Abstract
The mental workload of subjects was tested under different lighting conditions, with colour temperatures ranging from 3000 to 6500 K and illuminance ranging from 300 to 1000 lx. We used both psychological and physiological responses for evaluation. The former was based on NASA Task Load Index (NASA-TLX, NASA), and the latter was based on the electroencephalogram (EEG) P3b analysis of event-related potentials using the “oddball” paradigm experimental task. The results show that as illuminance increases, and the response time becomes longer with a colour temperature of 3000 K (P < 0.01). However, when the colour temperature is set at 6500 K, the response time becomes shorter as the illuminance increases (P < 0.01). P3b amplitudes were significantly affected by colour temperature (P = 0.009) and illuminance (P = 0.038) levels. The highest amplitudes occurred at 3000 K and 750 lx, which is consistent with the trend shown by the subjective scale. The data analysis of error rates is not significant. These results suggest that an office environment with a colour temperature of 3000 K and illumination of 750 lx, which exerts the lowest mental workload, is the most suitable for working. However, the interaction between colour temperature and illuminance in affecting the mental workload of participants is not clear. This work provides more appropriate lighting choices with colour temperature and illuminance to reduce people’s mental workload in office settings.
Introduction
The effects of lighting on health and productivity have raised the level of concern in recent years, as people are spending increasingly more time in artificial light conditions in their everyday lives. Research reports that artificial light has diverse and complex effects on humans, as it affects not only vision but also circadian rhythms1. Furthermore, artificial light also impacts migraines2, physiological activity levels3, mood4, etc.
Illuminance and colour temperature are two indicators that can be controlled by artificial lighting, and they also affect human physiology and psychology5,6. Illuminance is defined as the luminous flux per square metre, in units of lux (lx). The colour temperature of a lamp equals the thermal temperature that an idealized black body would be required to assume in order to produce the same colour as the lamp, in units of K7. Kruithof proposed a diagram that defined comfortable lighting conditions by combining illuminance and correlated colour temperature8. He indicated that illuminance and colour temperature have an interactive effect on comfort; thus, through the diagram, we could obtain comfortable illuminance ranges at different colour temperatures. Even though there is not enough evidence to support his study, the idea is regarded as pioneering in the field of lighting characteristics. Based on Kruithof’s diagram, much research has been carried out, which has been focused on adjusting the range of comfortable lighting conditions. Kakitsuba suggested a new diagram of comfortable lighting illuminance and colour temperature based on psychological and physiological responses9. The authors used this research method to further analyze the responses of different genders to the comfort of LED lighting10. In addition, as a physiological response, electroencephalogram (EEG) is often used to study the effect of light conditions on cognition11,12,13,14,15,16.
Mental workload is the amount of processing capacity that a participant needs to perform a task in a given time17. Attention and memory tasks were chosen as the cognitive tasks in the present study, given that attention is one of the most fundamental features of our conscious performance in daily life18,19. Among different aspects of attention, sustained attention tasks are closely related to office work and can induce brain electrical activity. Sustained attention involves the ability to maintain concentration on continuous tasks or information sources20,21. Thakral et al. and Lee et al. demonstrated that sustained attention influences neural activity in the parietal brain region22,23. An uncomfortable working environment can lead to cognitive fatigue and reduced work performance, thereby increasing the mental workload under conditions of sustained attention24.
The evaluation method of mental workload includes three aspects: subjective assessment, objective performance, and event-related potential (ERP) analysis. The NASA Task Load Index (NASA-TLX, NASA) is one of the most effective measures of perceived mental workload for subjective assessment. It provides a workload index ranging from 0 to 100 based on six related sources of workload: mental demand, physical demand, temporal demand, performance, effort, and frustration25. Thus the subject’s response time and error rate to complete the task are evaluated as objective performance. ERP refers to EEG voltage fluctuations related to physical or mental occurrences over time26. The P300 ERP is the maximum positive deflection of the EEG signal that occurs approximately 300 ms after the subject pays attention to the stimulation, including P3a and P3b components27. Amplitude (in µv) and latency (in ms) have received widespread attention as evaluations of mental workload28. There are many reports that have studied the relationship between P3b component and mental workload through the oddball paradigm29,30. It has been reported that the amplitude of P3b decreases with an increase in mental workload29,31,32, and its latency increases with an increase in mental workload30,33.
In previous studies, the effects of colour temperature on sustained attention have been measured using various tasks, such as word repetition34, the Chu Attention Test35, short-term memory recall36, the Paced Visual Serial Addition Task37, and the Numerical Verification Task38,39. It has been shown that lighting with warm-white colour temperature usually results in better cognitive performance. Chellappa demonstrated that higher colour temperature is associated with faster reaction times in attention-related tasks37. Kocaoğlu found that 4000 K significantly increases the number of errors, while 6500 K is more appropriate for university learning environments40. In addition, Min reported that higher illuminance conditions (700 lx) markedly led to lower parietal tonic EEG alpha activity and longer reaction times, than other illuminance conditions (150 lx)41. Low-level illuminance often contributes to higher performance appraisals and a better long-term memory rate42,43, while high-level illuminance tends to make people less sleepy and more energetic44.
However, there is little information in the existing reports about the effect of combinations of illuminance and colour temperature on mental workload. Therefore, we hypothesized that there exists an interaction between illumination and colour temperature that affects mental workload as well as lighting comfort. In addition, the effect of colour temperature and illuminance on mental workload may be nonlinear, but there is a suitable range for relatively low mental workload. The purpose of the present study was to investigate the effects of illuminance and colour temperature on mental workload by EEG and to find appropriate lighting conditions for office environments.
Results
Subjective assessments
The NASA-TLX scores of different lighting conditions are described in Fig. 1. The higher the score is, the greater the mental workload is45. The lowest score was obtained at the colour temperature of 3000 K and illuminance of 750 lx. However, there was no significant main effect result for either the illuminance or the colour temperature and the interaction effect between illuminance and colour temperatures was also not significant. A portion of the mean subjective scores are shown in Table 1.
NASA-TLX scores (mean ± SD) under different conditions of colour temperatures (3000 K/4000 K/6500 K) and illuminance (300 lx/750 lx/1000 lx) levels. There was no significant main effect result for either the illuminance or the colour temperature. The interaction effect between illuminance and colour temperatures was also not significant.
Behavioural performance
The average response time and the accuracy rate in the oddball experimental task were analysed. The accuracy rate was not significantly influenced by illuminance or colour temperature. Moreover, the response time was significantly influenced by the illuminance according to the min–max normalization scores. As shown in Fig. 2a,b, as illuminance increases, the response time becomes longer with a colour temperature of 3000 K (P < 0.01). However, when the colour temperature is set at 6500 K, the response time becomes shorter as illuminance increases (P < 0.01).
(a) The min–max–normalized response time of the experimental task under different illuminances at 3000 K. (b) The min–max–normalized response time of the experimental task under different illuminances at 6500 K. The upper diagram shows that the response times of 3000–300 lx and 6500–1000 lx were significantly lower than others (*P < 0.05; **P < 0.01).
Physiological measurements
The amplitude and latency of P3b with different combinations of colour temperature and illuminance were analysed. Latency was not significantly influenced by illuminance or colour temperature. There was no significant interaction between these factors regarding P3b latency. For P3b amplitude, the analysis of variance on some electrode sites is listed in Table 2. The results indicate that amplitude is significant for the colour temperature (F[2,22] = 5.102, P = 0.009 at CP4) and illuminance levels (F[2,22] = 3.478, P = 0.038 at P4). However, no interaction of colour temperature and illuminance levels was observed (F[4,44] = 2.501, P = 0.053).
Examples of P3b amplitudes with different colour temperatures under 750 lx illuminance are presented in Fig. 3, and the grand average waveforms of P3b at the CP4 electrode and the scalp topographic distributions are described in Fig. 4. In the case of 750 lx, the results show that the P3b amplitude at CP4 is obviously higher at 3000 K than at 4000 K (P = 0.047) or 6500 K (P = 0.001), meaning that the lowest mental workload occurs at 3000 K.
Grand average waveforms of P3b at the CP4 electrode and scalp topographic distributions with different colour temperatures (3000 K/4000 K/6500 K) under 750 lx illuminance. The shaded box shows a significant difference in P3b waves. And the circles at topographic maps represent the CP4 electrode when P3b occurred near 300 ms.
Examples of P3b amplitudes with different illuminance levels under 3000 K colour temperature are displayed in Fig. 5, and the grand average waveforms of P3b at the P4 electrode and the scalp topographic distributions are described in Fig. 6. In the case of 3000 K, the results show that the P3b amplitude at P4 is significantly higher at 750 lx than at 300 lx (P = 0.001) or 1000 lx (P = 0.001), meaning that the lowest mental workload occurs at 750 lx. The means ± SD of P3b amplitudes at 3000 K are listed in Table 1.
Grand average waveforms of P3b at the P4 electrode and scalp topographic distributions with different illuminance levels (300 lx/750 lx/1000 lx) under 3000 K colour temperature. The shaded box shows a significant difference in P3b waves. And the circles at topographic maps represent the P4 electrode when P3b occurred near 300 ms.
Discussion
The effect of lighting illuminance and colour temperature on mental workload was investigated in the present study by subjective assessment, behavioral performance, and EEG analysis. However, no significant interaction between illuminance and colour temperature on mental workload was found from all aspects of the data, as outlined in Table 2. These results are not consistent with the hypothesis that there would be an interaction between illumination and colour temperature on mental workload, as well as lighting comfort.
Neither illuminance level nor colour temperature shown a significant effect on NASA-TLX scores. The principal reason may be individual differences and the limited sample size.
As indicated in Fig. 2a,b, when the colour temperature is set at 3000 K, the response time will be delayed as the illuminance increases, which is consistent with the findings of a previous study41. Yan et al. also found that learning efficiency was higher at a 2700 K colour temperature and 300 lx illuminance46. However, the response time becomes shorter as the illuminance increases when the colour temperature is set at 6500 K. We consider 6500 K, which is called a “cool temperature”, to make people more alert and better able to concentrate when the illuminance improves. Chellappa found that light at 6500 K led to significantly faster reaction time in the sustained attention task37. Li Lan also reported that greater positive moods were expressed at 2000 lx, 6000 K and 300 lx, 3000 K compared to 2000 lx, 3000 K and 300 lx, 6000 K47.
For ERP analysis, a significant effect of light conditions on the P3b latency was not observed. The P3b latency represents the time of mental processing, and it is often increased when the classification of evoked stimuli becomes more difficult48. There was no change in the difficulty of the categorizing tasks that elicited stimuli in the present study. The P3b amplitude represents the amount of attention given to evoked stimuli49. Different light conditions affect people's ability to focus attention on visual tasks through their impact on the visual system50. And the amplitude of P3b decreases with an increase in mental workload48. As described in Figs. 3 and 5, P3b amplitudes are observably influenced by colour temperature and illuminance. When the illuminance is 750 lx, the highest amplitude occurs at 3000 K. Manav demonstrated that 2700 K was suggested for ‘relaxation’ and ‘saturation evaluation’51. This may indicate that approximately 3000 K is a low mental workload colour temperature. At a colour temperature of 3000 K, the highest amplitude occurs at 750 lx. In either an extremely bright or dark environment, we cannot concentrate and are unable to complete the memory task well. This is the same as the relationship between arousal and attention. The ability to focus attention effectively is nonlinear with arousal. It is roughly like an inverted U-shaped function52. Hancock also reported a similar inverted-U law53 and that environmental stressors can transform an individual’s adaptability from a stable state to an unstable state. Very low or very high task requirements will reduce adaptability, thereby leading to increased mental workload54.
Another purpose of the present study was to find more suitable lighting conditions for the office environment. As described in Table 1, through the analysis of EEG data, a combination of 3000 K colour temperature and 750 lx illumination produced the lowest mental workload. This is also consistent with the results of the subjective assessment.
Conclusion
Under different lighting conditions of colour temperature (3000 K/4000 K/6500 K) and illuminance (300 lx/750 lx/1000 lx), twelve postgraduates participated in sustained attention task experiments. In addition, NASA-TLX scores as psychological responses, accuracy and response time as behavioral performance, and EEG data as physiological responses were recorded continuously in the experiment. The results show that the combination of 3000 K colour temperature and 750 lx illumination, which requires the lowest mental workload, is better suited to office settings. However, no significant interaction between colour temperature and illumiance for mental workload was found.
This study included only for healthy student participants. Vision problems such as myopia, hyperopia, astigmatism, etc. may lead to deviations in test results. More studies with larger sample sizes are needed to test the findings reported here. In addition, compared with the background lighting conditions, the influence of the screen stimulus light on the experimental results may be more remarkable. The issue of how to set the screen lighting condition to minimize its impact on experimental results needs further research.
Methods
Subjects
Twelve postgraduates from Beihang University participated as subjects for this study. All the participants (male, age: 23–27 years, mean 24.3) were right-handed and possessed normal vision. Caffeinated drinks, medication, smoking, and exercise were prohibited before the experiment for one week. Meanwhile, all the participants reported that they slept between six and eight hours the night before the study. The experimental protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of Beihang University, and all methods were carried out in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations. All research activities adhered to the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki, and written informed consent was obtained from the participants before the experiment.
Cognitive task
A computerized task was chosen for this experiment because office workers under artificial lighting obtain the majority of their information from display terminals. The experimental task was a modified “oddball” paradigm controlled by E-prime software (Psychology Software Tools, Inc., Pittsburgh, PA) that placed demands on sustained attention and memory. In this task, the target stimulus was a five-digit number, and the non-target stimulus was a two-digit number. Each block contained 120 target stimuli and 480 non-target stimuli. Between every two target stimuli, there were approximately two to six non-target stimuli. The presentation time for each stimulus was 600 ms, and 3.5 s were allowed for responding stimulus. The task flow diagram is shown in Fig. 7. In the task flow, the start signal occurs in the middle of the computer screen. The ' + ' signal means you should focus, and the sequences of numbers take place in the center of the screen. The image used in the current study was 1280 × 1024 pixels.
Each experimental block corresponding to one illuminance and colour temperature combination took approximately 25 min. Subjects were instructed to memorize each target stimulus number as soon as possible. Then, they were required to enter the number on a form after it disappeared on the computer. If a non-target stimulus appeared, the participants were asked to identify it as such and to press the “Enter” button after it disappeared.
Procedure
The experimental office (dimensions: 10 m L × 4.5 m W × 3.0 m H) is shown in Fig. 8. The air temperature was approximately 25℃. Black curtains were used to exclude natural sunlight. The suspended ceiling was white. The walls were painted light white, and the floor was light grey brick. The lighting system was supplied by nine luminaires that were recessed in the suspended ceiling. The total number of fluorescent lamps (Philips TLD/36 W/830; Philips TLD/36 W/840; Philips TLD/36 W/865) was 27. Each of them was connected to dimmable electronic ballasts, which were regulated by a Light Master 100 lighting control system. The lighting system was designed to provide nine different lighting scenarios, which were numbered on a remote control. This lighting system could be adjusted to maintain constant illumination levels of 300, 750, and 1000 lx on the working surface, while colour temperature of 3000, 4000, and 6500 K could be created separately. Illuminance values were measured by an LMT PO1704 Luxmeter (0—99,999 lx, ± 0.01 lx), and colour temperature values were measured by a TES-136 colourimeter (0–9,999 K, ± 0.02 K). The lighting scenarios in this experiment were in random order to avoid any influence of the sequence. Each experiment consisted of nine trials of 25 min each, followed by breaks of 30 min i.e., enough time for rest and adaptation to the new illumination.
The target stimulus was presented on a PC (Lenovo) screen in front of the participant. In this task, all the participants were required to practice the task until they were skilled at it, or rather when their accuracy rate was 80% or more. The participants were required to focus, to do their best to complete the task as fast as possible, and to ensure the accuracy of their answers. A small reward was given for good performance. During the experiment, the participants’ response times and accuracy rates were recorded by E-prime.
All the participants had 30 min to adapt to the new environment. After that, the participants needed to accomplish the task block. Each subject should complete the NASA-TLX after each block, which was used to evaluate mental workload regarding participants’ subjective feelings. The experiments were conducted from 08:00 to 11:30 and from 14:00 to 18:30. The participants could finish the task in 30 min and rest for 30 min.
EEG data recording and processing
EEG was recorded by 32 Ag/AgCl electrodes with the international 10/20 system layout mounted in an elastic cap (Easycap, Germany). The signals were recorded by NeuroScan SynAmps amplifiers and Curry 7.0 software (Compumedics NeuroScan, Charlotte, NC), with a 1000 Hz sampling rate. The impedances of all electrodes were kept below 5 kΩ. The reference electrode was FCz, and the ground electrode was placed on the forehead55. Additional electrodes were placed at the outer left canthus and below the left eye to measure electrooculography (EOG) activity with a bipolar recording.
EEG data preprocessing was conducted by Curry 7.0 software. The EEG data were first re-referenced to an average of bilateral mastoids, and the bandpass filter was from 0.1 to 30 Hz. Epochs of 1000 ms were obtained for each trial starting 200 ms before the target stimulus onset and ending 800 ms after. A baseline correction was applied (baseline − 200 to 0 ms). A correction algorithm was applied to remove eye movement artefacts (e.g., eye blinks, muscle artefacts) 56.
P3b analysis
In the following we focused on the P3b component elicited by the target stimuli. P3b amplitude and latency were used to infer mental workload under different conditions of colour temperature and illumination. The mean amplitude and latency were calculated at electrode sites (C3, Cz, C4, CP3, CPz, CP4, P3, Pz, and P4).
Statistics analysis
All variables were first tested for normality with Shapiro–Wilk tests57, and all variables followed a normal distribution. Then, the data were analysed with a repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA), which included two within-subjects factors (illuminance: 300 lx/750 lx/1000 lx, colour temperature: 3000 K/4000 K/6500 K). The means were compared by Duncan’s test at a probability of 95%. Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS Statistics 17.0 (SPSS, Inc., Chicago, IL, USA). Because of limited numbers of participants, individual differences influenced the raw results. To reduce this influence, the min–max normalization data standardization method that transferred the value of data to a range of [0, 1]58 was used to analyse the NASA-TLX and behavioral performance data.
Data availability
The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.
References
Stevens, R. G., Blask, D. E., Brainard, G. C., Hansen, J., Lockley, S. W., Provencio, I., & Reinlib, L. Meeting report: The role of environmental lighting and circadian disruption in cancer and other diseases. Environ. Health Perspect. 115(9), 1357–1362. https://doi.org/10.1289/ehp.10200 (2007).
Boyce, P. R. The impact of light in buildings on human health. Indoor Built Environ. 19(1), 8–20. https://doi.org/10.1177/1420326X09358028 (2010).
Saito, Y. et al. Effect of bright light exposure on muscle sympathetic nerve activity in human. Neurosci. Lett. 219(2), 135–137. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3940(96)13171-2 (1996).
Fernandez, D. C. et al. Light affects mood and learning through distinct retina-brain pathways. Cell 175(1), 71–84. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2018.08.004 (2018).
Taniguchi, Y., Miki, M., Hiroyasu, T., & Yoshimi, M. Preferred illuminance and colour temperature in creative works. in 2011 IEEE International Conference on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics 3255–3260. https://doi.org/10.1109/ICSMC.2011.6084171 (IEEE, 2011).
Miki, M., Ikegam, H., Motoya, Y., & Fujimoto, S. Combination of preferred illuminance and preferred colour temperature for work. in 2013 IEEE International Conference on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics 4577–4582. https://doi.org/10.1109/SMC.2013.779 (IEEE, 2013).
Wessolowski, N., Koenig, H., Schulte-Markwort, M. & Barkmann, C. The effect of variable light on the fidgetiness and social behavior of pupils in school. J. Environ. Psychol. 39, 101–108. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2014.05.001 (2014).
Kruithof, A. A. Tubular luminescence lights for general illumination. Philips Tech. Rev. 6(3), 65–96 (1941).
Kakitsuba, N. Comfortable indoor lighting conditions evaluated from psychological and physiological responses. Leukos 12(3), 163–172. https://doi.org/10.1080/15502724.2015.1061945 (2016).
Kakitsuba, N. Comfortable indoor lighting conditions for LEDlights evaluated from psychological and physiological responses. Appl. Ergon. 82, 102941. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.apergo.2019.102941 (2020).
Ermolaev, R. Y. & Kleinman, D. The effect of background illumination on pattern onset visual evoked potentials. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 55(5), 546–556. https://doi.org/10.1016/0013-4694(83)90165-7 (1983).
Yagi, A., Imanishi, S. & Konishi, H. Brain potentials associated with eye fixations during visual tasks under different lighting systems. Ergonomics 41(5), 670–677. https://doi.org/10.1080/001401398186838 (1998).
Noguchi, H. & Sakaguchi, T. Effect of illuminance and colour temperature on lowering of physiological activity. Appl. Hum. Sci. 18(4), 117–123. https://doi.org/10.2114/jpa.18.117 (1999).
Maher, A. M. et al. Effect of luminance level on electro-encephalogram alpha-wave synchronisation. Med. Biol. Eng. Comput. 39(6), 672–677. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf02345440 (2001).
Shin, J. Y., Chun, S. Y., & Lee, C. S. Analysis of the effect on attention and relaxation level by correlated colour temperature and illuminance of LED lighting using EEG signal. J. Korean Inst. Illumin. Electr. Install. Eng. 27(5). https://doi.org/10.5207/JIEIE.2013.27.5.009 (2013).
Park, J. Y., Min, B. K., Jung, Y. C., Pak, H., Jeong, Y. H., & Kim, E. Illumination influences working memory: An EEG study. Neuroscience 247, 386–394. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.05.016 (2013).
Gopher, D., & Donchin, E. Workload: An examination of the concept. in Handbook of Perception and Human Performance. Cognitive Processes and Performance (Boff, K. R., Kaufman, L. & Thomas, J. P. Eds.). Vol. 2. 1–49. (1986).
Sohlberg, M. M., & Mateer, C. A. Introduction to Cognitive Rehabilitation: Theory and Practice. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(90)90117-7 (Guilford Press, 1989).
Sohlberg, M. M., & Mateer, C. A. (Eds.). Cognitive Rehabilitation: An Integrative Neuropsychological Approach. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awg003 (Guilford Press, 2001).
Cimprich, B. A theoretical perspective on attention and patient education. Adv. Nurs. Sci. 14(3), 39–51. https://doi.org/10.1097/00012272-199203000-00007 (1992).
Solso, R. L., MacLin, M. K., & MacLin, O. H. Cognitive Psychology, 7th edn. https://psycnet.apa.org/record/2004-19852-000 (Pearson Education New Zealand, 2005).
Thakral, P. P. & Slotnick, S. D. The role of parietal cortex during sustained visual spatial attention. Brain Res. 1302, 157–166. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2009.09.031 (2009).
Lee, J., Ku, J., Han, K., Park, J., Lee, H., Kim, K. R. & Kim, S. I. rTMS over bilateral inferior parietal cortex induces decrement of spatial sustained attention. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7, 26. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00026 (2013).
Subramanyam, M., Muralidhara, P., & Pooja, M. Mental workload and cognitive fatigue: A study. IUP J. Manag. Res. 12(2). https://doi.org/10.2478/BF02475910 (2013).
Finomore, V. S., Warm, J. S., Matthews, G., Riley, M. A., Dember, W. N., Shaw, T. H., & Scerbo, M. W. Measuring the workload of sustained attention. in Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting Vol. 50(16) 1614–1618. https://doi.org/10.1177/154193120605001621 (SAGE Publications, 2006).
Picton, T. W., Bentin, S., Berg, P., Donchin, E., Hillyard, S. A., Johnson Jr, R., & Taylor, M. J. Guidelines for using human event‐related potentials to study cognition: Recording standards and publication criteria. Psychophysiology 37(2), 127–152. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-8986.3720127 (2000).
de Tommaso, M., Betti, V., Bocci, T., Bolognini, N., Di Russo, F., Fattapposta, F., & Valeriani, M. Pearls and pitfalls in brain functional analysis by event-related potentials: A narrative review by the Italian Psychophysiology and Cognitive Neuroscience Society on methodological limits and clinical reliability—Part I. Neurol. Sci. 1–25 (2020).
Marucci, M., Di Flumeri, G., Borghini, G., Sciaraffa, N., Scandola, M., Pavone, E. F., & Aricò, P. The impact of multisensory integration and perceptual load in virtual reality settings on performance, workload and presence. Sci. Rep. 11(1), 1–15. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-84196-8 (2021).
Novak, G. P., Ritter, W., Vaughan, H. G. Jr. & Wiznitzer, M. L. Differentiation of negative event-related potentials in an auditory discrimination task. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 75(4), 255–275. https://doi.org/10.1016/0013-4694(90)90105-S (1990).
Horat, S. K. et al. Assessment of mental workload: A new electrophysiological method based on intra-block averaging of ERP amplitudes. Neuropsychologia 82, 11–17. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.12.013 (2016).
Ghani, U., Signal, N., Niazi, I. & Taylor, D. ERP based measures of cognitive workload: A review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.07.020 (2020).
Causse, M., Fabre, E., Giraudet, L., Gonzalez, M. & Peysakhovich, V. EEG/ERP as a measure of mental workload in a simple piloting task. Proc. Manuf. 3, 5230–5236. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.promfg.2015.07.594 (2015).
Gaál, Z. A., Csuhaj, R. & Molnár, M. Age-dependent changes of auditory evoked potentials—Effect of task difficulty. Biol. Psychol. 76(3), 196–208. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.07.009 (2007).
LaPointe, L. L., Maitland, C. G., Blanchard, A. A., Kemker, B. E., Stierwalt, J. A., & Heald, G. R. The effects of auditory distraction on visual cognitive performance in multiple sclerosis. J. Neuro-ophthalmol. 25(2), 92–94. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.wno.00001611660.91272.18 (2005).
Huang, R. H., Lee, L., Chiu, Y. A. & Sun, Y. Effects of correlated colour temperature on focused and sustained attention under white LED desk lighting. Colour Res. Appl. 40(3), 281–286. https://doi.org/10.1002/col.21885 (2015).
Knez, I. & Hygge, S. Irrelevant speech and indoor lighting: Effects on cognitive performance and self-reported affect. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 16(6), 709–718. https://doi.org/10.1006/jevp.2001.0222 (2002).
Chellappa, S. L. et al. Non-visual effects of light on melatonin, alertness and cognitive performance: Can blue-enriched light keep us alert?. PLoS ONE 6(1), e16429. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0016429 (2011).
Davis, R. G. & Garza, A. Task lighting for the elderly. J. Illum. Eng. Soc. 31(1), 20–32 (2002).
Yamagishi, M., Yamaba, K., Kubo, C., Nokura, K. & Nagata, M. Effects of LED lighting characteristics on visual performance of elderly people. Gerontechnology 7(2), 243. https://doi.org/10.4017/GT.2008.07.02.180.00 (2008).
Kocaoğlu, R. The Effects of Correlated Colour Temperature on Sustained Attention and Mood of University Students in Learning Environments (Doctoral Dissertation, Bilkent University, 2015).
Min, B. K., Jung, Y. C., Kim, E. & Park, J. Y. Bright illumination reduces parietal EEG alpha activity during a sustained attention task. Brain Res. 1538(22), 83–92. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2013.09.031 (2013).
Baron, R. A., Rea, M. S. & Daniels, S. G. Effects of indoor lighting (illuminance and spectral distribution) on the performance of cognitive tasks and interpersonal behaviors: The potential mediating role of positive affect. Motiv. Emot. 16(1), 1–33. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00996485 (1992).
Jung, H. C., Kim, J. H. & Lee, C. W. The effect of the illuminance of light emitting diode (LED) lamps on long-term memory. Displays 49, 1–5. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.displa.2017.05.001 (2017).
Smolders, K. C., De Kort, Y. A. & Cluitmans, P. J. M. A higher illuminance induces alertness even during office hours: Findings on subjective measures, task performance and heart rate measures. Physiol. Behav. 107(1), 7–16. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2012.04.028 (2012).
Hart, S. G., & Staveland, L. E. Development of NASA-TLX (Task Load Index): Results of empirical and theoretical research. in Advances in Psychology Vol. 52 139–183. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0166-4115(08)62386-9 (North-Holland, 1988).
Yong-hong, Y. A. N., Ning, Y. A. N., Yang, G. U. A. N., & Heng-zhi, Z. E. N. G. Impact on brain wave rhythm and learning efficiency by colour temperature of artificial light sources. J. Civ. Architect. Environ. Eng. 1, 76–79. https://doi.org/10.11835/j.issn.1674-4764.2012.01.015 (2015).
Lan, L., Hadji, S., Xia, L., & Lian, Z. The effects of light illuminance and correlated colour temperature on mood and creativity. in Building Simulation Vol. 14(3) 463–475. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12273-020-0652-z (Tsinghua University Press, 2021).
Käthner, I., Wriessnegger, S. C., Müller-Putz, G. R., Kübler, A. & Halder, S. Effects of mental workload and fatigue on the P300, alpha and theta band power during operation of an ERP (P300) brain–computer interface. Biol. Psychol. 102, 118–129. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.07.014 (2014).
Dyke, F. B., Leiker, A. M., Grand, K. F., Godwin, M. M., Thompson, A. G., Rietschel, J. C., & Miller, M. W. The efficacy of auditory probes in indexing cognitive workload is dependent on stimulus complexity. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 95(1), 56–62. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2014.12.008 (2015).
Lee, C. W., & Kim, J. H. The influence of LED lighting on attention and long-term memory. Int. J. Opt. https://doi.org/10.1155/2020/8652108 (2020).
Manav, B. An experimental study on the appraisal of the visual environment at offices in relation to colour temperature and illuminance. Build. Environ. 42(2), 979–983. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.buildenv.2005.10.022 (2007).
Purves, D. et al. Cognitive Neuroscience (Sinauer Associates, Inc., 2008).
Hancock, P. A. A dynamic model of stress and sustained attention. Hum. Factors 31(5), 519–537. https://doi.org/10.1177/001872088903100503 (1989).
Dehais, F., Lafont, A., Roy, R. & Fairclough, S. A neuroergonomics approach to mental workload, engagement and human performance. Front. Neurosci. 14, 268. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2020.00268 (2020).
Wu, J., Zhou, Q., Li, J., Kong, X. & Xiao, Y. Inhibition-related N2 and P3: Indicators of visually induced motion sickness (VIMS). Int. J. Ind. Ergon. 78, 102981. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ergon.2020.102981 (2020).
Gratton, G., Coles, M. G. & Donchin, E. A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 55(4), 468–484. https://doi.org/10.1016/0013-4694(83)90135-9 (1983).
Shapiro, S. S. & Wilk, M. B. An analysis of variance test for normality (complete samples). Biometrika 52(3/4), 591–611. https://doi.org/10.2307/2333709 (1965).
Hassler, A. P., Menasalvas, E., García-García, F. J., Rodríguez-Mañas, L., & Holzinger, A. Importance of medical data preprocessing in predictive modeling and risk factor discovery for the frailty syndrome. BMC Med. Inform. Decis. Mak. 19(1), 1–17. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12911-019-0747-6 (2019).
Acknowledgements
This study was supported by Grant-in-aid for scientific research from the National Natural Science Foundation of China (No. 11772038).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
X.S. designed the study, J.B. performed the data analysis and wrote the manuscript. Y.B. performed the debugging and measurement of experimental parameters and collected data. L.Y. and Q.X. revised the manuscript. All authors reviewed the manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Bao, J., Song, X., Li, Y. et al. Effect of lighting illuminance and colour temperature on mental workload in an office setting. Sci Rep 11, 15284 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-94795-0
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-94795-0
Comments
By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.