Abstract
Behavioral automatization usually makes us more efficient and less error-prone, but may also foster dysfunctional behavior like alcohol abuse. Yet, it has remained unclear whether alcohol itself causes the shift from controlled to habitual behavior commonly observed in alcohol use disorder (AUD). We thus investigated how the acute and post-acute effects of binge drinking affect the automatization of motor response sequences and the execution of automated vs. controlled motor response sequences. N = 70 healthy young men performed a newly developed automatization paradigm once sober and once after binge drinking (half of them intoxicated and half of them hungover). While we found no significant effects of alcohol hangover, acute intoxication (~ 1.2 ‰) had two dissociable effects: Firstly, it impaired the automatization of complex motor response sequence execution. Secondly, it eliminated learning effects in response selection and pre-motor planning processes. The results suggest that alcohol hangover did not affect controlled or automated processes, and disprove the assumption that alcohol intoxication generally spares or facilitates motor response sequence automatization. As these effects could be specific to the investigated explicit learning context, acute intoxication might potentially still improve the execution of pre-existing automatisms and/or the implicit acquisition of motor response sequence automatisms.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
Binge drinking is a risky pattern of alcohol consumption observed worldwide, but is most prevalent in Europe, where it is most common among young adults (33.9% of all 20- to 24-year-olds)1. The US National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (NIAAA) defines binge drinking as consuming at least four drinks for females and five drinks for males within a few hours, resulting in blood alcohol concentrations (BAC) of at least 0.8 ‰2. Similarly, the European School Survey Project on Alcohol and Other Drugs (ESPAD) Group defines binge drinking as consuming at least five drinks on one or more occasions within a 30-day period, where one drink comprises of around twenty milliliters of ethanol3. Aside from the acute intoxication effect, binge drinking may also have post-acute effects, as it often results in alcohol hangover (starting at 0.00 ‰). Both of these effects are known to change cognition and behavior4,5,6,7,8,9,10,11,12. In particular, acute intoxication has been shown to detrimentally interfere with top-down cognitive control, while automatic behavior seems rather unaffected in comparison13,14,15,16,17.
It is commonly assumed that in case of regular binge drinking, this imbalance between decreased controlled and preserved habitual behavior may lead to increasing difficulties in controlling one’s drinking behavior18,19, eventually increasing the risk of developing an alcohol use disorder (AUD)20,21,22. Further supporting this assumption, AUD patients have been reported to show deficits in behavioral control23 and increases in behavioral automaticity24. Yet, these findings largely refer to consumption-related behavior, so that it has remained unclear whether alcohol-associated changes are based on a general underlying mechanism. Furthermore, it has remained unclear whether the imbalance between controlled and automated behavior is actually caused by alcohol consumption, or merely a premorbid/comorbid phenomenon found in AUD patients.
In order to investigate whether alcohol causes this behavioral shift, one needs to account for the acute effect of alcohol (i.e., intoxication), but also for its aftermath (i.e., hangover), as they represent distinct phenomena which might contribute differently to the overall consequences of excessive alcohol consumption. While intoxication is defined as the presence of alcohol in the body (typically measured via blood or breath concentrations), hangover is defined as “the combination of negative mental and physical symptoms which can be experienced after a single episode of alcohol consumption, starting when blood alcohol concentration (BAC) approaches zero”25. Further stressing the functional distinction between acute intoxication and hangover, previous studies have demonstrated that these two states do not always have the same effect on cognitive control26, and may potentially even produce opposing effects on processes like information accumulation for response selection27. Although there is only little research on whether and how post-acute binge drinking effects modulate the balance between goal-directed and automated processes, a recent study suggested that alcohol hangover may not modulate the interplay of controlled vs. automated response processes26. Likewise, hungover participants did not seem to alter their application of goal-directed and/or habitual learning strategies28. Yet still, alcohol hangover has been shown to impair cognitive control functions29,30,31,32,33, which could promote a relative advantage of response automatization as well as poor behavioral choices beyond acute intoxication, like the habitual continuation of drinking.
While single actions (like drinking a bottle of beer) may of course become habitual, it is much more likely that complex action sequences, which consist of several single actions (e.g., going to the fridge, opening the door, grabbing a drink, opening the bottle, and then finally consuming it), are eventually automated34. But while habitual action sequences play an important role in AUD and other dysfunctional behavior35,36,37, surprisingly little is known about how binge drinking, a factor known to increase AUD vulnerability38, affects the balance between the automated vs. controlled execution of action sequences39. Given the prevalent focus on directly addiction-related aspects of alcohol consumption in research, it has also rarely been investigated whether alcohol-unrelated action sequences are affected as well. Given that this would be needed to identify a general shift in cognitive functioning (rather than just proving altered consumption), it is important to focus on processes, which are involved in planning and execution of motor response sequences40. If the same motor response sequence is repeatedly executed and/or rewarded, increasingly stronger stimulus–response (S-R) associations are established, which ultimately results in the automatization of the given motor response sequence. Generally, the automatization of motor response sequences is a highly efficient way of prompting and organizing various behaviors41,42, but at the price that automatization leads to desensitization towards outcome devaluation43,44,45. The latter may give rise to dysfunctional behavior like excessive drinking, as behavior will then be maintained despite negative outcomes39,43,44,46. In the long run, this could contribute to the manifestation of dysfunctional behaviors, as seen in AUD. In order to investigate alcohol as a causal factor in the shift from controlled to habitual behavior, and to furthermore investigate whether this represents a shift in general cognitive functioning (i.e., independent of consumption-related behavior), we focused on how the acute and post-acute effects of binge drinking affect the acquisition of new automated motor response sequences and the execution of automated vs. controlled motor response processes in general.
To investigate these research questions, we developed a new paradigm that assesses the strength with which motor response sequences are automatized as the relative performance advantage over non-automatized (i.e., top-down controlled) performance. More specifically, this is achieved by comparing “control” task blocks to “automatization” task blocks. In the control blocks, motor response sequence generation requires top-down controlled processes due to the random generation of required response sequences. In automatization blocks, motor response sequence generation becomes automatized over the course of the experiment due to a high-frequency repetition of the same response sequence. We therefore expected that performance would be better in automatization blocks than in control blocks. Furthermore, this performance gap should increase from the first to the last task block (i.e., over the course of the experiment) as increasing motor response sequence automatization benefits behavior. In contrast to common motor sequence tasks (in which a single stimulus triggers the entire motor sequence), our paradigm investigates sequences of S-R associations, as a distinct stimulus triggers a single motor response. Eventually, multiple single motor responses form a motor response sequence. Based on previous findings of rather selective control deficits during acute and post-acute binge drinking12,13,15,16,29,30, we hypothesized that compared to sober performance, there should be alcohol-induced performance impairments in the control blocks, while performance should be relatively preserved in the automatization blocks. As a consequence, the performance gap between controlled and automated motor response sequence generation should be greater during acute intoxication and alcohol hangover than during sobriety. Additionally, the alcohol-related result pattern should be more pronounced during acute intoxication than during alcohol hangover. As behavioral measures reflecting overall performance, we assessed response accuracy and the duration of the entire response from stimulus onset to the last response (entire sequence duration / ESD). The latter was further subdivided into the time from stimulus onset to the first response to obtain a measure reflecting the cognitive processes involved in planning and selection of the motor response sequence (RT1) and into the time from the first to last motor response to obtain a measure reflecting processes involved in the coordination and execution of the motor response sequence (MSD). To this end, we subjected healthy young men to this paradigm in a mixed crossover study design, where each participant was either assigned to the alcohol hangover or the acute alcohol intoxication group. In both groups, we yielded for an average high-dose intoxication level of approximately 1.2 ‰. Both alcohol manipulation groups were experimentally intoxicated and tested twice in a counterbalanced order, that is once at a sober and once at an alcohol appointment (i.e., either intoxicated or hungover).
Taken together, we investigated how different effects of binge drinking, that is acute intoxication and alcohol hangover, affect the automatization of complex motor response sequences as well as the relative difference between controlled and automated motor response sequences. This is of particular importance as a potential imbalance towards increased automated and decreased controlled behavior during binge drinking could foster dysfunctional behavior in everyday life.
Results
Sample characteristics
N = 76 men were initially recruited and tested (mean age 23.2 ± 3.3 years old). Out of these, n = 6 had to be excluded from the analyses for the following reasons: One hangover group participant had a residual BrAC of 0.45 ‰ at the beginning of the hangover appointment and could not spend the required four to five hours waiting time until his BrAC had returned to zero. Three intoxication group participants had to be excluded due to poor performance during acute intoxication (accuracy below 50% in at least one of the four condition combinations). One intoxication group participant had to be excluded due to technical problems on the intoxication appointment, and another participant of that group had to be excluded due to very slow responses during intoxication (which resulted in the exclusion of more than 50% of the trials when applying the exclusion criteria described in the “Statistical analyses” section). Eventually, the data of n = 70 participants entered statistical analyses. N = 34 participants were in the intoxication group and n = 36 participants were in the hangover group. In both groups, n = 18 had their sober appointment before their alcohol appointment. N = 18 hangover group participants and n = 16 intoxication group participants had their alcohol appointment before their sober appointment. Table 1 provides statistical comparisons of sociodemographic characteristics and questionnaire scores between both alcohol groups as well as their group-specific alcohol-related data.
Comparability between the two samples
Given that previous studies suggested potential effects of drinking habits onto cognitive performance22,47, we wanted to make sure that the two experimental groups were indeed comparable despite some differences in their everyday drinking habits (AUDIT scores, please refer to Table 1 for details and the “Participants” section for the underlying reason). Therefore, we correlated the AUDIT score of all included participants with their behavioral performance for the entire sequence duration (ESD) measure and for the accuracy measure. None of these correlations reached significance (all r ≤ 0.190; p ≥ 0.115) and add-on Bayesian analyses provided moderate to strong evidence for the null hypothesis (no relevant correlation), as all BF01 > 3. We then also compared the behavioral performance at the sober appointment across alcohol manipulation groups using independent t-tests for the entire sequence duration (ESD) measure and Mann–Whitney-U tests for accuracy measure. We found no significant group differences for both the ESD (all t ≤ 1.759; all p ≥ 0.083) and accuracy (all U ≤ 682; all p ≥ 0.213). Additional Bayesian independent samples t-tests for all of these measures yielded BF01 > 1 for all behavioral parameters, thus showing that the null hypothesis (assumption of no group differences) was to be preferred over the alternative hypothesis in all cases. Taken together, this strongly suggests that drinking habits did not modulate performance and that the two groups did not perform differently at the sober (“baseline”) assessment. They should therefore be considered sufficiently comparable despite slightly different recruitment criteria.
For the hangover group, we additionally assessed subjective ratings of sleep duration, and hangover symptoms at both the sober and hangover appointment (fur further details, please see Supplement). The rating of overall hangover severity was assessed with a single item, which is considered to be a better and more accurate assessment than composite questionnaire scores48. It was significantly higher during the hungover appointment (3.75 ± 0.4) than during the sober appointment (0 ± 0) (Z = 5.108; p < 0.001), thus indicating a successful induction of alcohol hangover in this group. Of note, similar overall hangover severity scores have been reported in other experimental studies28,49,50, as well as in naturalistic studies where participants could drink ad libitum51,52,53,54.
Task effects (irrespective of alcohol)
To demonstrate that and how the newly developed task assesses (motor response sequence) automatization effects, the following paragraph exclusively reports main effects and interactions of the task manipulations (i.e., disregarding all main and interaction effects of alcohol manipulation group and alcohol administration). The accuracy and ESD data for the task effects is visualized in Fig. 1.
Regarding accuracy, there was a main effect of condition (F(1,68) = 59.475; p < 0.001; η2p = 0.467), which showed better performance in auto trials (94.1% ± 0.5) than in control trials (89.8% ± 0.6). This performance improvement in trials where motor response sequence automatization was possible clearly demonstrates beneficial automatization effects. In addition, the interaction between condition and block showed a trend towards significance (F(1,68) = 3.986; p = 0.050; η2p = 0.055). However, separate post hoc comparisons showed no significant differences between block 1 and block 5 in neither the control nor the auto condition (all t ≤ 1.910; all p ≥ 0.060). Given the lack of significance for the interaction effect as well as the lack of significant block effects in both conditions, we refrained from further post hoc analyses. The main effect of block was non-significant (F(1,68) = 1.177; p = 0.282).
The results of the mixed ANOVA for the ESD measure (i.e., the time interval between stimulus onset and last response in trials where all four responses were entered correctly and in the correct order) are summarized in Table 2. There was a main effect of condition, as ESDs were significantly shorter in auto trials (2228 ms ± 56) than in control trials (2797 ms ± 38). Like the condition effect found in the accuracy measure, this performance improvement in trials where motor response sequence automatization was possible clearly demonstrates beneficial automatization effects. Additionally, the main effect of block demonstrated a general learning effect, as ESDs were shorter in block 5 (2345 ms ± 44) than in block 1 (2681 ms ± 50). Furthermore, the interaction between condition and block reached significance (see Fig. 1). Separate post hoc comparisons for the two conditions showed that the participants got significantly faster from block 1 (2542 ms ± 63) to block 5 (1905 ms ± 62) in the auto condition (t(69) = 14.459; p < 0.001), but not in the control condition (t(69) = 1.151; p = 0.254). Thus, the learning effect (block 1 minus block 5) was significantly larger in the auto condition, where automatization of the entire motor sequence was possible over the course of the experiment (637 ms ± 44), than in the control condition, where automatization of the entire motor sequence was not possible (35 ms ± 31) (t(69) =|12.928|; p < 0.001). This demonstrates that as intended, effective automatization took place in the automatization condition, but not in the control condition.
Alcohol effects on the ESD measure
As the ESD reflected learning and automatization better than the accuracy measure in the task effect analyses, the alcohol-related analyses focused on the ESD. A graphic depiction of alcohol effects in each single condition and group can be found in the Supplementary Material.
In the ESD ANOVA, we found a main effect of alcohol manipulation group (see Table 2). As ESDs were longer in the intoxication group (2658 ms ± 64) than in the hangover group (2368 ms ± 62), and we had initially not found significant group differences in the sober assessment, this most likely evidences that performance was more strongly impaired by intoxication than by hangover. Additionally, significant effects were found for the interactions of alcohol administration x alcohol manipulation group, alcohol administration x condition x alcohol manipulation group, and alcohol administration x block x alcohol manipulation group. In the following two text sections, we separately report the post hoc findings of these two three-way interactions. To further investigate whether these specific alcohol effects observed for the ESD measure were more strongly based on response selection and planning, or on motor response execution, we repeated the four factor mixed ANOVA with the RT1 measure (i.e., the time passed between stimulus onset and the first motor response) and with the MSD measure (i.e., the time passed between the first and last response). In order to keep the results section concise, we only report the outcomes of the two three-way interactions of interest that were found to be relevant for the ESD measure.
Alcohol effects on the automated response execution of motor response sequences
For the interaction of alcohol administration x condition x alcohol manipulation group in the ESD measure (see Table 2 and Fig. 2), we conducted separate post hoc tests for each alcohol manipulation group. The post hoc ANOVA for the hangover group showed no significant interaction between alcohol administration and condition (F(1,35) = 0.917; p = 0.345). That is, alcohol hangover had no significant effect on the combination of task conditions, as compared to sobriety. In the post hoc ANOVA for the intoxication group, we obtained a significant interaction between alcohol administration and condition (F(1,33) = 6.930; p = 0.013; η2p = 0.174). A post hoc dependent samples test revealed that auto condition ESDs were longer at the intoxicated appointment (2549 ms ± 116) than at the sober appointment (2220 ms ± 89) (t(33) =|4.227|; p < 0.001). Control condition ESDs were also longer at the intoxication appointment (3003 ms ± 66) than at the sober appointment (2860 ms ± 68) (t(33) =|2.065|; p = 0.047). Yet, the size of the intoxication effect (sober minus intoxicated) was significantly larger in the auto condition (− 329 ms ± 78) than in the control condition (− 142 ms ± 69) (t(33) =|2.633|; p = 0.013). This demonstrates stronger intoxication-induced impairments in the auto condition (where automatization of the entire motor sequence was possible), than in the control condition (where this automatization was not possible). As a consequence thereof, we found that the automatization effect (control minus auto) had become significantly smaller during acute intoxication (454 ms ± 79) as compared to the sober appointment (640 ms ± 57) (t(33) =|2.633|; p = 0.013). To further explore the three-way interaction of administration x condition x alcohol manipulation group, we ran add-on independent samples tests that compared the two alcohol manipulation groups at the alcohol appointment (as these comparisons had not yielded any group differences for the sober condition, see section "Comparability between the two samples"). We observed longer ESDs in both auto and control condition at the intoxicated appointment (auto = 2549 ms ± 116; control = 3003 ms ± 66) compared to the hungover appointment (auto = 2022 ms ± 65; control = 2647 ms ± 60) (Uauto = 912; p < 0.001; Ucontrol = 914.5; p < 0.001). However, it should be noted that performance was always better in the auto condition than in the control condition (as indicated by the significant main effect of condition in the separate post hoc ANOVAs of both groups [Fhangover(1,35) = 246.339; p < 0.001; η2p = 0.876; Fintoxication(1,33) = 86.864; p < 0.001; η2p = 0.725]). This indicates that automatization took place in both alcohol states, even though acute intoxication was more detrimental to performance than being hungover.
Regarding the RT1 and MSD measure, the three-way interaction of alcohol administration x condition x alcohol manipulation group was non-significant in the RT1 ANOVA (F(1,68) = 0.992; p = 0.323), but significant in the MSD ANOVA (F(1,68) = 4.852; p = 0.031; η2p = 0.067) (please see Fig. 2). We conducted post hoc tests for this three-way interaction in the MSD measure in the same way as we had done for the ESD measure. Matching the ESD findings, there was no interaction of alcohol administration x condition in the hangover group (F(1,35) = 1.419; p = 0.242). In the post hoc ANOVA for the intoxication group, the interaction of alcohol administration x condition did not reach significance (F(1,33) = 3.337; p = 0.077). As this interaction effect was however significant in ESDs and showed a non-significant trend in MSDs, we performed an add-on Bayesian analysis as suggested by Masson55 in order to determine whether the null or alternative hypothesis of the intoxication effect was more likely. This yielded substantial positive evidence for the alternative hypothesis56, given the obtained data (PBIC(H1|D) = 93.1%; BF01 < 0.1). We thus decided to perform further post hoc analyses. Post hoc dependent samples tests revealed that MSDs were not significantly longer at the intoxicated appointment than at the sober appointment in either the auto or the control condition (Zauto =|1.547|; p = 0.122; tcontrol(33) =|1.438|; p = 0.160). The size of the intoxication effect (sober minus intoxicated) was not significantly larger in the auto condition (− 192 ms ± 91) than in the control condition (− 67 ms ± 46), but showed a trend (t(33)= |1.827|; p = 0.077), which suggests a trend towards slightly stronger intoxication-related impairments in the auto condition (where automatization of motor execution was possible) compared to the control condition (where it was not possible). While the automatization effect (control minus auto) was smaller during acute intoxication (184 ms ± 65) as compared to the sober appointment (310 ms ± 38) on the descriptive level, the Wilcoxon-signed rank test did not reach significance (Z = |1.325|; p = 0.185). Further add-on analyses comparing the two alcohol manipulation groups at the alcohol appointment showed no significant differences in MSDs for neither auto condition (U = 723; p = 0.192) nor control condition (t(68) =|1.951|; p = 0.055). However, it should be noted that performance was always better in the auto condition than in the control condition (as indicated by the main effect of condition in the separate post hoc ANOVAs of both groups [Fhangover(1,35) = 144.739; p < 0.001; η2p = 0.805; Fintoxication(1,33) = 36.691; p < 0.001; η2p = 0.526]), which indicates that the automatization of motor response execution still took place in both alcohol states.
Alcohol effects on response selection and planning-related learning
For the interaction of alcohol administration x block x alcohol manipulation group in the ESD measure (please see Table 2 and Fig. 3), we conducted separate post hoc tests for the two alcohol manipulation groups. While the interaction of alcohol administration x block was not significant in the post hoc ANOVA for the hangover group (F(1,35) = 1.757; p = 0.194), it was significant in the post hoc ANOVA for the intoxication group (F(1,33) = 10.074; p = 0.003; η2p = 0.234). Post hoc dependent sample tests showed that in block 5, ESDs were significantly longer during intoxication (2669 ms ± 89) than during sobriety (2343 ms ± 69) (t(33) =|5.548|; p < 0.001). This effect could not be found in block 1 (t(33) =|1.797|; p = 0.082). The size of the intoxication effect (sober minus intoxicated) was significantly larger in block 5 (− 326 ms ± 59) than in block 1 (− 145 ms ± 80) (t(33) =|3.174|; p = 0.003). Furthermore, we found that the learning effect (block 1 minus block 5) had become significantly smaller during acute intoxication (213 ms ± 58) as compared to the sober appointment (395 ms ± 56) (t(33) =|3.174|; p = 0.003). Taken together, this demonstrates intoxication- induced learning impairments that seemed to occur irrespective of automatization (as the factor of condition did not play any role for this interaction). To further investigate the interaction of administration x block x alcohol manipulation group, we ran add-on independent samples tests that compared the two alcohol manipulation groups at the alcohol appointment (as these comparisons had not yielded any group differences for the sober condition, see section "Comparability between the two samples"). For both blocks, we observed longer ESDs at the intoxicated appointment (block 1 = 2882 ms ± 91; block 5 = 2669 ms ± 89) as compared to the hungover appointment (block 1 = 2538 ms ± 69; block 5 = 2130 ms ± 52) (tblock1(68) =|3.021|; p = 0.004; Ublock5 = 1024; p < 0.001). In short, this indicates that intoxication impairs learning processes related to the generation of complex motor response sequences to a much stronger degree than hangover.
Regarding the RT1 and MSD measure, the three-way interaction of alcohol administration x block x alcohol manipulation group was non-significant in the MSD ANOVA (F(1,68) = 1.212; p = 0.275), but significant in the RT1 ANOVA (F(1,68) = 10.949; p = 0.002; η2p = 0.139) (please see Fig. 3). We conducted post hoc tests for this three-way interaction in the RT1 measure in the same way as we had done for the ESD measure. Matching the ESD findings, there was no interaction of alcohol administration x block in the hangover group (F(1,35) = 3.518; p = 0.069), while the post hoc ANOVA for the intoxication group revealed a significant interaction of alcohol administration x block (F(1,33) = 8.051; p = 0.008; η2p = 0.196). Post hoc dependent samples tests showed that in block 5, the RT1 was slower during intoxication (1398 ms ± 48) than during sobriety (1237 ms ± 39) (Z = |4.163|; p < 0.001). This effect could not be found in block 1 (t(33) =|1.325|; p = 0.194). The size of the intoxication effect (sober minus intoxicated) was significantly larger in block 5 (161 ms ± 32) than in block 1 (45 ms ± 34) (t(33) =|2.837|; p = 0.008). Furthermore, we found that the learning effect (block 1 minus block 5) had become significantly smaller during acute intoxication (4 ms ± 31) as compared to the sober appointment (119 ms ± 32) (t(33) =|2.837|; p = 0.008). This demonstrates intoxication-induced learning impairments that seemed to occur irrespective of automatization. Add-on analyses of alcohol manipulation group comparisons at the alcohol appointment yielded slower RT1s in both block 1 and block 5 at the intoxicated appointment (block 1 = 1402 ms ± 43; block 5 = 1398 ms ± 48) compared to the hungover appointment (block 1 = 1231 ms ± 31; block 5 = 1086 ms ± 27) (tblock1(68) =|3.228|; p = 0.002; tblock5(68) =|5.743|; p < 0.001). Taken together, this indicates that acute intoxication seems to eradicate any learning effects with respect to response selection and planning.
Summary of alcohol effects
To summarize, we did not find a significant interaction of all included factors. Instead, we found two different, seemingly independent alcohol-induced effects, which seem to arise from different sub-processes. Firstly, we found that alcohol intoxication affected the automatization of a complex motor response sequence execution to a higher degree than alcohol hangover, which did not differ from the sober state. The fact that this ESD effect was only reflected by the MSD measure suggests that it mainly arose from intoxication-associated automatization deficits during motor response sequence execution. Secondly, we found that unlike sober and hungover participants, intoxicated participants did not display a learning effect. The fact that this ESD effect was only reflected by the RT1 measure suggests that it mainly arose from intoxication-associated learning deficits in response selection and pre-motor planning of the motor response sequence.
Discussion
Alcohol-related cognitive control deficits have been widely researched, but still, rather little is known about how alcohol affects the establishment of automatisms and the balance between goal-directed and habitual behavior. This is however all the more important as an imbalance towards decreased control and increased habitual actions fosters the development and maintenance of an alcohol use disorder (AUD). In line with this, it has also remained fairly unclear how the different effects of binge drinking (e.g., alcohol intoxication and alcohol hangover) affect the balance between controlled and automated (pre-)motor processes, especially with respect to the latter. Specifically, it has remained unclear how these two alcohol-induced states affect the acquisition of new automatisms, even though a faster/more pronounced acquisition of automatisms may potentially contribute to (relative) control deficits observed during alcohol intoxication and hangover. Therefore, the aim of this study was to investigate acute and post-acute alcohol effects on the controlled vs. automatized planning and execution of complex motor response sequences in healthy young males. For this, we used a mixed study design in which each participant was either assigned to the intoxication group or the hangover group and completed a newly developed paradigm once sober and once at an alcohol appointment. Of note, the motor response sequences in this paradigm reflected sequences of S-R associations, as single stimuli were used to trigger each single motor responses within the motor response sequence.
Task effects
Regarding the newly developed task itself, we investigated the emergence of automated processes. This was done by comparing situations that require top-down controlled motor response sequence generation to situations where motor response sequence generation could become automatized based on the repeated presentation of the same stimulus array (and thus the requirement to repeatedly perform the same motor response sequence) over the course of the experiment. When motor response sequence automatization was possible, participants could rely on less cognitively effortful, bottom up motor response generation in order to execute the correct motor response sequence. We expected that this automatization should be increasingly established with increasing repetition (automatization effect). In contrast to this, participants had to rely on top-down cognitive control in order to execute the correct response whenever motor response sequence automatization was not possible (i.e., when the required motor response sequence was not predictable). We therefore hypothesized that complex motor response sequence performance should be more accurate and faster in the auto condition (i.e., when automatization was possible) than in the control condition (i.e., when automatization was not possible). We furthermore hypothesized that this performance gap should increase from block 1 to block 5 (i.e., over the course of the experiment) due to an increasing degree of automatization in the auto condition.
These hypotheses were fully confirmed by the interaction between condition and block in the ESD measure. This measure reflects the duration of the entire process, i.e., from stimulus recognition to response selection and then to motor response sequence execution. Specifically, task performance was faster in situations when automatization of the motor response sequence was possible compared to situations when automatization of motor response sequences was not possible. This automatization effect was significantly larger in the last block, as compared to the first block of the experiment. Importantly, this demonstrates that motor response sequence generation was increasingly automated with increasing repetition. We therefore concluded that the newly developed task does in fact allow to investigate the automatization of complex motor response sequences as compared to their top-down execution.
Regarding accuracy, the hypothesized result pattern was only partly obtained: We found more accurate performance when motor response sequence automatization was possible as compared to when it was not possible. However, this automatization effect did not increase over the course of the experiment. As the accuracy was already quite high in the first auto block (around 94%), it is however possible that a potential ceiling effect prevented further significant improvement from there to the last automated block at the end of the task. Irrespective of whether this was indeed the underlying reason for the lack of increasing automatization effects over the course of the experiment, this clearly shows that accuracy does not reflect the predicted automatization of complex motor response sequences as well as the ESD measure.
In conclusion, the ESD showed to be the more sensitive and suitable measure to investigate automatization processes of complex motor response sequences in this study. Therefore, we limited the alcohol-related analyses to the ESD measures and included motor sequence duration (MSD; i.e., the time elapsed between first and last response) and first motor response time (RT1; i.e., the time elapsed between stimulus onset and first motor response) in follow-up analyses to further clarify the origin of the observed ESD effects. This will be detailed in the following sections.
Alcohol effects
Based on previous findings that controlled processes seem to be more impaired by alcohol than automated ones13,15,16,17, we had expected that alcohol should have a (more) detrimental effect on controlled motor response sequence generation and execution, while automatized motor response sequence generation and execution should remain (largely) preserved (see Fig. 4). We had further hypothesized that these effects should be more pronounced towards the end of the experiment, when automated processes should become more consolidated. Additionally, we had expected that acute intoxication should have a greater impact on controlled motor response sequence generation than alcohol hangover. This would have shown in the interaction of all four factors (i.e., alcohol administration, alcohol manipulation group, task condition, and block number). However, we did not find this four-way interaction to be significant. Instead, we observed that alcohol impaired motor response sequences in two different ways, which was shown in two separate three-way interactions. On the one hand, we found alcohol effects on the automatization of motor response execution, and on the other hand, we found alcohol effects on the learning of response selection and pre-motor planning.
Alcohol effects on the automated response execution of motor response sequences
Partly opposing our initial hypotheses, we found stronger intoxication-related impairments when motor response sequence automatization was possible as compared to when it was not possible. Alcohol hangover had no impact on automated or controlled processes (as compared to sobriety). The automatization effect itself (i.e., the difference between control and auto condition performance) was still present in both groups and at both appointments (i.e., sober and alcohol), which indicates that the automatization of complex motor response sequences still took place in both alcohol states. In other words, the formation and benefit of new habits/automatisms was still possible under the influence of alcohol, albeit at a generally lower level during acute intoxication. The fact that we found this ESD result pattern to be more likely to be reflected in the MSD measure (than in the RT1 measure) suggests that these intoxication-related impairments mainly arose from deficits in processes related to motor response sequence execution (but not to the processes associated with selecting and planning the required motor responses). That is, intoxication was more detrimental to the automatization of motor response sequence execution than being sober or hungover. In comparison to this, controlled motor response sequence execution seemed to be less affected by alcohol, even though performance was still worse than during the automatization condition.
At first glance, these intoxication effects might seem to contradict previous studies, which mainly reported stronger impairments in controlled behavior, than in automatic behavior13,15,16. Yet, differences in the investigated functional domains may provide an explanation when taking a closer look: Other than previously used paradigms like the Simon No-Go task13 or the stimulus–response-binding task15, the task we used in the current study did not rely on already existing automatisms, like the tendency to respond on the side where a stimulus occurs, or the tendency to automatically associate events and responses that co-occur. Instead, the task used in this study required to form new, explicit automatisms as the task proceeded. It is thus possible that the different findings of intoxication effects on automated processes could be attributed to differently affected functional domains: The explicit acquisition of new habits or automatisms may be impaired by intoxication as shown in our study, while the execution of pre-established and/or partly implicit automatisms may remain relatively stable13,15. In line with our results, Obst and colleagues (2018) showed that alcohol impaired habitual decision making in a two-step task, instead of promoting it57. They further demonstrated a shift away from habitual behavior and towards goal-directed behavior in intermediate-risk drinkers as compared to low-risk drinkers, which further stresses the fact that there may be certain conditions under which alcohol impairs the habitualization of behavior and instead promotes top-down controlled response selection.
The lack of significant hangover effects is in line with previous findings investigating the potential effect of hangover on the interplay between controlled and automated processes26, where participants showed no differential effects on automated vs. controlled processes. Likewise, alcohol hangover did not affect the balance between model-based or model-free behavior28. Further evidence on how alcohol hangover affects automated processes or motor response sequence execution is however still rather rare.
In summary, alcohol hangover did not differentially affect controlled vs. automated processes, while acute intoxication likely impaired the automatization of complex motor response sequence execution to a stronger degree than the top-down execution of motor response sequences. In this context, it should however be noted that while automatization effects were reduced in size, they were still evident during acute intoxication.
Alcohol effects on response selection and planning-related learning
Interestingly, we furthermore found intoxication-induced learning impairments that seemed to be independent from the aforementioned automatization deficits. Specifically, acutely intoxicated participants failed to show any learning effects (i.e., behavioral performance improvements from block 1 to block 5). While we had expected an intoxication-related reduction in the learning effect, this did however not interact with automatization, thus suggesting that the effects of acute alcohol intoxication onto learning and automatization might not be mediated via the same mechanisms. The fact that this ESD result pattern was reflected in the RT1 measure (but not in the MSD measure) suggests that the intoxication-induced learning deficits most likely arose from impairments in response selection and planning (but not in motor response sequence execution). That is, acute intoxication was detrimental to the learning effect arising from the repeated planning of complex motor response sequences, while alcohol hangover did not affect these learning processes. This finding is partly supported by a study that reported impaired motor preparation of 3-key responses in hazardous drinkers as compared to healthy controls58. Importantly, both these results as well as our observations in healthy young participants demonstrated alcohol-related learning deficits in pre-motor planning. Taken together, this suggests that alcohol is a causal factor for deficits in planning complex motor response sequences. Yet, motor response planning and associated learning effects can take place both explicitly and implicitly. In the task used for this study, learning took place on an explicit level, as the same geometric figure configuration was presented in 70% of the auto condition trials. Previous studies suggested that acute alcohol intoxication selectively impairs explicit learning processes59,60,61, as implicit learning was not equally impaired during alcohol intoxication59,60,62,63. Likewise, implicit sequence learning did neither differ between abstinent alcohol-dependent patients and healthy controls64, nor in Korsakoff’s syndrome patients65. These findings suggest that alcohol may have a dissociative effect on learning: While alcohol intoxication seems to impair explicit learning, implicit learning might remain largely stable. Importantly, this implies that the type of learning might be a major determinant in whether or not detrimental effects of intoxication can be observed (please refer to the “Outlook” section for further implications).
Alcohol hangover had no impact on the general learning effect as compared to sobriety. The lack of hangover effects on response selection and planning is (partly) in line with a previous publication of our group demonstrating that hungover participants showed no significant decline in response selection, even though Bayesian analysis provided evidence for slightly decreased response selection in hungover participants, as compared to sober participants26. Furthermore, alcohol hangover had no effect on explicit learning as measured by word-learning tasks4,7,66, although some other studies found significant hangover-induced learning impairments29,67. As suggested before, more research on how alcohol hangover affects motor response sequence learning is needed.
In summary, alcohol hangover did not modulate explicit motor response learning effects, but acute intoxication eradicated any learning effects based on the repetition of response selection and planning. Importantly, this effect seemed to be independent of the observed alcohol effects onto automatization, which were not only reflected by a different interaction, but also in a different behavioral measure.
Limitations
Although our newly developed paradigm allows investigating how complex motor response sequence automatisms are established (as it is very likely that the sequence trial will be holistically processed with increasing automatization), it does not allow investigating implicit learning, implicitly established automatisms, or pre-established/inherent automatisms. Given the potential distinction between implicit and explicit learning outlined in the previous text section, more research is needed on these issues, specifically when considering that AUD patients tend to involuntarily shift away from goal-directed towards habitual behavior.
We refrained from using a placebo condition because initial piloting had shown that the difference between a placebo and the large amounts of alcohol administered by us was very obvious, as all pilot participants could easily identify the placebo. Knowledge about the intoxication and hangover investigated in this study might therefore have produced expectancy effects and/or compensatory effort during task performance. While this is unfortunately largely unavoidable in case of high-dose intoxication, Devenney et al.68 demonstrated that this does not necessarily apply in case of alcohol hangover, as they found no expectancy effects on the cognitive performance in the hangover state when informing one of their study groups about the study purpose.
Further limitations include gender and age. Based on the decision of the local ethics committee, females were not allowed to partake in this study. This is rather unfortunate because previous findings indicated that compared to males, females may be more prone to hangover69 and deficits in cognitive performance after heavy alcohol consumption70,71,72, although this effect was not unequivocally reported by all studies73. Females have a lower first-pass metabolism due to lower ADH enzyme activity74,75, so that alcohol persists for longer in their system76. For these reasons, it is conceivable that females may show stronger cognitive impairments during acute intoxication and/or alcohol hangover than males. Lastly, we did not recruit individuals older than 30 years as we intended to obtain a homogeneous and healthy sample. However, the occurrence of alcohol-related hangover and the severity of hangover symptoms seem to change with age77,78. In order to obtain a more complete picture of the acute and post-acute effects of alcohol consumption, it would be therefore appropriate to include both females and adults of middle to older age in future studies.
Outlook
In this study, we used a paradigm for which a new automatization of motor response sequences was established in an explicit way via the repetition of the same stimulus (and response) combination. On a neurobiological level, NMDA glutamate receptor activation has been shown to play an important role in (explicit) learning79,80. This might explain some of our findings as acute ethanol intoxication inhibits NMDA receptor activity81,82,83, which may in turn have hampered explicit learning. Given that implicit learning has so far not been shown to be equally impaired by alcohol intoxication59,60,62,63, it is hence possible that we observed intoxication-induced deficits in automatization and learning effects due to the explicit nature of the task. Because this does however not necessarily have to be the case in an implicit learning task, this should motivate further studies investigating whether and how implicitly acquired motor response sequence automatisms are affected by acute intoxication and alcohol hangover. As the selection and execution of pre-existing automatisms also seem to be rather unaffected by alcohol13,17,19, it could further be speculated that AUD patients and/or binge-drinkers strongly rely on habits because the necessary automatisms have already been previously established (inherently or potentially through implicit learning), and not because the explicit acquisition of new habits is facilitated during intoxication. Yet, this remains to be investigated in future studies.
Concerning the differential effects of acute intoxication and alcohol hangover, a possible explanation might be found in the opposing effects of ethanol and its major metabolite acetaldehyde on GABAergic neurotransmission84. In fact, it has been shown that GABA plays an important role in motor learning85,86. Accordingly, stronger GABA-mediated inhibition was associated with poorer performance in a motor sequence learning task85. As acute ethanol intoxication enhances GABAergic signaling87, this mechanism may have led to the intoxication-impaired motor response sequences learning in our study as well. In contrast to this, there might be no comparable motor learning deficits during alcohol hangover, as acetaldehyde seems to reduce the activity of the GABAergic system84,88. These issues also deserve future investigations.
Conclusion
To sum up, we investigated whether alcohol leads to a stronger impairment of top-down controlled motor response sequences, as compared to the establishment and execution of motor response sequence automatisms. Interestingly, we did not observe an alcohol-induced shift from controlled to habitual behavior, which would have indicated a shift in the “metacontrol” of the balance between those two functionally opposing operation modes. Instead, we observed two statistically independent effects reflecting that alcohol intoxication (but not hangover) impaired two different processes of complex motor response sequences. Partly opposing our initial hypothesis, we found that alcohol intoxication interfered with the automatization of motor response sequences more strongly than with their controlled execution. Moreover, alcohol intoxication eradicated learning effects on response selection and planning, irrespective of automatization. The finding that the planning of motor response sequences as well as the automatization of motor response sequence execution are both impaired by intoxication speaks against the hypothesis that automatization tendencies are generally and unequivocally spared and/or reinforced by acute intoxication. While our findings contradict the assumption that alcohol intoxication facilitates the acquisition of explicit learning and automatization, this does however not exclude the possibility that acute intoxication may improve the execution of pre-existing automatisms and/or the implicit acquisition of motor response sequence automatisms.
Methods
Participants
Given that we used a newly developed paradigm, we did an a priori estimation of the required sample size for our study design using G*power software89. Yielding for a small-to-medium effect size of f = 0.1590 with an alpha error probability of 5%, a power of 95%, two alcohol manipulation groups (intoxication vs. hangover), eight measurements (appointment [sober vs. alcohol] x task condition [control vs. auto] x task block [block 1 vs. block 5]), and an assumed correlation among repeated measures of r = 0.5, we obtained a sample size of n = 62. To compensate for potential issues and dropouts, we initially recruited n = 76 healthy males aged between 18 and 30 years for this study. All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision, no prior history of physical, neurological or mental illness, and no acute or chronic medication intake that could influence liver or kidney function, or the central nervous system. The participants’ eligibility was determined via a telephone screening. This included the assessment of their physical and mental health with a semi-structured interview and their drinking pattern using the alcohol use disorders identification test (AUDIT)91. Individuals who reported alcohol-induced failure to perform daily routines and/or alcohol-related memory problems and/or binge drinking (i.e., consuming eight or more standard units of alcohol per occasion) “daily” or “almost daily”, were excluded because this would have indicated extensive problems associated with alcohol (ab)use. In order to minimize the risk of intoxication-associated complications, we furthermore required that all participants had practiced voluntary binge drinking at least once within the past 12 months. Further inclusion criteria on drinking patterns slightly varied between the intoxication and the hangover group, as the administered amount of alcohol was lower in the intoxication group than in the hangover group (please refer to the “Experimental procedure and alcohol administration” section). The reason for this was that we were required to only include participants who reported to have recently (within the last year) tolerated alcohol amounts comparable to those administered in their experimental group, and the absolute amount administered in the hangover group was higher than that administered in the intoxication group—even though we had of course planned to achieve comparable maximal intoxication levels in both experimental groups.
For the intoxication group, individuals who reported to binge drink at least once a year but no more than once a month, and who had an overall moderate/ non-high AUD risk pattern of alcohol use were included. This was defined by AUDIT scores between 1 and 15 points91. For the hangover group, individuals who reported to binge drink at least once a month but no more than weekly/less than (almost) daily, and who had an overall moderate to harmful pattern of alcohol use, but without AUD diagnosis according to the ICD-10 criteria, were included. This was defined by AUDIT scores between 2 and 19 points91. Experienced psychologists screened all participants to make sure that none of them met the full diagnostic criteria required for an AUD diagnosis according to ICD-10. All participants provided written informed consent prior the beginning of the experiment and received a financial reimbursement (intoxication group: 70 € in total, hangover group: 80 € in total) or study credits. The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Medicine of the TU Dresden (ID number: EK293082014).
Experimental design
In this study, we used a mixed 2 × 2 × 2 experimental design, in which the within-subject factor alcohol administration (alcohol vs. sober), and the between-subject factors appointment order (alcohol on first vs. second appointment) and alcohol manipulation group (intoxicated vs. hungover) were systematically varied. This is illustrated in Fig. 5.
Each participant was tested twice, i.e., once sober and once under the influence of alcohol. Regarding the alcohol manipulation, each participant was either assigned to the intoxication group or to the hangover group. The appointment order was balanced across all participants so that half of the participants had their sober appointment before their alcohol appointment, whereas the other half had their alcohol appointment before their sober appointment. For this reason, systematic differences between the first and second appointment could not be investigated in the statistical analyses reported in the main text, but we provided additional analyses on the order irrespective of the alcohol manipulation (T1 vs. T2) in the Supplementary Material. Both appointments were scheduled for a minimum of 48 hours and a maximum of 7 days apart from each other. All participants were asked to refrain from the use of caffeine, guanine, nicotine and all other sedative or stimulant substances within the last four hours prior to the start of each appointment. At the beginning of each appointment and at several time points during acute intoxication, breath alcohol concentration (BrAC) was assessed using the breathalyzer “Alcotest 3000” (Drägerwerk, Lübeck, Germany). Participants were required to be sober (BrAC = 0.00 ‰) at the beginning of each appointment. The experimental procedure only resumed when sobriety was given.
Experimental procedure and alcohol administration
For each alcohol manipulation group, the experimental procedure and the alcohol administration followed protocols that had already been used in previous studies on alcohol intoxication or alcohol hangover, respectively12,15,26,92. An overview of the study protocols is provided in Figs. 6 and 7, while the details of those protocols are provided in the Supplementary Material. All participants were experimentally intoxicated in a controlled laboratory environment. Participants in the intoxication group were administered 1.98 g of alcohol per liter of estimated total body water (TBW) on an empty stomach within 30 minutes, while participants in the hangover group were administered 2.64 g of alcohol per liter of estimated TBW on a full stomach within 2 hours or more. Based on the assumption of different resorption deficits on an empty vs. full stomach (20% vs. 30–40%), we however aimed for average maximal BrACs of approximately 1.2 ‰ in both experimental groups.
Questionnaires
At the beginning of their respective intoxication appointment (and thus before alcohol administration), all participants provided information on height, weight, and socio-demographic characteristics and filled in the Beck Depression Inventory93,94 to determine potential depressive symptoms. In the hangover group, participants additionally rated the severity of 24 hangover symptoms on an 11-point Likert scale, which reached from 0 (non-existent) to 10 (extreme), at the beginning of both their sober and their hangover appointment. For this purpose, we used the hangover symptoms list introduced by Hogewoning and colleagues95 and added another item to asses sleep problems.
Task
We used a newly developed experimental paradigm to investigate the automatization of motor response sequences. This was done by comparing the execution of complex motor response sequences in situations that require top-down controlled motor response sequence generation to situations where motor response sequence generation could be automatized. Specifically, the motor response sequences generated in this paradigm were sequences of S-R associations, as single stimuli triggered single motor responses within the motor response sequence. The task is visualized in Fig. 8.
The task was presented on a 17'' high quality flat screen monitor with a refresh rate of 144 Hz in a well-lit and quiet room. Stimulus presentation and recording of the behavioral responses were implemented using Presentation® software (Version 16.5, Neurobehavioral Systems, Inc., Berkeley, CA, USA). The task comprised an exercise and the actual experiment.
Exercise
The aim of the exercise was to establish single S-R associations between visual stimuli and response keys. Importantly, this pre-experimental establishment of single S-R associations allows to investigate the controlled vs. automatized generation of motor response sequences free of potentially (strong) confounding effects stemming from inter-individual differences in the establishment of single S-R associations. For this reason, the main exercise was always conducted while the participants were still sober. As this created a temporal gap of about 60 min between exercise and experiment when testing acute intoxication effects (but not at the sober appointment, or in the hangover group), participants received an additional brief “fresh up” exercise right before starting their task performance while intoxicated. A total of six simple geometric figures were linked to six different keys on a standard QWERTZ keyboard. This is detailed in Fig. 8C. In the exercise, participants were instructed to respond to single geometric figures by pressing the corresponding key with the respective finger to make sure all participants had formed stable S-R associations for all stimuli and responses before starting the actual experiment. Participants were asked to keep their fingers on the response keys during the entire duration of the exercise. Each trial (compare Fig. 8A) started with the central presentation of one of the six white geometric figures on black background. Stimulus presentation ended with a key press or after 3000 ms (in case of a missed response). A black screen was then displayed for 400–500 ms, after which feedback was given. In case of a wrong or missed response, a red minus sign was centrally presented for 500 ms at T1 and 1000 ms at T2. In case of a correct response, a green plus sign was centrally presented for 500 ms at T1 and 1000 ms at T2. Eventually, a white fixation cross was presented during the inter-trial interval, which randomly varied between 600 and 1100 ms. On the first appointment, the exercise consisted of 20 mandatory blocks. The first ten blocks comprised six trials each. In each of these blocks, every single geometric figure was presented once in a fixed order (starting with the respective key press from left to right on the keyboard). The subsequent ten blocks comprised twelve trials each. In each of those blocks, every single figure was presented twice in a randomized order. The exercise at T1 took approximately 10–15 min. On the second appointment, each block contained twelve trials, that is, every single figure was randomly presented twice. The exercise was finished as soon as one whole block (i.e., 12 trials in succession) was performed without any errors or misses, which took roughly 2–5 min. Due to the brief interval between both appointments, a short task refreshing of the pre-established S-R associations on the second appointment was sufficient to reactivate the single stimulus S-R associations at T2. In case either the experimenters or the participants did not feel that stable S-R associations had been formed during the exercise, the exercise was repeated until that point was reached.
Experiment
In the actual experiment, participants were instructed to respond to four geometric figures by successively pressing the corresponding keys as quickly and as accurately as possible when “reading” them from left to right. In each trial (compare Fig. 8B), an array consisting of four horizontally aligned white geometric figures was centrally presented on black background. Stimulus presentation ended with the fourth key press or after 5000 ms. After stimulus offset, a black screen was displayed for 500 ms. In case of at least one wrong or missed response, feedback was given by a red minus centrally presented for 500 ms. In case all four responses were correct, another black screen was displayed for 500 ms. Eventually, a white fixation cross was presented during the inter-trial interval, which randomly varied between 700 and 1100 ms. The experiment comprised 300 trials, which were divided into ten blocks of equal size (i.e., 30 trials each). Out of these, five blocks were so-called “control” blocks and the other five blocks were “auto(matization)” blocks. The experiment always started with a control block and the two block types alternated, so that all odd blocks were control blocks and all even-numbered blocks were auto blocks. In control blocks, the occurrence and configuration of the four geometric figures were randomized in each trial. Because the figure configuration was not predictable in any of the control block trials, participants had to rely on top-down cognitive control in order to configure and execute the correct motor response sequence. In auto blocks, the configuration of the four geometric figures was identical in 70% of all trials (auto identical trials; i.e., the same four figures were presented in the same order, thus requiring the same motor response sequence), while the figure configuration was randomized in the remaining 30% of all trials (comparable to the control block trials). While the randomized trials prevented premature responding before/upon stimulus onset, the frequent repetition of the same figure configuration in the auto identical trials allowed for the automatization of motor response sequence generation. This means that in auto identical trials, participants could rely on less cognitively effortful, bottom up motor response generation in order to execute the right response. With increasing repetitions (i.e., with increasing block numbers), this automatization is increasingly established. This should be reflected in faster and more accurate responses in auto identical trials, as compared to control trials. Importantly, the figure configuration of the auto identical trials differed between first and second appointment (at the first appointment: circle, cloud, hexagon, diamond; at the second appointment: triangle, star, circle, hexagon), but always required two responses from each hand. This was done to prevent/minimize learning effects between the two study appointments. The experiment took approximately 25 min and participants were offered to take breaks after each block.
Eventually, the block conditions control and auto, of which only auto identical trials were kept, and the respectively first and last (i.e., fifth) block entered the statistical analyses. Behavioral measures that can be obtained from the task (please see Fig. 8D) are accuracy (i.e., percentage of trials with four correct responses) and entire sequence duration (ESD), which can be further subdivided into first motor response time (RT1) and motor sequence duration (MSD). ESD is the total response time from stimulus onset to the fourth response and thus reflects overall differences in automatic vs. controlled motor response sequence generation that modulate the duration of this process. RT1 is the time between stimulus onset and the first response, thus mainly reflecting cognitive processes involved in planning and selection of the motor response sequence. MSD is the duration of the motor response sequence itself (i.e., the time from the first to the fourth response), thus mainly reflecting processes involved in the coordination and execution of the motor response sequence.
Statistical analyses
Only trials in which participants responded correctly to all four geometric figures (within a time window of 200–3000 ms after stimulus onset for the first response and within a time window of 25–1500 ms after each previous key press for the second to fourth response) entered statistical analyses. In case this exclusion procedure resulted in less than 10 remaining trials in any of the condition combinations (i.e., in any block), the participant was excluded from the analyses.
The obtained accuracy and ESD measures were analyzed using separate mixed ANOVAs with the between-subject factor alcohol manipulation group (intoxicated vs. hungover), and the three within-subject factors alcohol administration (sober vs. alcohol), condition (control vs. automatization), and block (block 1 vs. block 5). To determine the most likely source of the obtained ESD effects, we subsequently ran the same mixed ANOVA separately for RT1 and for MSD. The degrees of freedom were Greenhouse–Geisser corrected whenever necessary. Behavioral variables were tested for variance homogeneity using Levene’s tests and for normal distribution using Kolmogorov–Smirnov tests. If these assumptions were violated, Welch-tests (in case of variance heterogeneity) and Mann–Whitney U tests or Wilcoxon signed-rank tests (in case of deviation from normality) were used to retest the significance of main effects and post hoc comparisons. The mean, minimum and maximum value, as well as the standard error of the mean as a measure of variability were reported for the presentation of descriptive statistics.
Data availability
The datasets generated during and/or analyzed during the current study is available online via https://osf.io/jmxac/.
References
World Health Organization. Global status report on alcohol and health 2018. https://apps.who.int/iris/handle/10665/274603. License: CC BY-NC-SA 3.0 IGO (World Health Organization, 2018).
National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism. NIAAA Council Approves Definition of Binge Drinking. https://pubs.niaaa.nih.gov/publications/Newsletter/winter2004/Newsletter_Number3.pdf. (Office of Research Translation and Communications, NIAAA, NIH, DHHS, 2004).
ESPAD Group. ESPAD Report 2019: Results from the European school survey project on alcohol and other drugs. https://doi.org/10.2810/877033 (EMCDDA Joint Publications, Publications Office of the European Union, Luxembourg, 2020).
Devenney, L. E., Coyle, K. B. & Verster, J. C. Memory and attention during an alcohol hangover. Hum. Psychopharmacol. Clin. Exp. https://doi.org/10.1002/hup.2701 (2019).
Field, M., Wiers, R. W., Christiansen, P., Fillmore, M. T. & Verster, J. C. Acute alcohol effects on inhibitory control and implicit cognition: Implications for loss of control over drinking. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 34, 1346–1352 (2010).
Gunn, C., Mackus, M., Griffin, C., Munafò, M. R. & Adams, S. A systematic review of the next-day effects of heavy alcohol consumption on cognitive performance. Addict. Abingdon Engl. 113, 2182–2193 (2018).
McKinney, A., Coyle, K. & Verster, J. Direct comparison of the cognitive effects of acute alcohol with the morning after a normal night’s drinking. Hum. Psychopharmacol. 27, 295–304 (2012).
Naranjo, C. A. & Bremner, K. E. Behavioural correlates of alcohol intoxication. Addict. Abingdon Engl. 88, 25–35 (1993).
Peterson, J. B., Rothfleisch, J., Zelazo, P. D. & Pihl, R. O. Acute alcohol intoxication and cognitive functioning. J. Stud. Alcohol 51, 114–122 (1990).
Stephens, R., Ling, J., Heffernan, T. M., Heather, N. & Jones, K. Review * A review of the literature on the cognitive effects of alcohol hangover. Alcohol Alcohol 43, 163–170 (2008).
Van Skike, C. E., Goodlett, C. & Matthews, D. B. Acute alcohol and cognition: Remembering what it causes us to forget. Alcohol 79, 105–125 (2019).
Zink, N., Bensmann, W., Beste, C. & Stock, A.-K. Alcohol hangover increases conflict load via faster processing of subliminal information. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 12, 316 (2018).
Chmielewski, W. X., Zink, N., Chmielewski, K. Y., Beste, C. & Stock, A.-K. How high-dose alcohol intoxication affects the interplay of automatic and controlled processes. Addict. Biol. 25, e12700 (2020).
Heinz, A. et al. Addiction Research Consortium: Losing and regaining control over drug intake (ReCoDe)-From trajectories to mechanisms and interventions. Addict. Biol. 25, e12866 (2020).
Stock, A.-K., Bensmann, W., Zink, N., Münchau, A. & Beste, C. Automatic aspects of response selection remain unchanged during high-dose alcohol intoxication. Addict. Biol. 26, e12852. https://doi.org/10.1111/adb.12852 (2019).
Stock, A.-K., Riegler, L., Chmielewski, W. X. & Beste, C. Paradox effects of binge drinking on response inhibition processes depending on mental workload. Arch. Toxicol. 90, 1429–1436 (2016).
Stock, A.-K., Schulz, T., Lenhardt, M., Blaszkewicz, M. & Beste, C. High-dose alcohol intoxication differentially modulates cognitive subprocesses involved in response inhibition. Addict. Biol. 21, 136–145 (2016).
Field, M., Schoenmakers, T. & Wiers, R. W. Cognitive processes in alcohol binges: A review and research agenda. Curr. Drug Abuse Rev. 1, 263–279 (2008).
Stock, A.-K. Barking up the wrong tree: Why and how we may need to revise alcohol addiction therapy. Front. Psychol. 8, 884 (2017).
Courtney, K. E. & Polich, J. Binge drinking in young adults: Data, definitions, and determinants. Psychol. Bull. 135, 142–156 (2009).
Knight, J. R. et al. Alcohol abuse and dependence among U.S. college students. J. Stud. Alcohol 63, 263–270 (2002).
Montgomery, C., Fisk, J. E., Murphy, P. N., Ryland, I. & Hilton, J. The effects of heavy social drinking on executive function: A systematic review and meta-analytic study of existing literature and new empirical findings: Executive function and alcohol use. Hum. Psychopharmacol. Clin. Exp. 27, 187–199 (2012).
Goldstein, R. Z. & Volkow, N. D. Dysfunction of the prefrontal cortex in addiction: Neuroimaging findings and clinical implications. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 12, 652–669 (2011).
Everitt, B. J. & Robbins, T. W. Drug addiction: Updating actions to habits to compulsions ten years on. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 67, 23–50 (2016).
Verster, J. C., Scholey, A., van de Loo, A. J. A. E., Benson, S. & Stock, A.-K. Updating the definition of the alcohol hangover. J. Clin. Med. 9, 823 (2020).
Opitz, A., Hubert, J., Beste, C. & Stock, A.-K. Alcohol hangover slightly impairs response selection but not response inhibition. J. Clin. Med. 8, 1317 (2019).
Stock, A.-K., Hoffmann, S. & Beste, C. Effects of binge drinking and hangover on response selection sub-processes-a study using EEG and drift diffusion modeling. Addict. Biol. 22, 1355–1365 (2017).
Berghäuser, J. et al. Alcohol hangover does not alter the application of model-based and model-free learning strategies. J. Clin. Med. 9, 1453 (2020).
Devenney, L. E., Coyle, K. B. & Verster, J. C. Cognitive performance and mood after a normal night of drinking: A naturalistic alcohol hangover study in a non-student sample. Addict. Behav. Rep. 10, 100197 (2019).
Gunn, C., Fairchild, G., Verster, J. C. & Adams, S. The effects of alcohol hangover on executive functions. J. Clin. Med. 9, 1148 (2020).
Heffernan, T., Samuels, A., Hamilton, C. & McGrath-Brookes, M. Alcohol hangover has detrimental impact upon both executive function and prospective memory. Front. Psychiatry 10, 282 (2019).
Ling, J., Stephens, R. & Heffernan, M. Cognitive and psychomotor performance during alcohol hangover. Curr. Drug Abuse Rev. 3, 80–87 (2010).
Scholey, A. et al. Effects of alcohol hangover on cognitive performance: Findings from a field/internet mixed methodology study. J. Clin. Med. 8, 440 (2019).
Dezfouli, A. & Balleine, B. W. Actions, action sequences and habits: Evidence that goal-directed and habitual action control are hierarchically organized. PLoS Comput. Biol. 9, e1003364 (2013).
Gillan, C. M. et al. Disruption in the balance between goal-directed behavior and habit learning in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 168, 718–726 (2011).
Sjoerds, Z. et al. Behavioral and neuroimaging evidence for overreliance on habit learning in alcohol-dependent patients. Transl. Psychiatry 3, e337–e337 (2013).
Wiers, R. W., Eberl, C., Rinck, M., Becker, E. S. & Lindenmeyer, J. Retraining automatic action tendencies changes alcoholic patients’ approach bias for alcohol and improves treatment outcome. Psychol. Sci. 22, 490–497 (2011).
Gowin, J. L., Sloan, M. E., Stangl, B. L., Vatsalya, V. & Ramchandani, V. A. Vulnerability for alcohol use disorder and rate of alcohol consumption. Am. J. Psychiatry 174, 1094–1101 (2017).
Heinz, A. et al. Addiction as learned behavior patterns. J. Clin. Med. 8, 1086 (2019).
Desrochers, T. M. & McKim, T. H. What is a sequence? The neural mechanisms of perceptual, motor, and task sequences across species and their interaction with addiction. Oxf. Res. Encycl. Neurosci. https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190264086.013.289 (2019).
Huys, Q. J. M. et al. Bonsai trees in your head: how the Pavlovian system sculpts goal-directed choices by pruning decision trees. PLoS Comput. Biol. 8, e1002410 (2012).
Otto, A. R., Gershman, S. J., Markman, A. B. & Daw, N. D. The curse of planning: Dissecting multiple reinforcement-learning systems by taxing the central executive. Psychol. Sci. 24, 751–761 (2013).
de Wit, S. & Dickinson, A. Associative theories of goal-directed behaviour: A case for animal-human translational models. Psychol. Res. 73, 463–476 (2009).
McKim, T. H., Shnitko, T. A., Robinson, D. L. & Boettiger, C. A. Translational research on habit and alcohol. Curr. Addict. Rep. 3, 37–49 (2016).
Yin, H. H. & Knowlton, B. J. The role of the basal ganglia in habit formation. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 7, 464–476 (2006).
Barker, J. M. & Taylor, J. R. Habitual alcohol seeking: Modeling the transition from casual drinking to addiction. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 47, 281–294 (2014).
Hoffman, L. & Nixon, S. J. Alcohol doesn’t always compromise cognitive function: Exploring moderate doses in young adults. J. Stud. Alcohol Drugs 76, 952–956 (2015).
Verster, J. C., van de Loo, A. J. A. E., Benson, S., Scholey, A. & Stock, A.-K. The assessment of overall hangover severity. J. Clin. Med. 9, 786 (2020).
Opitz, A., Beste, C. & Stock, A.-K. Alcohol hangover differentially modulates the processing of relevant and irrelevant information. J. Clin. Med. 9, 778 (2020).
Scholey, A., Ayre, E., Stock, A.-K., Verster, J. C. & Benson, S. Effects of rapid recovery on alcohol hangover severity: A double-blind, placebo-controlled, randomized, balanced crossover trial. J. Clin. Med. 9, 2175 (2020).
Verster, J. C. et al. The impact of mood and subjective intoxication on hangover severity. J. Clin. Med. 9, 2462 (2020).
Verster, J. C. et al. The association between alcohol hangover frequency and severity: Evidence for reverse tolerance?. J. Clin. Med. 8, 1520 (2019).
Benson, S. et al. Alcohol hangover and multitasking: Effects on mood, cognitive performance, stress reactivity, and perceived effort. J. Clin. Med. 9, 1154 (2020).
Gunn, C., Fairchild, G., Verster, J. C. & Adams, S. Does alcohol hangover affect emotion regulation capacity? Evidence from a naturalistic cross-over study design. Alcohol Alcohol. Oxf. Oxfs. https://doi.org/10.1093/alcalc/agaa123 (2020).
Masson, M. E. J. A tutorial on a practical Bayesian alternative to null-hypothesis significance testing. Behav. Res. Methods 43, 679–690 (2011).
Raftery, A. E. Bayesian model selection in social research. Sociol. Methodol. 25, 111 (1995).
Obst, E. et al. Drunk decisions: Alcohol shifts choice from habitual towards goal-directed control in adolescent intermediate-risk drinkers. J. Psychopharmacol. (Oxf.) 32, 855–866 (2018).
Chan, J. S. Y., Jin, Y., Luo, Y. & Yan, J. H. Dysfunctional preparatory processes in hazardous drinkers: An ERP study. Alcohol Fayettev. N 77, 71–77 (2019).
Duka, T., Weissenborn, R. & Dienes, Z. State-dependent effects of alcohol on recollective experience, familiarity and awareness of memories. Psychopharmacology 153, 295–306 (2001).
Ray, S., Mun, E.-Y., Buckman, J. F., Udo, T. & Bates, M. E. Memory for emotional picture cues during acute alcohol intoxication. J. Stud. Alcohol Drugs 73, 718–725 (2012).
Benedek, M. & Zöhrer, L. Creativity on tap 2: Investigating dose effects of alcohol on cognitive control and creative cognition. Conscious. Cogn. 83, 102972 (2020).
Kirchner, T. R. & Sayette, M. A. Effects of alcohol on controlled and automatic memory processes. Exp. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 11, 167–175 (2003).
Ray, S. & Bates, M. E. Acute alcohol effects on repetition priming and word recognition memory with equivalent memory cues. Brain Cogn. 60, 118–127 (2006).
Virag, M. et al. Competition between frontal lobe functions and implicit sequence learning: Evidence from the long-term effects of alcohol. Exp. Brain Res. 233, 2081–2089 (2015).
Van Tilborg, I. A. D. A., Kessels, R. P. C., Kruijt, P., Wester, A. J. & Hulstijn, W. Spatial and nonspatial implicit motor learning in Korsakoff’s amnesia: Evidence for selective deficits. Exp. Brain Res. 214, 427–435 (2011).
Verster, J. C., van Duin, D., Volkerts, E. R., Schreuder, A. H. C. M. L. & Verbaten, M. N. Alcohol hangover effects on memory functioning and vigilance performance after an evening of binge drinking. Neuropsychopharmacology 28, 740–746 (2003).
McKinney, A. & Coyle, K. Next day effects of a normal night’s drinking on memory and psychomotor performance. Alcohol Alcohol. Oxf. Oxfs. 39, 509–513 (2004).
Devenney, L. E., Coyle, K. B. & Verster, J. C. The impact of expectancy on cognitive performance during alcohol hangover. BMC Res. Notes 11, 730 (2018).
van Lawick van Pabst, A. E., Devenney, L. E. & Verster, J. C. Sex differences in the presence and severity of alcohol hangover symptoms. J. Clin. Med. 8, 867 (2019).
Haut, J. S., Beckwith, B. E., Petros, T. V. & Russell, S. Gender differences in retrieval from long-term memory following acute intoxication with ethanol. Physiol. Behav. 45, 1161–1165 (1989).
Nederkoorn, C., Baltus, M., Guerrieri, R. & Wiers, R. W. Heavy drinking is associated with deficient response inhibition in women but not in men. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 93, 331–336 (2009).
Townshend, J. M. & Duka, T. Binge drinking, cognitive performance and mood in a population of young social drinkers. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 29, 317–325 (2005).
Fillmore, M. T. & Weafer, J. Alcohol impairment of behavior in men and women. Addiction 99, 1237–1246 (2004).
Baraona, E. et al. Gender differences in pharmacokinetics of alcohol. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 25, 502–507 (2001).
Frezza, M. et al. High blood alcohol levels in women. N. Engl. J. Med. 322, 95–99 (1990).
Erol, A. & Karpyak, V. M. Sex and gender-related differences in alcohol use and its consequences: Contemporary knowledge and future research considerations. Drug Alcohol Depend. 156, 1–13 (2015).
Huntley, G. et al. An event-level investigation of hangovers’ relationship to age and drinking. Exp. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 23, 314–323 (2015).
Tolstrup, J. S., Stephens, R. & Grønbaek, M. Does the severity of hangovers decline with age? Survey of the incidence of hangover in different age groups. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 38, 466–470 (2014).
Hu, C., Chen, W., Myers, S. J., Yuan, H. & Traynelis, S. F. Human GRIN2B variants in neurodevelopmental disorders. J. Pharmacol. Sci. 132, 115–121 (2016).
Tang, Y.-P. et al. Genetic enhancement of learning and memory in mice. Nature 401, 63–69 (1999).
Lovinger, D., White, G. & Weight, F. NMDA receptor-mediated synaptic excitation selectively inhibited by ethanol in hippocampal slice from adult rat. J. Neurosci. 10, 1372–1379 (1990).
Schummers, J. & Browning, M. D. Evidence for a role for GABA(A) and NMDA receptors in ethanol inhibition of long-term potentiation. Brain Res. Mol. Brain Res. 94, 9–14 (2001).
Swartzwelder, H. S., Wilson, W. A. & Tayyeb, M. I. Differential sensitivity of NMDA receptor-mediated synaptic potentials to ethanol in immature versus mature hippocampus. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 19, 320–323 (1995).
Martí-Prats, L. et al. Opposite motor responses elicited by ethanol in the posterior VTA: The role of acetaldehyde and the non-metabolized fraction of ethanol. Neuropharmacology 72, 204–214 (2013).
Kolasinski, J. et al. The dynamics of cortical GABA in human motor learning. J. Physiol. 597, 271–282 (2019).
Stagg, C. J., Bachtiar, V. & Johansen-Berg, H. The role of GABA in human motor learning. Curr. Biol. 21, 480–484 (2011).
Lobo, I. A. & Harris, R. A. GABAA receptors and alcohol. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 90, 90–94 (2008).
Kuriyama, K., Ohkuma, S., Taguchi, J. & Hashimoto, T. Alcohol, acetaldehyde and salsolinol-induced alterations in functions of cerebral GABA/benzodiazepine receptor complex. Physiol. Behav. 40, 393–399 (1987).
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A. & Lang, A.-G. Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav. Res. Methods 41, 1149–1160 (2009).
Cohen, J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences (Psychology Press, 2009).
World Health Organization. AUDIT: the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test: guidelines for use in primary health care / Thomas F. Babor ... [et al.], 2nd ed. https://apps.who.int/iris/handle/10665/67205 (World Health Organization, 2001).
Bensmann, W., Zink, N., Werner, A., Beste, C. & Stock, A.-K. Acute alcohol effects on response inhibition depend on response automatization, but not on GABA or glutamate levels in the ACC and striatum. J. Clin. Med. 9, 481 (2020).
Beck, A. T. An inventory for measuring depression. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 4, 561 (1961).
Hautzinger, M., Bailer, M., Worall, H. & Keller, F. Beck-Depressions-Inventar (BDI) (Huber, 1994).
Hogewoning, A. et al. Characteristics of social drinkers with and without a hangover after heavy alcohol consumption. Subst. Abuse Rehabil. 7, 161–167 (2016).
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by a Grant from the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG) TRR 265 B07. We thank all individuals who took part in this study.
Funding
Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
A.O.: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Validation, Visualization, Writing—original draft, Writing—review & editing. F.G.: Data curation, Formal analysis, Validation, Writing—review & editing. J.H.: Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Writing—original draft. J.C.V.: Conceptualization, Supervision, Writing—review & editing. C.B.: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Resources, Supervision, Writing—review & editing. A.S.: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Resources, Project administration, Validation, Supervision, Writing—review & editing.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
J.C.V. has held grants from Janssen and Sequential Medicine, and acted as a consultant/expert advisor to More Labs, Red Bull, Sen-Jam Pharmaceutical, Toast!, Tomo, and ZBiotics. A.S. has received funding from the Daimler and Benz Foundation. A.O., F.G., J.H., and C.B. declare no potential conflict of interests.
Additional information
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Opitz, A., Ghin, F., Hubert, J. et al. Alcohol intoxication, but not hangover, differentially impairs learning and automatization of complex motor response sequences. Sci Rep 11, 12539 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-90803-5
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-90803-5
Comments
By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.