Abstract
Prospective momentary psychological and biological measures of real-time daily life stress experiences have been examined in several psychiatric disorders, but not in adults with an autism spectrum disorder (ASD). The current electronic self-monitoring study examined associations between momentary daily life stressors and (i) negative affect (NA; emotional stress reactivity) and (ii) cortisol levels (biological stress reactivity) in males and females with ASD (N = 50) and without ASD (N = 51). The Experience Sampling Method, including saliva sampling, was used to measure three types of daily life stress (activity-related, event-related, and social stress), NA, and cortisol. Multilevel regression analyses demonstrated significant interactions between group and stress (i.e., activity-related and event-related stress) in the model of NA, indicating stronger emotional stress reactivity in the ASD than in the control group. In the model of cortisol, none of the group × stress interactions were significant. Male/female sex had no moderating effect on either emotional or biological stress reactivity. In conclusion, adults with ASD showed a stronger emotional stress (but not cortisol) reactivity in response to unpleasant daily life events and activities. The findings highlight the feasibility of electronic self-monitoring in individuals with ASD, which may contribute to the development of more personalized stress-management approaches.
Introduction
Observational and experimental stress studies report increased emotional stress levels in adults with an autism spectrum disorder (ASD) with respect to controls1,2. However, there is no intensive time-series data on real-life, real-world momentary emotional stress reactivity derived from individuals with ASD. Emotional stress reactivity, defined as the effect of subjective appraisals of everyday stressors on negative affect (NA) can be studied via ecological momentary assessment (EMA) and has been used in a wide range of psychiatric disorders. For example, an increased emotional stress reactivity has been found in individuals at high risk for psychosis3, with psychotic illness4, and remitted bipolar disorder5 compared to the general population.
Evidence also shows that dysregulation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical (HPA) axis may play a role in the altered stress processing of individuals with ASD, indicating that the stress hormone cortisol may be disturbed. However, reports on cortisol outcome measures are not very consistent. Both increased6,7, decreased8,9, or equal10,11 cortisol responses to social stressors have been found in children and adolescents with ASD compared to non-ASD individuals. To our knowledge, only two experimental studies have investigated the cortisol response in adults with ASD; both studies found a comparable cortisol response to a social stressor in the ASD and control group1,12. Because of this inconsistent pattern and the artificial nature of laboratory settings, studying cortisol response in a naturalistic environment may shed new light on the relationship between stress and cortisol in ASD. In the past decades, EMA studies investigated biological stress reactivity by studying associations between minor daily life stressors and momentary cortisol levels. For example, an increased cortisol response associated with daily stressors was demonstrated in 556 females in the general population13. In addition, studies on psychiatric samples showed an increased cortisol response to daily stressors in participants with above-average risk for psychosis14 and a blunted cortisol response in participants with 22q11.2 deletion syndrome15 relative to controls.
Not only cortisol dysregulation per se is important, but also its role in the pathway to mood disturbance. Multiple lines of research have explored the pathways involved in the stress response; the most replicated finding in the literature is that cortisol mediates the association between stress and mood in humans16,17 or behavior in animals18,19. Although this pathway has not yet been explored in adults with ASD, studies using animal models in ASD (i.e., BTRB mice) showed evidence for a mediating effect of stress hormones in the behavioral response to stressors20,21. The current study will, for the first time, explore whether momentary cortisol mediates emotional stress reactivity in daily life.
In addition to the above-mentioned understudied themes, sex differences in emotional and biological stress response in individuals with ASD is another area of neglect. It is well-known that stress-related disorders (e.g., depression) are twice as prevalent in females than males22,23 and it also has been shown that the HPA axis is particularly influenced by female sex hormones24. Indeed, a recent observational study demonstrated higher levels of perceived stressful life events in adult females with ASD relative to males25. Of note, using EMA in patients with psychosis, an increased momentary emotional stress response (i.e., increased NA) to daily stress was found in females than in males26. This has led to the idea that the affective pathway to psychosis may be more dominant in females, whereas the developmental pathway to psychosis may be more prominent in males. Due to a neurobiological, phenomenological, and genetic overlap between the autism and psychosis spectrum27,28, greater emotional, and possibly biological, stress reactivity in females could be expected in individuals with ASD. Studying sex differences in momentary stress reactivity is pivotal as it may indicate a sex-dependent underlying vulnerability to develop mood and anxiety symptoms29,30, knowledge that is vital for the development of tailored-treatment.
This is the first study investigating momentary emotional and biological stress reactivity in the natural flow of daily life in adults with ASD. This was done with the Experience Sampling Method (ESM), an EMA tool. The ESM is a valid and reliable method31 in which short questionnaires about momentary experiences are presented to participants at random moments in time. This method is less susceptible to bias and has been applied to a wide range of psychiatric disorders32. Although ESM has only been used in a few studies on ASD, the feasibility and usefulness of this method have been supported in this population33,34. For the present study, we used three valid ESM stress measures35, i.e., activity-related stress, event-related stress, and social stress, to measure emotional stress reactivity3,36,37. In addition, to assess cortisol fluctuations during the day, momentary cortisol sampling was also integrated as previously described13,14,15.
Altogether, this study examined associations between momentary daily life stressors and (i) NA (emotional stress reactivity) and (ii) cortisol (biological stress reactivity) and compared these associations across groups and sex. It was hypothesized that adults with ASD would experience greater emotional and biological stress reactivity, particularly in females, relative to controls. Second, an exploratory analysis was done to investigate whether cortisol had a mediating effect on emotional stress reactivity.
Results
Sample characteristics
The total sample consisted of 50 adults with ASD and 51 controls (N = 101 participants). None of the participants were excluded. Sociodemographic and clinical characteristics are presented in Table 1. The groups did not differ on estimated IQ (p = 0.636) and sex (p = 0.918). However, the mean age was significantly higher in adults with ASD relative to controls (p = 0.028). Participants completed 7861 valid ESM reports. Adults with ASD filled out more ESM reports but the difference between groups was not significant (p = 0.116).
Group differences in ESM measures
The ASD group reported significantly higher levels of NA (B = 0.83, p < 0.001), activity-related (B = 0.61, p < 0.001), event-related (B = 0.09, p = 0.028), and social stress (B = 1.21, p < 0.001) than controls. There was no group difference in cortisol levels (B = 0.02, p = 0.760).
Group and sex differences in emotional and biological stress reactivity
Emotional stress reactivity
None of the three-way interactions were significant. As shown in Table 2, significant two-way interactions were found between group and activity-related stress or event-related stress in the model of NA. The simple slope analyses showed stronger positive associations between activity-related stress or event-related stress and NA in the ASD group relative to controls (Table 3, Fig. 1). No significant social stress × group interaction was found.
Biological stress reactivity
None of the two-way or three-way interactions reached significance (Table 4). To facilitate comparisons between studies, the estimated marginal means of the three stressors on cortisol in both the ASD and control group are presented in Supplementary Table S1.
Sensitivity analysis
All analyses were repeated within the new sample (ASD N = 41, controls N = 51), excluding participants with depression and the use of antipsychotics. The results remained similar for all the hypotheses (see Supplementary Tables S2 and S3) .
Exploratory analysis: the mediating effect of cortisol on stress and negative affect
Conditions for mediation were only met for event-related stress. Therefore, activity-related and social stress were excluded from further analyses. Results demonstrated a significant total effect of event-related stress on NA in the ASD group (B = 0.20, SE = 0.03, p < 0.001, CI [0.15, 0.25]), but no indirect effect (B < 0.01, SE < 0.01, p = 0.287, CI [− 0.00, 0.01]), i.e., cortisol did not mediate the association between event-related stress and NA. Moreover, there was a significant total effect of event-related stress on NA in controls (B = 0.12, SE = 0.02, p < 0.001, CI [0.08, 0.16]), but no indirect effect (B < 0.01, SE < 0.01, p = 0.057, CI [− 0.00, 0.01]). Thus, results showed a significant effect of event-related stress on NA in both groups but the association between event-related stress and NA was not mediated by cortisol levels.
Discussion
This study investigated momentary emotional and biological stress in the daily life of adults with and without ASD. A significantly stronger momentary stress reactivity in the ASD than in the control group was demonstrated, i.e., the associations between momentary NA and unpleasant events and daily activities in adults with ASD were significantly stronger than in controls, with no evidence for sex differences. Cortisol reactivity was not significantly stronger in the ASD group than in controls; associations between momentary cortisol and daily stress were neither dependent on group or sex. Lastly, no evidence for a mediating role of cortisol was found on emotional stress reactivity.
Group and sex differences in emotional and biological stress reactivity
Adults with ASD reported more daily life activity- and event-related stress compared to controls, with both of these real-life, real-world, stressors being more strongly associated with NA in ASD. These results bridge the gap in the literature by using ESM to measure momentary emotional stress reactivity in the natural flow of daily life, multiple times a day, for a longer period. Of note, previous observational and experimental research only investigated perceived stress by using retrospective traditional questionnaires, similarly demonstrating higher stress levels in children and adults with ASD1,6. The current study surprisingly showed that there was no group difference in the NA increase associated with social stress even though adults with ASD reported more often that they would rather be alone than the control group. An explanation for these findings may be that adults with ASD experience the same level of social support when in the company of others38,39, and social support may, therefore, be a protective factor against stress40. There was no moderating effect of sex on emotional stress reactivity levels in both groups, such as in a previous ESM study of psychotic disorder26. Based on the latter study, we hypothesized a greater emotional stress reactivity in females with ASD as there is overlap between the autism and psychosis spectrum. However, the current results imply a shared stress sensitivity between males and females with ASD, which could reflect the fact that ASD is primarily a neurodevelopmental disorder that may obscure stress-related sex differences. As increased stress reactivity is associated with the emergence of diverse psychiatric disorders, the current results may comply with the absence of significant sex differences in studies on psychiatric comorbidity in ASD41,42. Nonetheless, future studies should aim for larger sample sizes since the current sample was relatively small to investigate sex differences.
None of the associations between the stressors and momentary cortisol levels were significantly moderated by group or sex, which although unexpected, seems consistent with some of the laboratory research showing equal cortisol responses in adults with ASD and controls1,12. With respect to sex differences (as mentioned above), we cannot exclude the possibility that the absence of sex differences in cortisol reactivity may be due to a lack of power, especially since there is an increased body of evidence of sex differences in cortisol reactivity43.
As mentioned in the introduction, well-validated stressors were used and the current methodology has previously been applied to individuals with (increased risk for) psychotic disorder14,44 and with 22q11.2 deletion syndrome15. Still, it should be noted, that cortisol response to stress is relatively slow and begins to rise within minutes of the onset of a stressful event and peaks within 20 min, with a gradual decline to baseline levels over the next hour or longer45. In the current study, activity-related stress and social stress were measured ‘in the moment’ and these stressors were registered at maximum 10 min after the beep signal, which also applies to the cortisol sampling. Therefore, it might be argued that the cortisol sampling occurred too early to detect changes in cortisol levels. Adding a 15–25 min time-lag between the self-report measures and the cortisol sampling could have solved this issue. Nonetheless, a recent review demonstrated that ESM studies using a 25-min lagged saliva collection versus concurrent stress assessments was equally effective46. A possible explanation for these findings may be that the duration of real-life stressors widely varies and that participants are often unable to report exactly when a stressful situation started or ended47. Thus, although the timing of cortisol measures in relation to daily stressors may be imprecise, it is possible to assess associations between these variables47.
Event-related stress was assessed differently because participants were asked to report an event between the previous and present beep (with an average of 90 min between each beep). Even though it is difficult to measure the duration of real-life stressors (as described above), an unpleasant event may already have happened. Therefore, some of the cortisol peaks may have been missed.
Taken together, the field could benefit from more knowledge on maximum momentary stress-cortisol cross-correlations, as was proposed by Schlotz46. This type of research could also gain from technological development46. Especially, since it is expected that wearables are going to play an important role in the next coming years48, e.g., to measure cortisol levels via sweat49. This may be less burdensome to participants compared to salivary samples, enabling researchers to study stress-cortisol correlations more easily.
In sum, the current study demonstrated a stronger emotional, but a comparable biological, stress reactivity in adults with ASD compared to controls. Current findings are in line with experimental studies that have found a significantly increased emotional response and a comparable cortisol response to stress in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) relative to controls50,51. Because of the neurobiological and genetic overlap between ASD and ADHD52,53, the question may be raised whether the current findings could be explained by specific underlying mechanisms affecting stress experience and processing in individuals with neurodevelopmental disorders. Future transdiagnostic studies are needed to investigate this.
The mediating effect of cortisol on stress and negative affect
In contrast with our expectations, cortisol did not mediate the emotional stress response, or, in other words, cortisol did not mediate the association between event-related stress and NA. We have to be careful to interpret these findings since cortisol and NA were measured at the same time. As mentioned before, event-related stress may have already happened a bit longer before the beep, which may explain why this was the only stressor meeting the conditions for mediation. Although we did not find a mediating effect of cortisol on emotional stress reactivity, the feasibility and relevance to study momentary stress responses in a naturalistic environment has been shown.
Strengths and limitations
This is the first electronic self-monitoring study on momentary emotional and biological stress reactivity in the natural flow of daily life. The ESM, easily applicable via a mobile phone app, may have large potential for wide (clinical) usage in the autism community. Most of the participants gave positive feedback on the usage of the app and had no problems filling out the daily questionnaires. It may facilitate insight into contextualized stress experiences and other psychological experiences, shared-decision making, and enhance care-user empowerment. Another strength of this study is that multiple stressors were studied in a representative population, by including individuals using medication and with comorbid disorders. These individuals are often excluded to create a more homogeneous ASD sample and because medication may influence the cortisol response, although the generalizability of the results may become less since many adults with ASD receive some form of pharmacotherapy54 and they are more prone to develop comorbid disorders55. Therefore, we ran a sensitivity analysis excluding individuals with antipsychotic medication or current depression, which did not substantially impact the results. The ASD group was minimally treated to avoid major prior treatment effects on the stress response. Due to a relatively large number of participants and a sufficient number of self-reports and cortisol samples, it was possible to study in-group differences in cortisol levels. Nonetheless, an even larger sample would have yielded more power to the interaction analyses with sex and enable the study of subgroups because of the heterogeneity in ASD.
A well-validated social stress measure was used, which has been successfully applied in studies on individuals with depression56 and (clinical high risk for) psychosis3,57. Still, one may argue whether the preference to be alone is entirely indicative of social stress in ASD since it has been reported that children with elevated cortisol levels were more likely to engage with their peers58. However, another study showed that children with ASD that have the highest levels of cortisol show less social motivation10. Because of these contrasting findings, it may be interesting to further study the interplay between social motivation, social stress, and cortisol response in individuals with ASD.
It was a challenge to fit both the multilevel regression and the lower-level mediation models in the mediation analyses. Converge difficulties indicated that the models, with their complex random effects structure, may have been overfitted. However, solutions were found, and through a thorough comparison of different methods and programs, we are confident that the results are robust. Lastly, activity-related stress, social stress, cortisol, and NA were assessed at the same point in time. Hence, no direct causality can be inferred from these results. Thus, one could just as well assume that NA influences the subjective appraisal of activity-related stress, instead of the other way around. Either explanation, however, has clinical relevance.
Conclusions
With respect to controls, adults with ASD showed stronger associations between momentary NA and unpleasant daily life events and activities as measured in a naturalistic environment. The associations between momentary cortisol and daily life stress were not dependent on either group or sex. The results highlight the feasibility and relevance of electronic self-monitoring in individuals with ASD, which may contribute to the development of more personalized stress-management approaches.
Methods
Sample
The sample included 50 participants with an ASD diagnosis (N = 26 males, N = 24 females) and 51 adults without a developmental or psychiatric disorder (N = 26 males, N = 25 females) between 18 and 65 years of age. Participants with ASD were recruited by contacting mental healthcare facilities in the South of the Netherlands, through patient associations, and via social media. The first author (KL) conducted the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule II59 module 4 (fluent speech) in all participants of the ASD group to confirm their diagnoses. Only those participants with ASD who had (i) a short-term psychological treatment history (maximum 2 years) and (ii) no past psychiatric admission were included. Medication use and other psychiatric disorders were no cause for exclusion except in the case of acute psychotic symptoms, suicidal tendencies, or bipolar disorder. The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI)60 was used to assess the presence of psychiatric disorders in participants with ASD. The control group was recruited via social media. Participants were excluded if they had a first-degree family member diagnosed with, or suspected of having, ASD. The Autism Spectrum Quotient61 was used to identify the degree of ASD features in the control group; a score above 26 led to exclusion62. The MINI was also used to exclude any controls with a current psychiatric disorder. General exclusion criteria were (i) suffering from known genetic abnormalities, brain injury, epilepsy, or metabolic disorders, and (ii) an intelligence quotient (IQ) below 70. The latter was screened with two subtests (matrix reasoning and vocabulary) of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Fourth Edition63.
Procedure
This study was approved by the medical ethics committee of Maastricht University (NL51997.068.15) and was carried out in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki55. All participants were well informed about the study and gave written informed consent before the first appointment. Of note, we recruited a group of adults with ASD without a co-occurring intellectual disability. There were no concerns regarding their mental competence or decision-making capacity. Hence, they were capable to give written informed consent. During the first appointment, participants were screened for meeting the inclusion criteria. The ESM protocol and the collection of the salivary samples were explained in the following session.
The experience sampling method
As reported in another paper on this sample64, daily life assessments were done with the ESM, delivered via the PsyMate application. Participants received an iPod or downloaded the app on their smartphone. During 10 days, 10 times a day, the application sent an alert at random moments between 07:30 h and 22:30 h. Participants then answered questions about mood, social context, and activities, completing their reports within an allotment of 10 min after the signal. The questionnaire consisted of 7-point Likert scales to capture momentary experiences and categorical questions to capture context (e.g., social context, activities). Participants were encouraged to follow their daily routines. All participants were contacted by telephone after 2 days of sampling to ask if they experienced any problems concerning the protocol. It was also possible for them to contact the researchers if they had questions or experienced problems with the ESM data collection. Exclusion from the analysis followed in case less than 30% valid reports were acquired (30 out of 100), as previous work has shown that these data are less reliable65.
Cortisol sampling
In line with previous studies13,14,15 participants were asked to take a saliva sample (to measure cortisol) using cotton swabs within a maximum time-frame of 10 min after signaling of the PsyMate application. Thus, when there was a beep signal, the participant took a cotton salivette (from a plastic tube) and placed it in his/her mouth. After this, the cotton salivette was put back in the plastic tube, and the participants were asked to write down the time on the tube. All saliva samples were placed in a freezer at the home of the participant until the debriefing session. In most cases, the debriefing session was scheduled within a few days after the completion of the ESM protocol.
After collecting the data, participants were invited for a debriefing session and their experiences were evaluated.
Measures
Momentary stress
As described in a recent publication on this sample64, stress was conceptualized as subjectively appraised stress after regular daily life encounters or activities. Three different stress measures were obtained: activity-related, event-related, and social stress.
Activity-related stress was operationalized, starting with the question “What are you doing?”. Three items followed this question, i.e., “I would rather do something else”; “This is difficult for me” and “I can do this well”, reverse coded. These questions were scored on 7-point Likert scales (1 = not, 7 = very) and were combined into a mean activity-related stress variable.
Event-related stress was based on the question “What was the most important event since the last beep?”. Participants subsequently scored how pleasant/unpleasant the event was on a bipolar scale (− 3 very unpleasant, 0 neutral, + 3 very pleasant). Positive events (scores 1, 2, and 3) were recorded to zero, and negative scores were reverse coded (i.e., higher ratings reflect more stress).
Social stress was operationalized by asking participants if they were in the company of others or alone. If in the company of others, they were asked to rate the item “I would prefer to be alone” (1 = not, 7 = very).
Negative affect
The choice for the NA items in this study was guided by the extensive previous ESM literature using the same construct of NA (e.g.,57,66,67). These items were originally based on the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS)68. More specifically, affect was measured with the items (“I feel ….”) down, insecure, lonely, anxious, irritated, relaxed, enthusiastic, satisfied, and cheerful. All items were rated on 7-point Likert scales (1 = not, 7 = very). Factor analyses showed that these items loaded on two factors: NA and positive affect. The items down, insecure, lonely, anxious, and irritated loaded on the NA factor. Irritated, however, also had high cross-loadings on the positive affect factor. Therefore, the mean of the four items down, insecure, lonely, and anxious was used as a measure of NA in the analyses.
Momentary cortisol
After collection, the samples were stored in a freezer (− 20 °C) at Maastricht University. At a later stage, the samples were sent by courier to Dresden Lab Service GmbH (Dresden, Germany) to be assayed. Cortisol levels were determined in duplicate using a time-resolved immunoassay with a fluorescence detector69. Samples with cortisol > 44 nmol/L were excluded (N = 3) from the statistical analyses. Raw cortisol values were log-transformed to reduce the skewness of their distribution, generating the variable CORT.
Statistical analyses
All analyses were carried out in Stata version 13.170 and R71. ESM data have a multilevel structure. Therefore, two-level mixed-effects regression models (using the 'mixed' command in Stata) were used to analyze the ESM data, with observations (level 1) nested within-subjects (level 2). The independent variables, their interactions, and the covariates were entered into the models as fixed effects. Random intercepts and random slopes were added at the subject level, using an unstructured covariance matrix for the random effects. Models were fitted using restricted maximum likelihood estimation (REML). Fixed effects were tested via Wald-type tests with α = 0.05 (two-sided). As a first step, separate multilevel models were fitted to test whether the levels of NA, momentary stress, and CORT (dependent variables) differed between groups (independent variable; 0 = controls, 1 = ASD).
Group and sex differences in emotional and biological stress reactivity
Models were fitted for each type of appraised stress (activity-related, event-related, and social stress) as a continuous predictor and NA or CORT as the outcome variable. Age and lifetime depression (yes/no) were examined as covariates in all models, and oral contraceptive use (yes/no) was added in all models involving CORT. Time of the day ('hour') and its square ('hour2') were included as predictors in all analyses regarding cortisol to model the diurnal cortisol curve. The 'hour' variable was centered at 15:00 h to reduce collinearity with its squared value. Two-way (stress × group, stress × sex, group × sex) and three-way (stress × group × sex) interactions were used to test whether associations between stress and NA or CORT differed by group or sex. Based on each fitted model, we computed the slopes (of stress on NA or CORT) for all four groups with corresponding 95% confidence intervals (CIs). In case of a significant three-way interaction, pairwise differences were computed between the simple slopes to investigate the effect of both group and sex in the association between stress and NA or CORT. In case of only a significant two-way interaction, the simple slopes for the associations between stress and NA were calculated (command: margins). Regarding the sample size (N = 101), simulation papers demonstrated that 100 subjects were sufficient to investigate two-way interactions72,73, and with a minimum of 30 reports for each subject, it was possible to detect either small, medium, and large effect sizes73. However, it was expected that the current sample size yielded limited power to investigate a three-way interaction74.
Sensitivity analysis
To verify whether the results of the primary analyses were robust, we performed a sensitivity analysis. We excluded those with depression because depression is known to be associated with perceived stress75 and NA76. There is also evidence that medication use may impact the emotional and biological stress response77. A priori analyses on this sample demonstrated that antipsychotic medication was a significant covariate in the models of NA and CORT: antidepressants, anxiety, and insomnia medication were not. Therefore, those using antipsychotics were excluded from the analyses. This led to the exclusion of 9 participants from the ASD group (current depression n = 3, antipsychotic use n = 6), and none in the control group.
Exploratory analysis: the mediating effect of cortisol on stress and negative affect
First, it was explored whether conditions for mediation were met. That is, we verified, in separate regression models, that the independent variable (each stress measure), the mediator (CORT), and the dependent variable (NA) were all significantly associated with each other. When conditions for mediation were met, a lower-level mediation analysis to estimate the indirect (i.e., mediated), total, and the direct effect was carried out. We ran lower-level (i.e., within-person) mediation models78 using the lmer function from the R package lme479. Stress, NA, and CORT were centered at the person mean, removing all between-subject effects80, with hour and hour2 as covariates. The model included random intercepts and slopes for the three different paths of the mediation model, and error variances were allowed to differ across the equations for the mediator as an outcome and Y as an outcome; optimx method 'nmkb' was used as an optimizer. The bootmlm package (using vcov_vc) was used to get the covariance matrix for the random effects from which the covariance between the X→mediator and the mediator→Y path was extracted. Finally, random indirect and random direct effects were calculated from these estimates using the equations in Bauer et al.78.
Data availability
The datasets for this manuscript are not publicly available due to patient confidentiality and participant privacy. Requests to access the datasets should be directed to Machteld Marcelis, m.marcelis@maastrichtuniversity.nl.
References
Bishop-Fitzpatrick, L., Minshew, N. J., Mazefsky, C. A. & Eack, S. M. Perception of life as stressful, not biological response to stress, is associated with greater social disability in adults with autism spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 47, 1–16 (2017).
Hirvikoski, T. & Blomqvist, M. High self-perceived stress and poor coping in intellectually able adults with autism spectrum disorder. Autism 19, 752–757 (2015).
van der Steen, Y. et al. Clinical high risk for psychosis: The association between momentary stress, affective and psychotic symptoms. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 136, 63–73 (2017).
Myin-Germeys, I., van Os, J., Schwartz, J. E., Stone, A. A. & Delespaul, P. A. Emotional reactivity to daily life stress in psychosis. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 58, 1137–1144 (2001).
Havermans, R., Nicolson, N. A., Berkhof, J. & de Vries, M. W. Mood reactivity to daily events in patients with remitted bipolar disorder. Psychiatry Res. 179, 47–52 (2010).
Corbett, B. A., Muscatello, R. A. & Blain, S. D. Impact of sensory sensitivity on physiological stress response and novel peer interaction in children with and without autism spectrum disorder. Front. Neurosci. 10, 278 (2016).
Schupp, C. W., Simon, D. & Corbett, B. A. Cortisol responsivity differences in children with autism spectrum disorders during free and cooperative play. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 43, 2405–2417 (2013).
Hollocks, M. J., Howlin, P., Papadopoulos, A. S., Khondoker, M. & Simonoff, E. Differences in HPA-axis and heart rate responsiveness to psychosocial stress in children with autism spectrum disorders with and without co-morbid anxiety. Psychoneuroendocrinology 46, 32–45 (2014).
Levine, T. P. et al. Physiologic arousal to social stress in children with autism spectrum disorders: A pilot study. Res. Autism Spectr. Disord. 6, 177–183 (2012).
Corbett, B. A. et al. Biobehavioral profiles of arousal and social motivation in autism spectrum disorders. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 55, 924–934 (2014).
Corbett, B. A., Muscatello, R. A. & Baldinger, C. Comparing stress and arousal systems in response to different social contexts in children with ASD. Biol. Psychol. 140, 119–130 (2019).
Smeekens, I., Didden, R. & Verhoeven, E. Exploring the relationship of autonomic and endocrine activity with social functioning in adults with autism spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 45, 495–505 (2015).
Jacobs, N. et al. A momentary assessment study of the relationship between affective and adrenocortical stress responses in daily life. Biol. Psychol. 74, 60–66 (2007).
Collip, D. et al. Daily cortisol, stress reactivity and psychotic experiences in individuals at above average genetic risk for psychosis. Psychol. Med. 41, 2305–2315 (2011).
van Duin, E. D. A. et al. Lower cortisol levels and attenuated cortisol reactivity to daily-life stressors in adults with 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. Psychoneuroendocrinology 106, 85–94 (2019).
Oldehinkel, A. J. & Bouma, E. M. Sensitivity to the depressogenic effect of stress and HPA-axis reactivity in adolescence: A review of gender differences. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 1757–1770 (2011).
McEwen, B. S. Protection and damage from acute and chronic stress: Allostasis and allostatic overload and relevance to the pathophysiology of psychiatric disorders. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1032, 1–7 (2004).
Qin, D. D. et al. Prolonged secretion of cortisol as a possible mechanism underlying stress and depressive behaviour. Sci. Rep. 6, 1–9 (2016).
Cohen, H. et al. Blunted HPA axis response to stress influences susceptibility to posttraumatic stress response in rats. Biol. Psychiatry. 59, 1208–1218 (2006).
Frye, C. A. & Llaneza, D. C. Corticosteroid and neurosteroid dysregulation in an animal model of autism, BTBR mice. Physiol. Behav. 100, 264–267 (2010).
Benno, R., Smirnova, Y., Vera, S., Liggett, A. & Schanz, N. Exaggerated responses to stress in the BTBR T+tf/J mouse: An unusual behavioral phenotype. Behav. Brain Res. 197, 462–465 (2009).
Kessler, R. C., McGonagle, K. A., Swartz, M., Blazer, D. G. & Nelson, C. B. Sex and depression in the National Comorbidity Survey I: Lifetime prevalence, chronicity and recurrence. J. Affect. Disord. 29, 85–96 (1993).
Marcus, S. M. et al. Gender differences in depression: Findings from the STAR*D study. J. Affect. Disord. 87, 141–150 (2005).
Bangasser, D. A. et al. Sex differences in corticotropin-releasing factor receptor signaling and trafficking: Potential role in female vulnerability to stress-related psychopathology. Mol. Psychiatry. 15, 896–904 (2010).
McGillivray, J. A. & Evert, H. T. Exploring the effect of gender and age on stress and emotional distress in adults with autism spectrum disorder. Focus Autism Other Dev. Disabil. 33, 55–64 (2018).
Myin-Germeys, I., Krabbendam, L., Delespaul, P. & Van Os, J. Sex differences in emotional reactivity to daily life stress in psychosis. J. Clin. Psychiatry 65, 805–809 (2004).
King, B. H. & Lord, C. Is schizophrenia on the autism spectrum?. Brain Res. 1380, 34–41 (2011).
Owen, M. J. & O’Donovan, M. C. Schizophrenia and the neurodevelopmental continuum: Evidence from genomics. World Psychiatry 16, 227–235 (2017).
McLaughlin, K. A. et al. Childhood social environment, emotional reactivity to stress, and mood and anxiety disorders across the life course. Depress. Anxiety. 27, 1087–1094 (2010).
Susman, E. J., Dorn, L. D., Inoff-Germain, G., Nottelmann, E. D. & Chrousos, G. P. Cortisol reactivity, distress behavior, and behavioral and psychological problems in young adolescents: A longitudinal perspective. J. Res. Adolesc. 7, 81–105 (1997).
Csikszentmihalyi, M. & Larson, R. Flow and the Foundations of Positive Psychology 35–54 (Springer, 2014).
Myin-Germeys, I. et al. Experience sampling research in psychopathology: Opening the black box of daily life. Psychol. Med. 39, 1533–1547 (2009).
Chen, Y.-W., Bundy, A., Cordier, R. & Einfeld, S. Feasibility and usability of experience sampling methodology for capturing everyday experiences of individuals with autism spectrum disorders. Disabil. Health J. 7, 361–366 (2014).
Kovac, M., Mosner, M., Miller, S., Hanna, E. K. & Dichter, G. S. Experience sampling of positive affect in adolescents with autism: Feasibility and preliminary findings. Res. Autism Spectr. Disord. 29, 57–65 (2016).
Vaessen, T. et al. Stress assessment using experience sampling: convergent validity and clinical relevance. Stress Self-assess. Questionnaires Choice Appl. Limits, 21–35 (2015).
Myin-Germeys, I. et al. Emotional reactivity to daily life stress in psychosis and affective disorder: An experience sampling study. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 107, 124–131 (2003).
Reininghaus, U. et al. Stress sensitivity, aberrant salience, and threat anticipation in early psychosis: An experience sampling study. Schizophr. Bull. 42, 712–717 (2016).
Gantman, A., Kapp, S. K., Orenski, K. & Laugeson, E. A. Social skills training for young adults with high-functioning autism spectrum disorders: A randomized controlled pilot study. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 42, 1094–1103 (2012).
Renty, J. & Roeyers, H. Individual and marital adaptation in men with autism spectrum disorder and their spouses: The role of social support and coping strategies. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 37, 1247–1255 (2007).
Brailovskaia, J. et al. A cross-cultural study in Germany, Russia, and China: Are resilient and social supported students protected against depression, anxiety, and stress?. Psychol. Rep. 121, 265–281 (2018).
Lugnegård, T., Hallerbäck, M. U. & Gillberg, C. Psychiatric comorbidity in young adults with a clinical diagnosis of Asperger syndrome. Res. Develop. Disabil. 32, 1910–1917 (2011).
Lai, M.-C. et al. A behavioral comparison of male and female adults with high functioning autism spectrum conditions. PLoS ONE 6, 6 (2011).
Carpenter, T., Grecian, S. & Reynolds, R. Sex differences in early-life programming of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis in humans suggest increased vulnerability in females: A systematic review. J. Dev. Origins Health Dis. 8, 244–255 (2017).
Vaessen, T. et al. Overall cortisol, diurnal slope, and stress reactivity in psychosis: An experience sampling approach. Psychoneuroendocrinology 96, 61–68 (2018).
Schlotz, W. et al. Covariance between psychological and endocrine responses to pharmacological challenge and psychosocial stress: A question of timing. Psychosom. Med. 70, 787–796 (2008).
Schlotz, W. Investigating associations between momentary stress and cortisol in daily life: What have we learned so far?. Psychoneuroendocrinology 105, 105–116 (2019).
Nicolson, N. A. Handbook of Physiological Research Methods in Health Psychology 37–74 (Sage Publications, 2008).
Mohammed, T.-E., Christian, R., Brendan, O. F. & Paul, G. A review of wearable solutions for physiological and emotional monitoring for use by people with autism spectrum disorder and their caregivers. Sensors 18, 4271 (2018).
Sekar, M. et al. Review—Towards wearable sensor platforms for the electrochemical detection of cortisol. J. Electrochem. Soc. 167, 067508 (2020).
Lackschewitz, H., Hüther, G. & Kröner-Herwig, B. Physiological and psychological stress responses in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Psychoneuroendocrinology 33, 612–624 (2008).
Corominas-Roso, M. et al. Cortisol response to stress in adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 18, 1–10 (2015).
Martin, J. et al. Biological overlap of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and autism spectrum disorder: Evidence from copy number variants. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 53, 761–770 (2014).
Ronald, A., Simonoff, E., Kuntsi, J., Asherson, P. & Plomin, R. Evidence for overlapping genetic influences on autistic and ADHD behaviours in a community twin sample. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 49, 535–542 (2008).
Buck, T. R. et al. Psychiatric comorbidity and medication use in adults with autism spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 44, 3063–3071 (2014).
Joshi, G. et al. Psychiatric comorbidity and functioning in a clinically referred population of adults with autism spectrum disorders: A comparative study. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 43, 1314–1325 (2013).
van Winkel, M. et al. Daily life stress reactivity in remitted versus non-remitted depressed individuals. Eur. Psychiatry 30, 441–447 (2015).
Myin-Germeys, I. et al. Emotional reactivity to daily life stress in psychosis and affective disorder: An experience sampling study emotional reactivity. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 107, 124–131 (2003).
Corbett, B. A., Schupp, C. W., Simon, D., Ryan, N. & Mendoza, S. Elevated cortisol during play is associated with age and social engagement in children with autism. Mol. Autism 1, 13 (2010).
Lord, C. et al. In Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule Second Edition (ADOS-2) Manual (Part 1): Modules 1–4 (Western Psychological Services, 2012).
Lecrubier, Y. et al. The Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI). A short diagnostic structured interview: Reliability and validity according to the CIDI. Eur. Psychiatry 12, 224–231 (1997).
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J. & Clubley, E. The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): Evidence from asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, malesand females, scientists and mathematicians. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 31, 5–17 (2001).
Woodbury-Smith, M. R., Robinson, J., Wheelwright, S. & Baron-Cohen, S. Screening adults for Asperger syndrome using the AQ: A preliminary study of its diagnostic validity in clinical practice. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 35, 331–335 (2005).
Wechsler, D. In Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Fourth Edition (WAIS–IV). (The Psychological Corporation, 2008).
van der Linden, K., Simons, C., van Amelsvoort, T. & Marcelis, M. Lifetime and momentary psychotic experiences in adult males and females with an autism spectrum disorder. Front. Psychiatry 11, 766–777 (2020).
Delespaul, P. Assessing Schizophrenia in Daily Life: The Experience Sampling Method (Maastricht University, 1995).
Wichers, M., Lothmann, C., Simons, C. J., Nicolson, N. A. & Peeters, F. The dynamic interplay between negative and positive emotions in daily life predicts response to treatment in depression: A momentary assessment study. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 51, 206–222 (2012).
Kasanova, Z., Hajdúk, M., Thewissen, V. & Myin-Germeys, I. Temporal associations between sleep quality and paranoia across the paranoia continuum: An experience sampling study. J. Abnormal. Psychol. 129, 122–130 (2020).
Watson, D., Clark, L. A. & Tellegen, A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 54, 1063–1070 (1988).
Dressendörfer, R., Kirschbaum, C., Rohde, W., Stahl, F. & Strasburger, C. Synthesis of a cortisol-biotin conjugate and evaluation as a tracer in an immunoassay for salivary cortisol measurement. J. Steroid Biochem. Mol. Biol. 43, 683–692 (1992).
StataCorp. Release 13. Statistical software (StataCorp LP, 2013).
RCore, T. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. http://www.R-project. org (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, 2016).
Maas, C. J. M. & Hox, J. J. Sufficient sample sizes for multilevel modeling. Methodology 1, 86–92 (2005).
Arend, M. G. & Schäfer, T. Statistical power in two-level models: A tutorial based on Monte Carlo simulation. Psychol. methods 24, 1–19 (2019).
Dawson, J. F. & Richter, A. W. Probing three-way interactions in moderated multiple regression: Development and application of a slope difference test. J. Appl. Psychol. 91, 917–926 (2006).
Bergdahl, J. & Bergdahl, M. Perceived stress in adults: Prevalence and association of depression, anxiety and medication in a Swedish population. Stress Health 18, 235–241 (2002).
Forbes, E. E., Williamson, D. E., Ryan, N. D. & Dahl, R. E. Positive and negative affect in depression: Influence of sex and puberty. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1021, 341–347 (2004).
Granger, D. A., Hibel, L. C., Fortunato, C. K. & Kapelewski, C. H. Medication effects on salivary cortisol: Tactics and strategy to minimize impact in behavioral and developmental science. Psychoneuroendocrinology 34, 1437–1448 (2009).
Bauer, D. J., Preacher, K. J. & Gil, K. M. Conceptualizing and testing random indirect effects and moderated mediation in multilevel models: New procedures and recommendations. Psychol. Methods 11, 142–163 (2006).
Bates, D. et al. Linear mixed-effects models using Eigen and S4. R package version Vol. 1 (2014).
Bolger, N. & Laurenceau, J.-P. Intensive Longitudinal Methods: An Introduction to Diary and Experience Sampling Research (Guilford Press, 2013).
Acknowledgements
The authors would like to thank the participants, Centrum autisme GGzE, GGz Breburg, Apanta GGz, and “Nederlandse Vereniging voor autisme (NVA)”. Additionally, we would like to thank Jeroen van Oosterhout, Truda Driesen, Mario Meulendijks, Taco Hübner, Richard van Roy, Jeanine van der Meijden, Kees Spitters, Peggy Gevers, Debora Op ‘t Eijnde, Dafne Op ‘t Eijnde, Karel Borkelmans, and Ron Mengelers for their input, management, and assistance in data collection, analysis, and technical support.
Funding
This work was financially supported by Mental Health Institute Eindhoven (Geestelijke Gezondheidszorg Eindhoven; GGzE) and Maastricht University.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
K.L., C.S., T.A., and M.M. designed the study. K.L. and E.O. collected the data. C.S., W.V., and K.L. performed and interpreted the analyses. K.L. drafted the manuscript and prepared the Tables and Figures, C.S. provided support in drafting the method section. C.S., W.V., E.O., T.A., and M.M. critically reviewed the manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
van der Linden, K., Simons, C., Viechtbauer, W. et al. A momentary assessment study on emotional and biological stress in adult males and females with autism spectrum disorder. Sci Rep 11, 14160 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-93159-y
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-93159-y
Comments
By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.