetic rescue guidelines with examples from Mexican wolves and Florida panthers. Conserv. Genet. 11(2), 615–626 (2010).
Greely, S. E. Mexican Wolf, Canis lupus baileyi, International Studbook 2018. Palm Desert, California. (2018).
Kalinowski, S. T., Hedrick, P. W. & Miller, P. S. No inbreeding depression observed in Mexican and red wolf captive breeding programs. Conserv. Biol. 13(6), 1371–1377 (1999).
Sakai, S. T., Whitt, B., Arsznov, B. M. & Lundrigan, B. L. Endocranial development in the coyote (Canis latrans) and gray wolf (Canis lupus): A computed tomographic study. Brain Behav. Evol. 91(2), 1–18 (2018).
Van Valkenburgh, B. Skeletal and dental predictors of body mass in carnivores in Body size in mammalian paleobiology: estimation and biological implications (eds. Damuth, J. & MacFadden, B. J.) (Cambridge University Press, 1990).
Rohlf, F. J. TPSDig2: a program for landmark development and analysis (2001).
Siciliano-Martina, L., Light, J. E., Riley, D. G. & Lawing, A. M. One of these wolves is not like the other: morphological effects and conservation implications of captivity in Mexican wolves. Anim. Conserv. 25, 77–90 (2021).
Zelditch, M. L., Donald, L., Swiderski, H., Sheets, D. & Fink, W. L. Geometric morphometrics for biologists: a primer. (Elsevier Academic Press, 2004).
Coster, A. pedigree: Pedigree functions. R package version 1.4 (2013).
Traylor-Holzer, K. (ed.). PMx user’s manual. Version 1.0. Apple Valley, MN: IUCN SSC Conservation Breeding Specialist Group. (2011).
Thomason, J. J. Cranial strength in relation to estimated biting forces in some Mammals. Can. J. Zool. 69, 2326–2333 (1991).
Schindelin, J. et al. Fiji: an open-source platform for biological-image analysis. Nat. Methods. 9(7), 676–682 (2012).
R Core Team R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. (2020).
Cofran, Z. Brain size growth in wild and captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Am. J. Primat. 80(7), 1–8 (2018).
Witzenberger, K. A. & Hochkirch, A. Ex situ conservation genetics: A review of molecular studies on the genetic consequences of captive breeding programmes for endangered animal species. Biodivers. Conserv. 20(9), 1843–1861 (2011).
Gómez-Sánchez, D. et al. On the path to extinction: Inbreeding and admixture in a declining grey wolf population. Mole. Ecol. 27(18), 3599–3612 (2018).
Elbroch, M. Animal skulls: a guide to North American species. (Stackpole Books, 2006).
Conde, D. A., Flesness, N., Colchero, F., Jones, O. R. & Scheuerlein, A. An emerging role of zoos to conserve biodiversity. Science 331, 1390–1391 (2011).
Prado, E. L. & Dewey, K. G. Nutrition and brain development in early life. Nutr. Rev. 72(4), 267–284 (2014).
Hecht, E. E. et al. Neuromorphological changes following selection for tameness and aggression in the Russian farm-fox experiment. J. Neurosci. 41(28), 6144–6156 (2021).
Bennett, E. L., Rosenzweig, M. R. & Diamond, M. C. Rat brain: Effects of environmental enrichment on wet and dry weights. Science 163(3869), 825–826 (1969).
Cummins, R. A., Walsh, R. N., Budtz-Olsen, O. E., Konstantinos, T. & Horsfall, C. R. Environmentally-induced changes in the brains of elderly rats. Nature 243(5409), 516–518 (1973).
Welch, B. L., Brown, D. G., Welch, A. S. & Lin, D. C. Isolation, restrictive confinement or crowding of rats for one year. I. Weight, nucleic acids and protein of brain regions. Brain Res. 75, 71–84 (1974).
Acknowledgements
This research was funded, in part, through the EEB Research Award from the Ecology and Evolutionary Biology program at Texas A&M University. This research would not have been possible without the use of natural history collections. We wish to thank all of the staff members at the Museum of Southwestern Biology in Albuquerque, New Mexico, particularly J. Cook, J. Dunnum, and A. Raniszewski. We also thank L. Fitzgerald and J.P. Martina for providing the electronic balances used in this study.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
L.S.M. and A.M.L. designed this work. All authors interpreted the data. All authors contributed to writing and editing this manuscript. All authors approved of the final document and have agreed to be accountable for the integrity of the work.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Siciliano-Martina, L., Michaud, M., Tanis, B.P. et al. Endocranial volume increases across captive generations in the endangered Mexican wolf. Sci Rep 12, 8147 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-12371-6
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-12371-6
Comments
By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.