Abstract
Most research on people’s representation of space has focused on spatial appraisal and navigation. But there is more to space besides navigation and assessment: people have different emotional experiences at different places, which create emotionally tinged representations of space. Little is known about the emotional representation of space and the factors that shape it. The purpose of this study was to develop a graphic methodology to study the emotional representation of space and some of the environmental features (non-natural vs. natural) and personal features (affective state and interoceptive sensibility) that modulate it. We gave participants blank maps of the region where they lived and asked them to apply shade where they had happy/sad memories, and where they wanted to go after Covid-19 lockdown. Participants also completed self-reports on affective state and interoceptive sensibility. By adapting methods for analyzing neuroimaging data, we examined shaded pixels to quantify where and how strong emotions are represented in space. The results revealed that happy memories were consistently associated with similar spatial locations. Yet, this mapping response varied as a function of participants’ affective state and interoceptive sensibility. Certain regions were associated with happier memories in participants whose affective state was more positive and interoceptive sensibility was higher. The maps of happy memories, desired locations to visit after lockdown, and regions where participants recalled happier memories as a function of positive affect and interoceptive sensibility overlayed significantly with natural environments. These results suggest that people’s emotional representations of their environment are shaped by the naturalness of places, and by their affective state and interoceptive sensibility.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
Our diverse activities commonly require us to move from one place to another1,2,3. We find our way from one place to the next using complex spatial representations, and we decide where to go and how long to stay there based on appraisals and expectations. Most of what is known about people’s representation of space comes from studies on spatial navigation and appraisal1,2,3,4,5,6,7,8. But the places that make up our environment are not only places we go to and come from; they are also the stages upon which we have experiences, upon which we experience our own bodies. Research on spatial representation has commonly overlooked two crucial facts. First, people’s representations of space are emotionally tinged. Second, people’s representations of space are based on internal models of their own bodies.
Representations of space are emotionally tinged
As we move through the environments, our representation of space (i.e., cognitive maps2,9) becomes intertwined with emotional experiences. Some of these experiences might be pleasant, others unpleasant, and others indifferent. We might wish for more of such experiences, or long for different ones. As a result, our cognitive maps get updated with both past emotional experiences and desired future states7,10,11. For instance, we might feel excited or happy as we stroll along a beach or hike up a mountain, whereas during the Covid-19 pandemic lockdowns we might feel cut off at home, and eager for the seaside or the mountains.
Representations of space are based on internal body models
Recent evidence shows that people’s assessment of their environment involves running an internal model of their body in the world12,13. Such a model reflects information about their affective state, physiological state, and desired future states12,13,14,15. This idea is supported by studies that show how differences in the way people feel and appraise their inner bodily states (i.e., affective state and interoception, respectively) influence how they relate to features of their environment16,17,18,19,20,21. Thus, crucial to the representation of space is the weighing of bodily representations against the computed value of certain environmental features22,23. In this context, people appraise and seek out certain environments because of the relevant resources for psychophysiological balance they afford24. This balance is achieved through the constant regulation and anticipation of future needs and challenges in the environment (i.e., allostasis)25,26.
In sum, people’s representation of space includes aspects related to navigation and appraisal but, crucially, also aspects related to emotion: the emotional representation of space. The main purpose of this study was to examine people’s emotional representation of space and how it is shaped by certain environmental features, such as whether a place is natural or non-natural, and certain personal features, such as affective state and interoceptive sensibility (Fig. 1).
The current study was conducted during the Spanish nationwide Covid-19 lockdown (March-June 2020) and restricted to the island of Mallorca, Spain (~ 100 km wide × 75 km long). This allowed us to examine past emotional experiences in space and desire for future environments after a period of distress and restricted mobility. The target territory, bounded by the sea, included a UNESCO World Heritage cultural site (where the natural landscape merges with small villages), fields, towns, crops, and cities, providing a diverse testing ground to examine our hypotheses.
To achieve our goal, we developed a new approach that integrated the recollection of autobiographical memories, self-report questionnaires, and graphic methods adapted from those used to analyze neuroimaging data (Fig. 2). Participants were asked to perform a computerized task consisting in shading four separate blank maps of Mallorca to show where they lived, where they had happy memories, where they had sad memories, and where they most looked forward to going after the Spanish national Covid-19 lockdown. This task required participants to access autobiographical memories, which are known to encompass past emotional experiences27,28. Importantly, their valence can be assessed by asking participants to associate these memories with ‘basic’ emotions28,29,30,31,32. Next, we examined how the representation of these emotional memories in space were shaped by the two person features noted above: affective state and interoceptive sensibility. To this end, we recorded participants’ scores in two self-report questionnaires: the Depression, Anxiety, and Stress scale (DASS21)33, which measures affective state34 in the form of scores in depression, anxiety, and stress, and the Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness (MAIA)35, which measures the capability to appraise inner bodily sensations (currently termed interoceptive sensibility)36. Factoring these measures into the analysis would us to examine whether the representation of emotional memories in space varied according to individual differences in affective state and the conscious processing of inner bodily states.
As noted above, we also wished to examine how place features contribute to shaping emotional representations of space. To this end, we contrasted the participants’ shaded areas with an objective representation of natural and non-natural environments. This allowed us to test the overlapping of their responses in one vs. the other environment (Fig. 2) and to contextualize our results within a growing number of studies that have compared peoples’ affect and physiological regulation in natural and urban locations37,38,39,40,41,42. Finally, given that emotional representations of space might be influenced by previous allostasis (our past experiences determine our present concerns, which in turn influence prospective future behaviours12,26), we asked our participants to represent those locations that they most looked forward to going after the national Covid-19 lockdown. Then, we contrasted again these responses with an objective representation of natural and non-natural environments (see “Methods” section; Table 1).
Each of the four shaded maps produced by each of the participants was subjected to Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM1243) a statical technique used to examine brain activity recorded in functional neuroimaging experiments (Fig. 2). We examined shaded pixels instead of voxels in the brain to quantify where in the maps and how strong participants’ responses relate to one’s geographical space (i.e., are certain locations consistently associated with particular emotions?). This approach does not rely on the semantic report of the participants (i.e., opinions about specific points in space), which might create problems for the statistical interpretation of the data, such as uneven information across participants or abstract judgments4 (see “Methods”). Also, it allows using the same method across different populations and target environments, as well as contrasting the subjective response of the population with more objective data (e.g., maps with environmental features).
Given the relatively small size of the island where the sample was taken and the study performed, and that cognitive maps function as shared repositories for personal and group information about places, relationships, beliefs, and values44, we hypothesized that participants would convergently shade in the same areas. Yet, we also postulated significant differences in the mapping response according to individual psychophysiological differences. Specifically, we hypothesized that certain locations would be associated with happier memories when accompanied by more positive affect and higher interoceptive sensibility. Moreover, considering that experiences in natural areas have been linked to improved mental health and physiological regulation37,38,39,40,41,42, we hypothesized that these regions and desired locations after the lockdown would be associated with natural environments.
Results
Spatial maps of emotional memories, environmental features, and individual differences in affect and interoceptive sensibility
We asked our participants to shade the maps where they have happy and sad memories. The mean proportion of shaded territory was 0.18 (SD = 0.15) for happy memories and 0.05 (SD = 0.04) for sad memories. The subject-wise combination of happy and sad maps yielded a mean proportion of shaded territory of 0.20 (SD = 0.15). Figure 3A shows participants’ mean map. Next, for the maps that participants shaded, we estimated the proportion of shaded pixels that overlapped with natural and non-natural environments. The latter included human-made locations such as cities or towns, and non-accessible human-altered environments such as crop fields and cattle raising areas (see “Methods” and Supp. materials). To this end, we used publicly available government forest maps, computed the proportion of shaded pixels that overlapped with each environment, and normalized these proportions according to all the pixels available per environment. Then, we computed these proportions in a repeated-measures ANOVA with two factors and two levels: environment (natural, non-natural) and Emotional memory (happy, sad). This analysis yielded significant main effects of Environment (F(1,200) = 33.55, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.144) and Emotional memory (F(1,200) = 165.45, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.453), and a significant interaction between them (F(1,200) = 69.81, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.259). We followed up this interaction by comparing the proportion of pixels reported for happy and sad emotional memories in each environment. This analysis revealed that the proportion of pixels were participants reported happy memories in natural areas (Mdn = 0.143), was significantly higher (Z = 5230, p = < 0.001, r = 0.480) than in non-natural areas (Mdn = 0.106). Conversely, the proportion of pixels reported for sad memories in natural areas (Mdn = 0.016) was significantly lower (Z = 14,522, p = < 0.001, r = 0.551) than in non-natural areas (Mdn = 0.027); Fig. 3B.
After the pixel quantification, we examined whether emotional memories can be consistently mapped in geographical space. We subjected the subject-wise maps to Statistical Parametric Mapping43. The results showed that participants’ emotional memories consistently relate to locations in space: there was an overlap in areas shaded by participants (across participants, same locations shaded with similar valence). Yet, this was only observed for places where they reported having happy memories (i.e., consistent mapping significantly > 0; FWE corr. Fig. 3C,D). Places where participants reported having negative memories were not consistently mapped. Alternatively, ambivalent places that were marked as both happy and sad could have cancelled each other out.
We also examined the relationship between participants’ affective state and emotional representations of space by correlating their scores in the Dass21 questionnaire (Spanish validated version45) with their spatial maps of emotional memories. This revealed that several areas correlated negatively with the Dass21, meaning that participants whose affective state was more positive consistently reported having happier memories at these locations (Fig. 4A). Also, we assessed the correlation of each subscale of the Dass21 questionnaire (i.e., depression, anxiety, and stress scales) with the participants’ spatial maps of emotional memories. This revealed smaller regional clusters that negatively correlated with each subscale (Supp. Figure 3).
Likewise, we also examined the relationship between participants’ interoceptive sensibility and their emotional representation of space by correlating their scores in the MAIA questionnaire (Spanish validated version46) and their spatial maps of emotional memories. This allowed examining whether the sensing and appraisal of inner bodily sensations, a key constituent of one’s cognitive and affective experience, relates to one’s emotional memories. The results showed that two dimensions of interoceptive sensibility correlated positively with certain areas of the map. These were Self-Regulation and Trusting of the body, namely, one’s ability to regulate distress by attending to bodily sensation and to experience one’s body as safe and trustworthy35. These areas were linked to happier memories when accompanied by high scores in these dimensions of interoceptive sensibility (Fig. 4B,C). Additionally, we inspected the proportion of pixels corresponding to natural and non-natural environments in the areas revealed by the SPM analysis. To this end, we computed the environment where participants reported happy memories, areas that negatively correlated with higher scores on the DASS scale, and areas that positively correlated with higher interoceptive sensibility. In all these areas, the proportion of reported pixels was greater for natural than for non-natural areas (all Ps < 0.01; Fig. 4A–C; see Supp. Figure 4 for the overlap between these areas and circumscribed—protected natural spaces).
Overall, our results show that people’s spatial maps of emotional memories relate to geographical areas in a consistent manner, yet this was only observed for places where they reported having happy memories. No consistent significant mapping (or additional correlations) was found for sad memories in space. In addition, our results show that certain regions were correlated with participants’ happy memories when they scored lower in negative affect and higher in interoceptive sensibility.
Preference for visiting natural environments after the Covid-19 lockdown
Studies on the psychological effects of the pandemic have revealed an increase in mental illness, somatization, and drug abuse47,48. Given that certain environments may feature relevant resources for the attainment of psychological and physiological balance (allostasis), we also asked our participants to report where they most looked forward to going after the nationwide Covid-19 lockdown. The mean proportion of colored territory in these maps was 0.22 (SD = 0.24); see participants’ mean map in Fig. 5. For this data, we calculated which pixels of the map corresponded to natural vs. non-natural areas. The analysis revealed that the proportion of pixels were participants wanted to go after the lockdown was significantly greater for natural (Mdn = 0.178) than for non-natural environments (Mdn = 0.010), Z = 4043, p = < 0.0001, r = 0.602. In line with previous studies41,42,49, our results suggest that natural environments might provide a key to palliate present and forthcoming psychological distress by aiding to attain one psychophysiological equilibrium. This relationship is also suggested in an exploratory analysis where we related the proportion of pixels per desired environment with participants’ affect and interoceptive sensibility (see Suppl. analysis and Supp. Figure 5).
Living location does not moderate peoples’ environmental preference
Based on the participants’ living location map, we computed whether they lived in a natural or non-natural location. Next, we added the factor Living Location back to the initial quantification of shaded pixels in the subject-wise maps of emotional memories (rmANOVA with factors Environment and Emotional memory). The following interaction was not significant (F(1,199) = 0.891, p = 0.346, ηp2 = 0.004), suggesting that participants’ living environment did not modulate their mapping response in environmental space.
Furthermore, we observed that many participants lived in urban regions (81%). It follows that many participants could have reported both happy and sad memories in urban areas because they lived there. Considering that shading the same area as happy and sad might cancel the values of the subsequent region (i.e., ambivalent area; after combining happy and sad memory maps), participants’ living location could bias our results by cancelling out more non-natural than natural areas. Therefore, we decided to repeat our initial rmANOVA (Environment x Emotional memory) without combining happy and sad memory maps. The subsequent analyses yielded similar results. A significant interaction and main effect with more happy memories in natural environments (all ps < 0.01). Likewise, we computed the same rmANOVA in the subject-wise emotional memory maps by mitigating the effect of one’s living location. To do this, we subtracted the participants’ living location maps from their subject-wise emotional maps, and renormalized the proportion of coloured areas per environment on an individual basis. After implementing this step, the results remained the same: happy memories were more frequent in natural areas (all ps < 0.01). Lastly, we also related participants’ living location and where they wanted to go after the nationwide lockdown. The results showed that participants’ living location did not predict their environmental preference after the lockdown (F(1,199) = 0.034, p = 0.855), R2 = -0.005; see Suppl. Figure 6.
Participants’ affect correlates with interoceptive sensibility
As measured by the Dass21 and MAIA questionnaires33,35, we observed that participants affect correlated with their capability to notice and appraise inner bodily sensations (i.e., interoceptive sensibility). Specifically, participants with lower scores in the Dass21 questionnaire also scored higher in interoceptive sensibility (Spearman’s rho = − 0.281, p = 5.467e−5, Fig. 6; see Suppl. Figure 7 for subscales correlations). Previous research that used the same self-report questionnaires reported a similar association between interoceptive sensibility, depression, anxiety and stress50,51. Specifically, higher scores on the Dass21 significantly correlated with lower scores on the MAIA. Consistent with such research, we found similar results but in a larger sample. These findings support the validity of our self-report data with those of previous studies.
Discussion
The representation of space is embedded with memories of experiences and emotions. Yet, most studies on the representation of space have focused on navigational behaviors or judgements about locations. Moreover, when relating people’s affective and overall experience in space, the current methods often rely on the participants’ position during the response time (e.g. linking self-reports and GPS coordinates/zip codes38,52) or semantic reports4,5. However, mobility across the geography and how we relate to our territory are complex dynamics that might be difficult to measure with such methods. For instance, we might feel and reason about our environment even in the absence of actual mobility (during the nationwide Covid-19 lockdown). Here we developed a new paradigm that even in this case allowed us to sample the relationship between the emotional representation of one’s space, types of environment, affect, and interoception.
We gave participants blank maps of the region where they lived and asked them to shade the regions where they had happy and sad memories, as well as where they most looked forward to going after the Covid-19 national lockdown (past emotional experience and future desired locations in space). They also filled in two self-report questionnaires about their affect and capability to appraise inner bodily sensations (i.e., interoceptive sensibility). Based on techniques for neuroimaging, we analyzed shaded pixels in the maps to reveal the consistency of participants’ shading in the maps (i.e., whether participants shaded in similar regions with similar values), the correlation of this shading with their scores in the self-reports, as well as the proportion of shaded pixels overlapping with natural and non-natural environments. Our results showed that (1) happy memories are consistently mapped in geographical space; (2) that participants’ mapping seems to vary according to how they feel (affect) and interoceptive sensibility; (3) that areas reflecting positive memories overlapped more with natural than non-natural environments; (4) and that after the nationwide Covid-19 lockdown, participants wanted to visit more those areas that also corresponded to natural environments. Accordingly, we highlight three aspects of this work that need to be considered: its biological significance, the possibility of applying our results to other territories, and the flexibility of our method to sample further person-space interactions.
Biological significance
Participants’ mapping of emotions revealed that certain areas were associated with positive memories, more positive affect, self-regulation, and trusting in bodily sensations. These later aspects refer to one’s ability to regulate distress by attending to bodily sensations and to experience one’s own body as safe. Research on these aspects often relates individual differences to psychophysiological variables53,54,55,56,57. However, how these aspects can be mapped across people’s surroundings has not been examined. In our study, natural environments overlapped most with participants’ happy memories in space, lower negative affect, and higher interoceptive sensibility. Based on this and the notion of allostasis (i.e., keeping psychophysiological stability through behavioural change25), we suggest there is an interaction between environment, stressor exposure, mental health, and interoceptive sensibility. These results are in line with studies showing that natural spaces constitute a fundamental resource for mental health and physiological engagement through cardiovascular exercise39,41,58,59,60,61,62. In this context, past experiences in certain areas could exert a more restorative effect upon participants’ affect (i.e., lessening levels of depression, anxiety and stress). For instance, green and blue spaces have been associated with less psychological stress and rumination37,39. Additionally, after the national Covid-19 lockdown, participants wanted to visit areas that corresponded to natural environments (as prospective behaviour).
We believe that natural environments were chosen because they could offer psychophysiological alleviation38,41 and not because these spaces were more suitable during a pandemic. The study was conducted after the very first month of the national lockdown, therefore, it is unlikely that our participants had enough knowledge about the spread of the virus and the upcoming policies about spatial restrictions. An alternative explanation, which does not exclude the former, is that people with better affect might benefit more from these environments. In addition, we found that happy memories in space correlated with two subscales of the MAIA questionnaire: self-regulation and Body trusting. Considering that this mapping response overlapped the most with natural environments, it is reasonable to think that certain natural environments require that people ‘trust their bodies’ to positively experience the environment. One aspect that needs further examination is the direction of this relationship (i.e., do people increase their interoceptive sensibility by experiencing natural environments? is that people with higher scores in these subscales who attend/experience these environments the most?). Further research could examine these novel observations by carrying on longitudinal studies and pre- and post- interventions with natural environments.
Generalization of our results
We tested our method in an environment that ranged from fully natural (e.g., UNESCO World Heritage site) to fully urbanized areas. Considering that numerous studies show that natural spaces promote health by providing psychophysiological alleviation38,41,42,61, we believe that our results can generalize beyond the idiosyncrasy of the mapped territory. In addition, we update previous accounts by suggesting that even past experiences in natural environments might aid mental health under challenging circumstances. This is indicated by our finding that certain natural locations reflect past positive experiences when accompanied by lower negative affect (as measured during the Covid-19 lockdown with the self-report Dass21). Interestingly, our participants reported comparable scores in this self-report (Ms = 6.3, 4, and 3 in depression, anxiety, and stress; normal severity) than those found in another study with a similar population and testing period48.
Methodological considerations
Previous studies have already mapped feelings and judgments in space, for instance by combining public participation and Information System Mapping5,6,8,63. This approach usually collects participants’ commentaries and opinions about specific points in space. Although of considerable interest, the use of semantic reports might face various challenges for the statistical interpretation of the data, such as the uneven distribution of information within and between participants’ reports (e.g., semantically uncertain and abstract judgments about places)4. In this context, while our method does not consider individual accounts, it does not rely on the semantic variability and interpretation of participants’ reports. Altogether, we believe that our work offers one approach to better understand human behaviour in its ecological niche, which should be complemented with methods as the ones mentioned above. Relatedly, our work is inspired by the field of neuroimaging, which started developing in the 90s. From our standpoint, what we offer here is just a glimpse of this development, i.e., a standard and relatively simple way to analyse spatially distributed data (and correlates with questionnaires). Nevertheless, we hope that future experimental studies will generate novel findings by using this or more complete approaches that could stem from the crossing between different fields.
On a different note, the spatial resolution of our method can vary according to the scope and sampled territory. In our maps, we sampled the territory with relatively low spatial resolution. Specifically, each pixel represented approximately ~ 0.06 km2 of the real land (~ the size of a Polo field), whereas it is likely that mapping smaller environments could lead to resolutions of square dekameter or meter. In addition, our method can be easily implemented to sample human cognition in a range of spatially extended environments (e.g., museums, towns, cities, countries, natural reserves). By doing so, not only basic science but also more practical matters such as the design, development, and allocation of resources in spatial locations could be improved64.
Conclusions
Converging evidence indicates that people’s valuation of the environment is accomplished by running an internal model of the body in the world12,13. Such a model reflects people’s psychophysiological condition, based on past experiences and predicted needs12,13,14,15. Under this model, the main goal of the subjective valuation of the environment is the attainment of psychophysiological equilibrium24. To achieve this, people probably weigh bodily representations against value computations22,23, namely, how we feel and what the environment can provide. Yet, studies in social cartography usually neglect such evidence. Our study integrates what current studies in human psychophysiology often overlook (i.e., one’s ecological niche, beyond arbitrary stimuli in laboratory settings) with what studies in social cartography usually neglect (i.e., psychophysiological traits need to be accounted because they modulate peoples’ appraisals). Accordingly, we included in our study measures of affect and interoceptive sensibility. This offers a clearer and more complete understanding of human cognition in space.
Our new method and results show emotional memories and desired locations are consistently mapped in geographical space; that participants’ mapping correlates with their levels of stress, anxiety, and depression, as well as of interoceptive sensibility; and that natural environments relate more to positive memories, better psychological state, and interoceptive sensibility. We conclude that positive memories, desire to visit future locations (after the national Covid-19 lockdown), affect, and bodily sensibility are associated with discrete, yet partially overlapping, locations in space. Importantly, these locations were associated primarily with natural environments. Given that data was collected during a nationwide Covid-19 lockdown, our results support the idea that in ‘times of despair’ natural environments could favor psychological and physiological restoration65,66 by linking bodily representations to value computations13,22,23,67. Our results are also in line with contemporary models of allostasis by considering the affective, environmental, and bodily dimensions of human affect in topographical space. By revealing spatial maps of human cognition and linking these to individual differences, we provide the basis for the development of spatially contextualized markers of physical and mental disorders.
Methods
Participants
A total of 201 participants (69 males, 132 females, mean age = 32yo, age range = 18–66yo) completed the online study that included two self-report questionnaires and a novel geographical self-report task. Participants were recruited through opportunity sampling via the departments of Psychology of the University of the Balearic Islands, as well as through the local news. Ethical approval was granted by the Research Ethics Committee of the Balearic Islands. All procedures were conducted following the Declaration of Helsinki and informed consent was obtained from all participants. Participants for which questionnaire or demographic data were missing were excluded (33 of 234 participants, 14%).
Task procedure
All participants performed the whole task procedure during the Spanish national Covid-19 lockdown (15th March–21st June 2020; data acquisition: 13th–26th April). First, participants completed two self-report questionnaires through the online platform Paperform: the Depression, Anxiety, and Stress Scale (Dass2133,68) and the Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness (i.e. MAIA35,36)—by using validated Spanish versions of the questionnaires45,46. The Dass21 measures negative affect associated with dysphoria and self-deprecation, autonomic arousal and experience of anxious affect, irritability and agitation. The MAIA measures 8 subscales that include the capability to notice, attend, regulate, listen, connect, and trust bodily sensations, as well as to ignore sensations of pain or discomfort.
Second, participants completed a geographical self-report task in which they had to shade within the outline of four identical blank maps (1) where they live, (2) where they had past positive and (3) negative experiences (happy and sad memories), and (4) where they most looked forward to going after the national Covid-19 lockdown. To do this, the participants downloaded an image file that included four maps with the corresponding indications above each map. Then, they shaded the maps by using a freely available online editor according to the instructions (Supp. Materials Fig. 1). Finally, they attached the resulting image to the end of the online study. The maps did not contain pointers, the painting was dynamic, thus successive strokes on a region increased the opacity of the paint (achieved by selecting lower opacity of the painting tool). More opacity corresponded to greater emotional intensity and the diameter of the painting tool was ~ 16 pixels. Finished images were stored in matrices where the paint intensity ranged from 0 to 1. Each map was represented by 63,741 pixels.
Use of emotion categories and self-reports in the current task
As detailed above, in the first part of the study we asked people to assess their topographical space as a function of emotional memories. This process relies on accessing autobiographical memories associated with specific locations. When accessing autobiographical memories, it is common to assess past experiences by using emotional labels such as happiness and sadness28,29,30,31. Furthermore, we were interested in a general assessment of space using this kind of ‘general and basic’ emotions. At the implementation of a new method, these might provide more general information about space than complex emotions such as safety, envy, etc.
Aside from that, our study is based on the use of self-reports. The conclusions that can be drawn from the results need to be put into perspective. Self-reports provide abundant information but are also prone to measurement error69. For instance, participants might not be able to introspect or interpret the questions about interoceptive sensibility, or they could rate their affect better than it was during the data acquisition (i.e., desirability bias). In the context of the current study, further research should follow up self-reports with other methods5,63 across different timescales. In our case, for instance, by examining if desired locations in space were visited after the lockdown and/or using more objective measures of interoception.
Preprocessing of data and statistical analysis
Data were screened manually and excluded for anomalous painting behaviour (e.g. drawing symbols). The geographical maps were pre-processed using a MATLAB (version R2018a) pipeline that was developed in-house. The image file with the four painted maps was split into four different matrices. All areas outside the boundaries of the maps were masked, such that pixels outside of the map could be discarded. The participants’ maps were analysed in three different ways. First, subject-wise emotional maps were created by subtracting the maps in which participants reported where they have experienced happy and sad memories. This allowed creating a continuous scale of positive–negative values associated with human emotional valence in geographical space. The corresponding maps were subjected to min–max normalization and smoothing of the data via 2D Gaussian smoothing kernel (as implemented in Matlab; Imgaussfilt function; sigma 1). Next, the emotional-spatial maps was subjected to Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM1243) a statical technique used to examine brain activity recorded in functional neuroimaging experiments. In this context, we examined shaded pixels instead of voxels in the brain to quantify where in the maps and how strong different emotions relate to one’s geographical space (i.e. are locations consistently associated with particular emotional memories?); see Fig. 1C. Relatedly, it is worth noting that the current method is inspired by the observation/method that emotions can be mapped on the body by using a topographical self-report measure70,71,72.
Second, we examined whether certain areas of the emotional-spatial maps correlated with the participants’ scores in the Dass21 and MAIA self-report questionnaires (i.e. are geographical locations related to one’s affect?). To this aim, we subjected both the emotional-spatial maps and the scores of the questionnaires to SPM analysis. Also, we examined the properties of the areas revealed in the above analysis. Specifically, we estimated whether the areas revealed in the two previous analyses corresponded to natural or non-natural locations (i.e., do emotions and affect relate to specific environments?). To this end, we contrasted the subject-wise emotional-spatial maps, as well as the maps where participants reported where they wanted to go after the Spanish nationwide Covid-19 lockdown with publicly available governmental maps (Supp. Materials Fig. 2).
Then, we quantified the number of pixels overlapping with each environment and normalized these according to the total number of pixels existing in natural and non-natural environments. Non-natural areas comprised urban, crop and cattle raising areas, which are not usually available to the public and are artificially altered (therefore, non-natural areas could include patches of rural areas). The subsequent normalized proportions of natural and non-natural shaded pixels were subjected to repeated-measures ANOVA and follow-up with Paired Samples T-test, in case of deviation from normality Wilcoxon signed-rank was used. The corresponding data were plotted by using the Raincloud plots package in R73.
Third, we also estimated whether the participants lived in a more natural or non-natural location by computing the number of pixels in the participants living location that overlapped with these environments. Next, we examined if this index influenced how they related to past emotional experiences and their desire to visit further environments after the Covid-19 lockdown.
Data availability
We used in-house scripts to mask, smooth, and normalise the maps, as well as to compute the pixel quantification with overlapping environments. The code and anonymized data will be available upon publication in Open Science Framework (https://osf.io).
References
Byrne, P., Becker, S. & Burgess, N. Remembering the past and imagining the future: A neural model of spatial memory and imagery. Psychol. Rev. 114, 340–375 (2007).
Epstein, R. A., Patai, E. Z., Julian, J. B. & Spiers, H. J. The cognitive map in humans: Spatial navigation and beyond. Nat. Neurosci. 20, 1504–1513 (2017).
Nau, M., Navarro Schröder, T., Frey, M. & Doeller, C. F. Behavior-dependent directional tuning in the human visual-navigation network. Nat. Commun. 11, 1–13 (2020).
Hauthal, E. & Burghardt, D. Mapping space-related emotions out of user-generated photo metadata considering grammatical issues. Cartogr. J. 53, 78–90 (2016).
Pánek, J. Emotional maps: Participatory crowdsourcing of citizens’ perceptions of their urban environment. Cartogr. Perspect. 2018, 17–29 (2019).
Caragea, C., Squicciarini, A., Stehle, S., Neppalli, K. & Tapia, A. Mapping moods: Geo-mapped sentiment analysis during hurricane sandy. Proceedings of the 11th International Conference on Information Systems for Crisis Response and Management. 642–651 (2014).
Ruotolo, F., Claessen, M. H. G. & van der Ham, I. J. M. Putting emotions in routes: The influence of emotionally laden landmarks on spatial memory. Psychol. Res. 83, 1083–1095 (2019).
Kahila-Tani, M., Broberg, A., Kyttä, M. & Tyger, T. Let the citizens map—public participation GIS as a planning support system in the Helsinki master plan process. Plan. Pract. Res. 31, 195–214 (2016).
Tolman, E. C. Cognitive maps in rats and men. Psychol. Rev. 55, 189 (1948).
Arzy, S. & Dafni-Merom, A. Imagining and experiencing the self on cognitive maps. In The Cambridge Handbook of the Imagination Vol 311 (Cambridge University Press, 2020).
Bottini, R. & Doeller, C. F. Knowledge across reference frames: Cognitive maps and image spaces. Trends Cogn. Sci. 24, 606–619 (2020).
Barrett, L. F. The theory of constructed emotion: An active inference account of interoception and categorization. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 12, 1–23 (2017).
Quigley, K. S., Kanoski, S., Grill, W. M., Barrett, L. F. & Tsakiris, M. Functions of interoception: From energy regulation to experience of the self. Trends Neurosci. 44, 29–38 (2021).
Tsakiris, M. & Critchley, H. Interoception beyond homeostasis: Affect, cognition and mental health. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 371, 10 (2016).
Barrett, L. F., Quigley, K. S. & Hamilton, P. An active inference theory of allostasis and interoception in depression. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 371, 20 (2016).
Tsakiris, M., Tajadura-Jimenez, A. & Costantini, M. Just a heartbeat away from one’s body: Interoceptive sensitivity predicts malleability of body-representations. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 278, 2470–2476 (2011).
Mehling, W. Differentiating attention styles and regulatory aspects of self-reported interoceptive sensibility. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 371, 20 (2016).
Terasawa, Y., Moriguchi, Y., Tochizawa, S. & Umeda, S. Interoceptive sensitivity predicts sensitivity to the emotions of others. Cogn. Emot. 28, 1435–1448 (2014).
Zamariola, G., Vlemincx, E., Luminet, O. & Corneille, O. Relationship between interoceptive accuracy, interoceptive sensibility, and alexithymia. Pers. Individ. Dif. 125, 14–20 (2018).
Schuette, S. A., Zucker, N. L. & Smoski, M. J. Do interoceptive accuracy and interoceptive sensibility predict emotion regulation?. Psychol. Res. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-020-01369-2 (2020).
Palser, E. R. et al. Alexithymia mediates the relationship between interoceptive sensibility and anxiety. PLoS One 20, 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0203212 (2018).
Damasio, A. R. Descartes’ Error: Emotion, Rationality and the Human Brain (Avon Books, 1994).
Damasio, A. R. The Feeling of What Happens: Body and Emotion in the Making of Consciousness (Harcourt, 1999).
Madl, T., Chen, K., Montaldi, D. & Trappl, R. Computational cognitive models of spatial memory in navigation space: A review. Neural Netw. 65, 18–43 (2015).
Schulkin, J. & Sterling, P. Allostasis: A brain-centered, predictive mode of physiological regulation. Trends Neurosci. 42, 740–752 (2019).
Sterling, P. Homeostasis vs allostasis implications for brain function and mental disorders. JAMA Psychiatr. 71, 1192–1193 (2014).
Conway, M. A. & Rubin, D. C. The structure of autobiographical memory. In Theories of Memory 103–137 (Psychology Press, 2019).
Holland, A. C. & Kensinger, E. A. Emotion and autobiographical memory. Phys. Life Rev. 7, 88–131 (2010).
Suardi, A., Sotgiu, I., Costa, T., Cauda, F. & Rusconi, M. The neural correlates of happiness: A review of PET and fMRI studies using autobiographical recall methods. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 16, 383–392 (2016).
El Haj, M., Antoine, P. & Nandrino, J. L. More emotional facial expressions during episodic than during semantic autobiographical retrieval. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 16, 374–381 (2016).
Markowitsch, H. J., Vandekerckhove, M. M. P., Lanfermann, H. & Russ, M. O. Engagement of lateral and medial prefrontal areas in the ecphory of sad and happy autobiographical memories. Cortex 39, 643–665 (2003).
Buchanan, T. W. Retrieval of emotional memories. Psychol. Bull. 133, 761 (2007).
Lovibond, P. F. & Lovibond, S. H. The structure of negative emotional states: Comparison of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) with the Beck Depression and Anxiety Inventories. Behav. Res. Ther. 33, 335–343 (1995).
Henry, J. D. & Crawford, J. R. The short-form version of the Depression anxiety stress scales (DASS-21): Construct validity and normative data in a large non-clinical sample. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 44, 227–239 (2005).
Mehling, W. E. et al. The Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness (MAIA). PLoS One 7, 20 (2012).
Mehling, W. E., Acree, M., Stewart, A., Silas, J. & Jones, A. The multidimensional assessment of interoceptive awareness, version 2 (MAIA-2). PLoS One 13, 1–12 (2018).
Mennis, J., Mason, M. & Ambrus, A. Urban greenspace is associated with reduced psychological stress among adolescents: A Geographic Ecological Momentary Assessment (GEMA) analysis of activity space. Landsc. Urban Plan. 174, 1–9 (2018).
Kondo, M. C., Fluehr, J. M., McKeon, T. & Branas, C. C. Urban green space and its impact on human health. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 15, 20 (2018).
Bratman, G. N., Hamilton, J. P., Hahn, K. S., Daily, G. C. & Gross, J. J. Nature experience reduces rumination and subgenual prefrontal cortex activation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 112, 8567–8572 (2015).
Laumann, K., Gärling, T. & Stormak, K. M. Selective attention and heart rate responses to natural and urban environments. J. Environ. Psychol. 23, 125–134 (2003).
White, M. P. et al. Spending at least 120 minutes a week in nature is associated with good health and wellbeing. Sci. Rep. 9, 1–11 (2019).
White, M. P., Pahl, S., Wheeler, B. W., Depledge, M. H. & Fleming, L. E. Natural environments and subjective wellbeing: Different types of exposure are associated with different aspects of wellbeing. Health Place 45, 77–84 (2017).
Ashburner, J. et al. SPM12 Manual 2464 (Wellcome Centre for Neuroimaging, 2014).
Golledge, R. G. Cognitive maps. In Encyclopedia of Social Measurement (ed. Kempf-Leonard, K.) 329–339 (Elsevier, 2005). https://doi.org/10.1016/B0-12-369398-5/00341-8.
Daza, P., Novy, D. M., Stanley, M. A. & Averill, P. The depression anxiety stress scale-21: Spanish translation and validation with a Hispanic sample. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 24, 195–205 (2002).
Valenzuela-Moguillansky, C. & Reyes-Reyes, A. Psychometric properties of the multidimensional assessment of interoceptive awareness (MAIA) in a Chilean population. Front. Psychol. 6, 1–12 (2015).
Qiu, J. et al. A nationwide survey of psychological distress among Chinese people in the COVID-19 epidemic: Implications and policy recommendations. Gen. Psychiatry 33, 1–4 (2020).
Ozamiz-Etxebarria, N., Idoiaga Mondragon, N., Dosil Santamaría, M. & Picaza Gorrotxategi, M. Psychological symptoms during the two stages of lockdown in response to the COVID-19 outbreak: An investigation in a sample of Citizens in northern Spain. Front. Psychol. 11, 1–9 (2020).
Gidlow, C. J. et al. Where to put your best foot forward: Psycho-physiological responses to walking in natural and urban environments. J. Environ. Psychol. 45, 22–29 (2016).
Dudley, L. & Stevenson, R. J. Interoceptive awareness and its relationship to hippocampal dependent processes. Brain Cogn. 109, 26–33 (2016).
Stevenson, R. J., Francis, H. M., Oaten, M. J. & Schilt, R. Hippocampal dependent neuropsychological tests and their relationship to measures of cardiac and self-report interoception. Brain Cogn. 123, 23–29 (2018).
Götz, F. M., Stieger, S., Gosling, S. D., Potter, J. & Rentfrow, P. J. Physical topography is associated with human personality. Nat. Hum. Behav. 4, 1135–1144 (2020).
Ainley, V., Apps, M. A. J., Fotopoulou, A. & Tsakiris, M. ‘Bodily precision’: A predictive coding account of individual differences in interoceptive accuracy. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 371, 20 (2016).
Garfinkel, S. N., Seth, A. K., Barrett, A. B., Suzuki, K. & Critchley, H. D. Knowing your own heart: Distinguishing interoceptive accuracy from interoceptive awareness. Biol. Psychol. 104, 65–74 (2015).
Price, C. J. & Hooven, C. Interoceptive awareness skills for emotion regulation: Theory and approach of mindful awareness in body-oriented therapy (MABT). Front. Psychol. 9, 1–12 (2018).
Sowden, S., Brewer, R., Catmur, C. & Bird, G. The specificity of the link between alexithymia, interoception, and imitation. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 42, 1687–1692 (2016).
Bonaz, B. et al. Diseases, disorders, and comorbidities of interoception. Trends Neurosci. 44, 39–51 (2021).
White, M. P., Alcock, I., Wheeler, B. W. & Depledge, M. H. Would you be happier living in a greener urban area? A fixed-effects analysis of panel data. Psychol. Sci. 24, 920–928 (2013).
Dadvand, P. et al. Green spaces and cognitive development in primary schoolchildren. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 112, 7937–7942 (2015).
Scott, E. E. et al. The autonomic nervous system in its natural environment: Immersion in nature is associated with changes in heart rate and heart rate variability. Psychophysiology https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.13698 (2020).
Mitchell, R. & Popham, F. Effect of exposure to natural environment on health inequalities: An observational population study. Lancet 372, 1655–1660 (2008).
Barton, J. & Pretty, J. What is the best dose of nature and green exercise for improving mental health—a multi-study analysis. Environ. Sci. Technol. 44, 3947–3955 (2010).
Pánek, J. Mapping citizens’ emotions: Participatory planning support system in Olomouc, Czech Republic. J. Maps 15, 8–12 (2019).
McDonnell, M. J. & MacGregor-Fors, I. The ecological future of cities. Science (80–) 352, 936–938 (2016).
Mitchell, R. Is physical activity in natural environments better for mental health than physical activity in other environments?. Soc. Sci. Med. 91, 130–134 (2013).
Bowler, D. E., Buyung-Ali, L. M., Knight, T. M. & Pullin, A. S. A systematic review of evidence for the added benefits to health of exposure to natural environments. BMC Public Health 10, 456 (2010).
Paulus, M. P., Feinstein, J. S. & Khalsa, S. S. An active inference approach to interoceptive psychopathology. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 15, 97–122 (2019).
Osman, A. et al. The depression anxiety stress Scales—21 (DASS-21): Further examination of dimensions, scale reliability, and correlates. J. Clin. Psychol. 68, 1322–1338 (2012).
Dang, J., King, K. M. & Inzlicht, M. Why are self-report and behavioral measures weakly correlated?. Trends Cogn. Sci. 24, 267–269 (2020).
Nummenmaa, L., Glerean, E., Hari, R. & Hietanen, J. K. Bodily maps of emotions. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 111, 646–651 (2014).
Nummenmaa, L., Hari, R., Hietanen, J. K. & Glerean, E. Maps of subjective feelings. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 115, 9198–9203 (2018).
Palser, E. R. et al. Reduced differentiation of emotion-associated bodily sensations in autism. Autism https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361320987950 (2021).
Allen, M. et al. Raincloud plots: A multi-platform tool for robust data visualization. Wellcome Open Res. 4, 63 (2021).
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank all those who participated in this study, as well as to Sandra García and Blau García for their helpful feedback.
Funding
This work was supported by the Autonomous Community of the Balearic Islands (CAIB), Postdoctoral Grant Margalida Comas Ref PD/036/2019 and the Balearic Islands Health Research Institute (IdISBa), Primus Grant Ref PRI19/16 to Alejandro Galvez-Pol.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
A.G.-P.: conceptualization, investigation, methodology, software, data acquisition, visualization, formal analysis, writing original draft, project administration, funding acquisition. M.N.R: methodology, visualization, writing—review and editing. J.-M.K: methodology, software, visualization, writing—review and editing.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Galvez-Pol, A., Nadal, M. & Kilner, J.M. Emotional representations of space vary as a function of peoples’ affect and interoceptive sensibility. Sci Rep 11, 16150 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-95081-9
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-95081-9
This article is cited by
-
Exploring spatiotemporal changes in the multi-granularity emotions of people in the city: a case study of Nanchang, China
Computational Urban Science (2022)
Comments
By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.