Introduction

Developments in medical technology have markedly improved the survival rates of low-birth-weight infants; however, such infants have increased risks of neurodevelopmental abnormalities1,2. Several studies have also reported that preterm birth and low-birth-weight are risk factors for future developmental disabilities such as intellectual disability (ID)3,4 and autism spectrum disorder (ASD)5,6,7,8,9. In recent years, assessing neurological development based on the characteristics of spontaneous movements and behaviours can be useful in considering interventions for infants at risk of developmental problems. In particular, it has been reported that a type of spontaneous movement, defined by Prechtl as general movements (GMs)10, is an early sensitive marker of cerebral palsy (CP) later in life11,12. Furthermore, a relationship between the qualitative abnormality of GMs, ID13,14, and ASD15,16,17 has been reported, but its accuracy is still inadequate compared with that of the relationship with CP. Regarding ID and ASD, observing the various behavioural characteristics rather than the qualitative abnormalities of GMs may be useful as an early sign18,19,20,21,22,23,24,25,26.

We have focused on the characteristics of spontaneous movements in early infancy and reported their relationship with neurological development26,27,28,29. We have reported that it is useful to quantify the various spontaneous movements that infants show; however, a detailed evaluation of their behavioural characteristics has thus far been sufficient. Therefore, we conducted a detailed observational evaluation of behavioural characteristics during early infancy in low-birth-weight infants using videos recorded for the assessment of GMs. In this study, we introduced the Infant’s Behaviour Checklist (IBC) and examined the relationship between behavioural characteristics of low-birth-weight infants assessed using the IBC and their neurodevelopmental outcomes at 6 years of age. The IBC lists 44 items considered important in assessing spontaneous movement and behavioural and postural characteristics of low-birth-weight infants. Clarifying the relationship between detailed behavioural characteristics in early infancy and later neurological development is important for enhancing developmental care for low-birth-weight infants.

Results

Participant characteristics

The distribution of the developmental outcomes at age 6 years in neonatal assessment (NA) was as follows: typical development (TD), n = 89; ID, n = 30; and ASD, n = 24. The distribution of the developmental outcomes at age 6 years in early infancy assessment (EA) was as follows: TD, n = 40; ID, n = 21; and ASD, n = 17. For the 45 infants who belonged to both groups, the distribution of the developmental outcomes at age 6 years was as follows: TD, n = 25; ID, n = 11; and ASD, n = 9.

The background factors of NA and EA are listed in Tables 1 and 2, respectively. In the NA group, the birth weight was significantly lower in the ASD than in the TD group. In the EA group, there were no significant differences between the groups in terms of birth weight, gestational age (weeks), or age in weeks at video recording.

Table 1 Characteristics and developmental outcome of the neonatal assessment group.
Table 2 Characteristics and developmental outcome of the early infancy assessment group.

Evaluation of the behavioural characteristics using IBC

The frequency of observation of each behaviour in all groups for NA and EA is shown in Table 3. In the NA group, the ID group exhibited significantly fewer instances of trunk rotation (item 19, 10%), separate finger flexion and extension (item 25, 20%), and lower limb flexion and hip internal rotation and external rotation (item 27, 27%) than did the TD group. In contrast, there were no items in the ASD group that showed a significant difference. In the EA group, the ID group showed significantly fewer instances of toe flexion and extension separately (item 32, 19%) than did the TD group. Meanwhile, the ASD group showed significantly more instances of asymmetrical tonic neck reflex (ATNR) (item 3, 35%) and toe flexion (item 16, 41%), and there were significantly fewer instances of head at the midline (item 2, 0%), neck rotation (item 18, 29%), and wrist rotation (item 24, 0%).

Table 3 Percentage of presence behaviour in all items by group in the neonatal and early infancy assessment.

Figure 1 shows the distribution of the data for each item in the ID and the ASD groups at NA and EA terms by the difference of percentage of participants for whom the posture and behaviour appeared. Each line represents the value of the TD group minus the value of the ID (blue line) and the ASD (red line) group, with larger positive values indicating a higher proportion of appearance compared to the TD group, and larger negative values indicating a lower proportion of appearance.

Figure 1
figure 1

Radar chart of the difference percentage between the typical development group and the other group at NA and EA terms. The red and blue line shows the distribution of the difference of percentage of participants for whom the posture and behaviour appeared. Each line represents the value of the TD group minus the value of the ID and the ASD group (note that the values of the TD group are all zero and the values have no units). *p < 0.05 in comparison with the TD group using Fisher’s exact test corrected by the Benjamini–Hochberg method.

Discussion

In this study, we introduced the IBC and examined the relationship between the behavioural characteristics of low-birth-weight infants assessed using IBC and their neurodevelopmental outcomes at 6 years of age. In particular, we focused on ID and ASD as developmental outcomes, and examined the characteristics of spontaneous movements and behaviour during the neonatal and early infancy periods. These periods are considered to be important for qualitative evaluation of the characteristics of spontaneous movements10,11,12,30 and also from the viewpoint of the developmental mechanism of the central nervous system10,31,32,33. In particular, fidgety movements (FMs) which appear during the EA period, have been reported to be strongly related to later neurological developmental outcomes10,11,12,13,14,15,16,17,29,30,31,32,33. The assessment of characteristics of spontaneous movements and behaviour at both NA and EA is considered important to verify early motor signs of ID and ASD.

Evaluation using the IBC revealed the following behavioural characteristics in the ID group: (1) Among the items of IBC, a significantly lower frequency of appearance was found in some items related to behaviour, but not in items related to posture. (2) The number of items related to behaviour with a significantly lower frequency of appearance was larger in the NA group than that in the EA group. In the ID group, the frequencies of items 19 (trunk rotation), 25 (separate finger flexion and extension), and 27 (lower limb flexion and hip internal rotation and external rotation) were significantly lower in NA; in contrast, the frequency of item 32 (toe flexion and extension separately) was significantly lower in EA. Meanwhile, there were no items related to posture that showed a significant difference between NA and EA. The characteristic behaviour in the ID group may reflect the low quantitative characteristics of spontaneous movements and behaviour in the ID group. A previous study that examined the characteristics of spontaneous limb movements by computer-based analysis using the same image data as in this study reported that the value of the quantitative indices (average velocity and number of movement units per min) in the NA was significantly lower in the ID than in the TD group27. In the evaluation using the IBC, items related to behaviour were judged ‘present’ when the behaviour was observed at least twice. Since a reduction in the activity of spontaneous movements also leads to a reduced frequency of appearance of each behaviour, it is considered that a significantly lower frequency of appearance in items related to behaviour was observed in the ID group. However, a previous study examining the characteristics of spontaneous limb movement in EA reported no significant difference in the values of quantitative indices between the ID group and the TD group26. The quantitative characteristics of spontaneous movements in the ID group may be more apparent in the NA than in the EA group. The results of the present study also indicated that the quantitative characteristics of spontaneous movements and behaviour in the ID group may appear more prominently in the NA than in the EA group, suggesting the importance of evaluating NA for the ID group.

The important results in the evaluation of the ASD group were as follows: (1) There were no items in the NA group that showed a significant difference. (2) In the EA group, there were many items related to posture that showed a significant difference in the appearance rate. (3) All items that showed a significant difference in the appearance rate were related to the movement of the neck and distal part of the upper and lower limbs. Previous studies that examined early signs of ASD by assessing the characteristics of spontaneous movements, posture, and behaviour reported features, such as abnormal upper extremity muscle tone21, postural asymmetries during lying18,20, head-lag during the pull-to-sit transition23, oral-motor abnormalities22, motor developmental delay in early infancy24, qualitative abnormalities in GMs16,17, poorer performance in maintaining the midline position of the head26, and lower motion complexity in legs34. All these early signs of ASD have been observed after 3 months of age. Therefore, it is highly possible that the results of this study also contain more information on the early behavioural characteristics of ASD in EA than in NA. In the EA evaluation, the frequencies of items 2 (head midline), 18 (neck rotation), and 24 (wrist rotation) were significantly lower, and 3 (ATNR) and 16 (toe flexion) were significantly higher in the ASD than in the TD group. Of these, items related to head movement and position (head midline, neck rotation, and ATNR) were considered to reflect the poorer performance in maintaining the midline position of the head in the previous study26. Meanwhile, the small amount of wrist rotation and the high ratio toe flexion may reflect the abnormality of FMs in ASD. FMs are a type of GM characterized by small continual movements of moderate speed and variable acceleration of the neck, trunk, and limbs in all directions that appears in early infancy and is defined as ‘an ongoing stream of small and circular movements of limbs’32,33. A study examining the kinematic features of FMs reported that the value of curvature in the movement trajectory of the distal limbs was related to the frequency of FMs appearance35. This value of the curvature is the magnitude by which a trajectory deviates from being straight and reflects the fine rotational movement of the wrist and the ankle. Previous studies have reported that the appearance of FMs is often absent in infants with ASD15,16,17, and the characteristics of distal limb movements and posture in the ASD group in this study were considered to be related to FMs abnormalities (absence of FMs), similar to results of previous studies.

Preterm birth and low-birth-weight are risk factors for ASD5,6,7,8,9. However, detecting the early motor signs of ASD during early infancy is a considerable challenge. We used the IBC to investigate the relationship between behaviours in the neonatal and early infancy periods with development at the age of 6 years due to disability. We also ascertained the characteristics of behaviours by disability in the neonatal and early infancy periods. Behavioural characteristics during early infancy in low-birth-weight infants reflected the diagnosis of later ID and ASD. Identifying factors that allow the early recognition of ASD is crucial because early intervention results in the development of better socially engaged imitation, eye contact, cognitive ability, adaptive behaviour, socialization, daily living skills, and motor skills for ASD36,37. Behavioural assessment using the IBC may provide promising clues when considering early intervention for low-birth-weight infants.

The limitations of this study need to be considered. Firstly, for consistency, the observational assessments were all performed by one assessor. In clinical practice, it would be important to have more than three assessors to ensure a reliable evaluation. Secondly, our study’s assessments using the IBC did not compare the severity of disabilities or scoring. Furthermore, several participants (13 of 24 in the NA group and 3 of 17 in the EA group) in the ASD group had difficulty in completing the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC) III due to the characteristics of ASD; thus, their intelligence quotient (IQ) could not be assessed accurately (see Supplementary Fig. S1 online). This is an important issue worthy of future investigation. Therefore, we would like to conduct further studies using a larger sample size to investigate the disability severity grading and scoring and the impact of comorbid ASD and ID.

Methods

Ethics

The present study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Nagano Children’s Hospital. The infants’ parents or guardians were provided with both written and oral explanations of how the data would be handled (use in written reports and presentations at academic conferences, protection of private information) before they provided signed consent. All procedures were performed in accordance with the approved guidelines and regulations.

Participants

We included 564 extremely-low-birth-weight infants who were hospitalized in the neonatal intensive care unit of Nagano Children’s Hospital between 2002 and 2010. We assessed the behaviours of 143 such infants during the neonatal period (36 weeks 0 days to 43 weeks 6 days, adjusted, the NA group) and 78 infants in the early infancy period (49 weeks 0 days to 60 weeks 6 days, adjusted, the EA group) (Fig. 2).

Figure 2
figure 2

Flow diagram showing study participant enrolment for behavioural assessment during the neonatal and early infancy periods.

Recording of behaviour

The infants’ behaviours were recorded by videography (DCR-TRV20 or DVR-PC350, SONY, Tokyo, Japan). The video recording method was modelled on that of Einspieler et al.23 The infants in the NA group were placed supine on a cot in the neonatal intensive care unit, and those in the EA group were placed supine on an outpatient bed mat wearing light clothing and diapers. The infants were monitored for 30 min. Video recordings were made at 3–5-min intervals, during which they were spontaneously moving without being excited or crying. Video recordings were recorded 1 h before or after nursing. The video recordings were made with a camera placed on a tripod directly above the infants. Using the videos, we assessed the behaviour over approximately 1 min during which the infants were alert and awake and showed continuous movements.

Assessment of behaviour in IBC

The behaviours and assessment criteria for the IBC are listed in Table 4. In this study, we have developed a list of observation items based on methods from the Neurological Assessment of the Preterm and Full-term Newborn Infant38 (item numbers 9, 16, 17, and 40–44) and the Newborn Individualized Developmental Care and Assessment Program39 (item numbers 33–39), which are commonly used in clinical practice to evaluate the behaviour of infants. In addition, the other items were picked up for postures and behaviour that the authors feel were commonly observed in clinical practice.

Table 4 Items of Infant’s Behaviour Checklist (IBC) and assessment criteria.

Items related to posture (item numbers 1 − 17) were judged ‘present’ when the condition was observed for more than half of the observation time; if not, they were judged ‘absent’. This criterion is based on the criteria for evaluating the temporal organization of fidgety movements in the Assessment of General Movements30 and the activity in the Neonatal Behavioral Assessment Scale40.

Items related to behaviour (item numbers 18 − 44) were judged ‘present’ when the behaviour was observed at least twice (uni- or bilaterally); if not, they were judged ‘absent’. Seven of the 44 items (item numbers 2, 13, 16, 24, 31, 32, and 38) were not included in the NA assessments because neonates cannot hold their heads upright or coordinate separate peripheral movements. For consistency, assessments using the IBC were performed by a single physical therapist (H.K.). Note that the assessor was blinded with regards to the participants’ characteristics (sex, birth weight, gestational age, and developmental outcome).

Assessment of developmental outcome at 6 years of age

Developmental outcomes were determined for children aged 6 years. The WISC III was administered at the development outpatient department of Nagano Children’s Hospital. The tests were administered by a speech-language-hearing therapist or a clinical psychologist. The WISC is an analytical intelligence test applied to children aged 5 to 16 years, in which verbal IQ, performance IQ, and full-scale IQ are each assessed as a deviation IQ (mean 100, one standard deviation 15) indicating their relative position in the same age group41,42. In this study, according to the criteria of the WISC, full-scale IQ of ≥ 80 was considered normal; 70–79, borderline; and ≤ 69, delayed. Diagnoses were made by a paediatric psychiatrist using the results of the WISC, brain imaging findings, and the criteria listed in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (fourth edition). Based on the diagnoses, the children were assigned to the TD, ID, ASD, or CP groups. Children who could not be diagnosed with ASD or CP but who had an IQ of ≥ 80 were placed in the TD group, and those with an IQ of ≤ 79 were placed in the ID group.

Statistical analyses

The background characteristics of all groups were compared using the Mann–Whitney U test. We calculated the frequency of observance of each behaviour in each group. Fisher’s exact test was used to compare the frequency of behaviours in the ID, ASD, and TD groups. Note that we corrected the p-values using the Benjamini–Hochberg method to account for the effect of false positives due to multiple testing43. The False Discovery Rate was set to 0.05 in order to carry out an exploratory test to find the important items.