Introduction

Meditation has become a popular psychological tool, used in clinical and non-clinical contexts alike1,2,3,4,5. Accordingly, the neural substrates of meditation have attracted increasing research interest6. Such research is highly relevant, because a granular understanding of the distinct brain mechanisms that support meditation would enable utilizing meditation practices in an informed way and optimise their targeted implementation7,8,9,10,11.

One central neurophysiologic issue is whether meditation is a generic (or unique) brain state which can be cultivated by different meditation styles or whether brain states in meditation are profoundly different depending on the specific mental skills practiced in each style. In a previous study12, we addressed this question by analysing brain connectivity patterns in highly experienced meditators during three commonly used types of meditation: focused attention meditation (FAM), open monitoring meditation (OMM) and loving kindness meditation (LKM). We showed that brain states during these three meditation states share generic patterns that are substantially different from rest. In addition, we identified connectivity patterns that were specific to each meditation state.

The aim of the current study was to unpack the involved processes further by zooming in on the role of cognitive brain networks. Given the growing evidence that brain mechanisms of cognitive control are involved in meditation6,13,14,15,16, we surmise that training-induced neuroplasticity of these networks may contribute to generic brain states of meditation as well as to specific states that depends on the type of meditation7,17. Towards this end, we re-analysed our previous data set12 in the following directions: (1) we used FAM, OMM and LKM as three representative meditation states that differentially engage specific cognitive functions, (2) we explored the involvement of relevant executive control networks by analysing their synchronization patterns, and (3) we examined experienced meditators with a broad range of accumulated lifetime meditation practice to estimate the potential influence of meditation expertise on the involvement of executive networks.

Meditation types

To address the question how executive control networks are involved in regulating cognitive processes in different meditation states, we focused on FAM, OMM and LKM12. Popular classification systems of meditation that integrate traditional understanding of meditation with psychological and neuro-functional perspectives single out these three meditation types as prominent meditation exemplars6,18,19,20,21.

FAM entails voluntarily focusing attention on a chosen object in a sustained fashion. In this context “objects” denotes physical objects in the external world, bodily sensations, such as the breath, but also mental objects, including thoughts, emotions or imagined visual forms. Focusing and sustaining attention on an object requires monitoring the focus of attention, detecting distraction, disengaging attention from the source of distraction, and (re)directing and engaging attention to the intended object6,15. Hence, the neuro-functional prediction is that specific neural systems in the human brain associated with selective attention, sustaining attention and conflict monitoring are involved in inducing and maintaining the state of FAM6,13.

OMM involves non-reactively monitoring experience from moment to moment without maintaining an explicit attentional focus on a specific object6. The aim of OMM is to cultivate meta-awareness of momentary sensations, thoughts and feelings, an ability closely linked to mindfulness14,15,19,22,23. Thus, OMM may primarily rely on brain systems that control and monitor cognitive states and regulate mind wandering, rather than on systems that establish and sustain object-focused attention14.

Whereas FAM and OMM cultivate distinct states of attention and meta-cognitive awareness, LKM aims to enhance prosocial and empathetic feelings, attitudes, and intentions19. This is achieved by cultivating acceptance of self and others, and the intention (motivation) toward wellbeing and happiness of self and others5,24. LKM shares some attributes with both FAM and OMM, such as top-down regulation of mind wandering, and the activation and maintenance of goal representations related to the meditation. Moreover, some aspects of LKM require object focus (e.g., focus on the mental representation of other people), resembling some aspects of FAM, whereas advanced forms of non-referential LKM bear similarity to OMM6,25. Therefore, it is plausible to expect that cognitive control, attentional and monitoring networks that are involved in FAM and OMM also play a role during LKM.

In the present study, we explored whether FAM, OMM and LKM would induce a common versus a differential involvement of attentional and cognitive monitoring brain systems, in line with the predicted differential involvement of these mental processes. Finding the same pattern across all three forms of meditation would indicate the activation of common executive processes in meditation, irrespective of the style, such as those supporting the intention or goal to perform the meditation task, attention to the present moment, sustained meta-awareness, regulation of mind wandering, and mental (executive) effort. By contrast, different patterns of activation of the attention and cognitive monitoring systems during each style of meditation would confirm the specificity of mental processes involved in meditation.

Executive control networks

Importantly, the cognitive mechanisms playing a role in FAM, OMM and LKM—attention regulation, cognitive control, cognitive monitoring, and meta-awareness—are associated with anatomically and functionally dissociable systems in the brain, primarily the fronto-parietal and medial frontal networks26,27.

Fronto-parietal networks

From a functional point of view, fronto-parietal (FP) networks have been associated with spatial attention and orienting of attention. The dorsal FP networks are involved in the control of spatial and feature-based attention and in stimulus–response mapping, while the right-lateralized ventral FP network is linked to reorienting of attention to unexpected but behaviourally relevant events26,28,29. FP networks also subserve attention-based conscious perception28,30,31,32, cognitive control and working memory33,34,35,36,37, with prominent activations found in the right hemisphere38. Hence, the dorsal and ventral FP networks are essentially recognized as the neural substrate of focused attention and attention re-allocation29,39. Major operating frequencies of FP networks have been established for both slow and fast-frequency EEG ranges, in particular theta, alpha and beta frequency bands40,41,42.

Medial frontal network

Specific neuro-functional substrates have also been identified for a cognitive control and monitoring system in the medial frontal brain including the anterior cingulate cortex and medial frontal (MF) regions, brain areas that are linked to behavioural adaptation and control27,43,44,45,46. The major operating frequency of this monitoring system appears to be in the theta frequency band: medial frontal theta oscillations have been attributed a major coordinating function within the executive control system47,48,49. Specifically, theta-synchronizations between anterior cingulate cortex (ACC)/mid-frontal cortical areas and the basal ganglia, nucleus accumbens and prefrontal cortical areas have been associated with planning, strategic readjustment and adaptation48, as well as key elements of executive control–conflict processing, detection of errors, inhibition, and performance monitoring43,49,50,51.

To shed light on the connectivity patterns within FP and MF networks during meditation, we recorded 64-channel electroencephalographic (EEG) activity and calculated the spatial synchronization of EEG oscillations from different frequency bands. Patterns of spatial EEG synchronization were explored using the imaginary part of coherence (ICoh), a refined measure of connectivity52, after spatially enhancing the EEG signals.

Meditation expertise

In the current study, highly experienced meditators were involved. Working with these virtuoso meditators offers a high degree of certainty that our participants are able to voluntarily enter and sustain the targeted mental states. Furthermore, we expected meditation training to be accompanied by neuroplastic and neurofunctional changes of relevant brain networks similar to other types of skill training. Including meditation experts with high levels of expertise may thus indicate how regular meditation practice shapes the dynamics of conventional brain FP and MF systems.

In summary, the aim of the present study was to characterize the functional connectivity of FP and MF networks in FAM, OMM and LKM in long-term practitioners.

Materials and methods

Participants

The sample of highly experienced meditators involved in our previous study12 was used to explore the role of cognitive control networks in meditation states. We studied 22 healthy right-handed volunteers (mean age = 44.2 years, SD = 9.9 years, age range 26–70 years, 4 females) who did not report any history of somatic disorders or neurological diseases. The participants were monks, nuns and novice practitioners residing at Amaravati Buddhist Monastery, in Southern England, or at Santacittarama Monastery, in Central Italy. Meditation practices at both monasteries are aligned with the Thai Forest Theravada Buddhist tradition, which is well established in the West. In this tradition, meditators practice FAM (Śamatha), OMM (Vipassana) and LKM (Metta) meditation forms in a balanced way, often in integrated sessions, including silent meditation retreats (at least 3 months per year). We estimated the meditation expertise of participants in hours, combining self-reported practice time before and during their monastic life. In this tradition, the monks, nuns, and novice practitioners typically practice 2 h per day with the monastery community, with regular more intensive practice during retreats (with several meditation sittings during the 3-month Winter retreat and other retreats). As suggested by the abbots of the monasteries, we estimated an average of 100 h of practice per month during monastic life, with a balance of FAM, OMM and LKM facets of meditation. The lifetime duration of meditation practice of the participants was estimated as a mean value = 19,358 h (SE = 3164), range 900–50,600 h. In line with the literature, and although characterized by intrinsic methodological problems, this estimation was based on an accurate recollection by the monastics, which was performed during several days before the experiment, by taking into account the average duration of daily practice over life periods with a consistent practice, as well as the average duration of daily practice during retreat periods and the number of retreat days. Furthermore, such recollection was facilitated by the practice regularities in the monastic life, both in terms of daily meditation sessions and intensive practice during retreats with established durations and routines over the years. The estimation of lifetime meditation practice was given by the sum of the hours of meditation practice during monastic life and the hours of comparable meditation practice before entering monastic life.

The study received approval by the Research Ethics Committee at Sapienza University of Rome, Italy. Prior to engaging with the study, all participants gave informed consent in line with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Experimental design

As in12, participants had to perform a non-meditative rest condition (REST) and three meditation conditions: FAM, OMM and LKM. They were verbally instructed to switch between conditions. The instructions for the four conditions, which were written together with the abbot of Amaravati Monastery, the internationally recognized teacher Ajahn Amaro, were as follows:

Rest: “Rest in a non-meditative relaxed state, without falling in sleep, while allowing any spontaneous thoughts and feelings to arise and unfold in the field of experience”.

Focused attention (Śamatha) meditation: “Sustain the focus of attention on breath sensations, such as at the nostrils, noticing readily and with acceptance any arising distraction, such as on thoughts or stimuli, and in case of detected distraction, return readily and gently to focus attention on the breath sensations”.

Open monitoring (awareness) meditation: “With an open receptive awareness, observe the contents of experience as they arise, change and fade from moment to moment, without restrictions or judgments—such contents including breath and body sensations, sensations arising from contact with external stimuli, feelings and thoughts”.

Loving kindness (Metta) meditation: “Generate and sustain metta, acceptance and friendliness towards yourself and the experience in the present moment, as well as towards any being, in any state or condition”.

EEG recordings and processing

Procedure: EEG was recorded with eyes closed in blocks of approximately 2.5–3 min each, repeating the sequence REST, FAM, OMM, LKM twice12. Thus, a total of approximately 5–6 min (2 blocks × 2.5–3 min) of data were recorded for each condition. EEG was acquired by a mobile wireless system (Cognionics; https://www.cognionics.net/mobile-128) using an electrode cap with 64 active Ag/AgCl electrodes located in accordance with the extended international 10/20 system and referenced to linked mastoids. Electrode impedances were kept below 10 kOhm and EEG signals were collected at a sampling rate of 500 Hz (resampled off-line to 250 Hz for data analysis). The experiment was conducted at the two monasteries in a quiet, dark room suitable for meditation and recording EEG. Participants were tested one at a time.

Analyses were performed by means of Brain Vision Analyzer 2.2 (Brain Products GmbH, Germany) and by custom software developed in Matlab R2013b (The MathWorks Inc.). EEG epochs with noise or non-physiological artefacts were removed after visual inspection. Bad channels were interpolated according to Hjorth53. For control of ocular artefacts, vertical and horizontal electro-oculogram (EOG) was also recorded and all EEG traces were EOG corrected by means of independent component analysis (ICA54).

To improve spatial resolution and reduce the influence of volume conduction between electrodes, current source density (CSD) was additionally applied with the parameters: order of splines = 10, maximal degree of Legendre polynomials = 4, lambda = 1E−7. Because of missing all neighbour electrodes, edge electrodes were excluded from all analyses, so that the number of channels was finally reduced to 50. All analyses were carried out with CSD transformed data from 50 electrodes12.

Transformation of EEG signals to frequency domain

As in12, EEG recordings from all conditions were segmented in equal-sized epochs of 4.096 s duration with 1.024 s overlap. The average number of epochs for each condition/participant was 70 (± 20). A Hanning window with a duration of 20% from the total epoch length was applied to all epochs. This was performed by multiplying the sampled signal values (EEG) by the Hanning function. In such a way, the ends of the time record are forced smoothly to zero regardless of the signal at those parts. This approach is useful when performing spectral analysis on segmented data and avoids artefacts due to abrupt change in the amplitude of the signal at both ends of the segment. After that the fast Fourier transform was computed, yielding the representation of complex values (real and imaginary parts) with a frequency resolution of 0.244 Hz (1/4.096 s).

Imaginary part of coherence

Coherence function is a measure of the linear relationship between two signals at a specific frequency (e.g.55). In practice, the magnitude of the coherency function is generally used as a measure of coherence. Coherence function Cxy(f) is defined as the normalized cross-spectrum (the cross-correlation between two time series in the frequency domain):

$${C}_{xy}\left(f\right)=\frac{\sum Sxy\left(f\right)}{\sqrt{\sum Sxx(f)\sum Syy(f)}},$$

where Sxy is the cross-spectral density (cross-spectral energy distribution per unit time) between two signals, and Sxx and Syy are the autospectral densities (energy distribution per unit time computed for one time series) for signals x and y, respectively. Coherence is defined as the magnitude of coherence function:

$${Coh}_{xy}\left(f\right)=\left|{C}_{xy}\left(f\right)\right|=\left|\frac{\sum Sxy\left(f\right)}{\sqrt{\sum Sxx(f)\sum Syy(f)}}\right|.$$

The estimated coherence for a given frequency ranges between 0 and 1, with a value of 0 indicating that the two signals are perfectly uncorrelated, and a value of 1 indicating the perfect correlation.

Instead of estimating the magnitude of the coherence function, Nolte et al.52 proposed using only the imaginary part of the coherency to investigate directed brain interactions. This is because the imaginary part of the coherency excludes coherent sources with zero phase lag and therefore reduces the effect of field spread due to volume conduction. The imaginary part of coherency (ICoh) is defined as

$${ICoh}_{xy}\left(f\right)=Im\left({C}_{xy}\left(f\right)\right)=Im\left(\frac{\sum Sxy\left(f\right)}{\sqrt{\sum Sxx(f)\sum Syy(f)}}\right) ,$$

where Im denotes the imaginary part. The imaginary part of coherency for a given frequency varies between − 1 and 1. If the value is positive, signals x and y are interacting and y is earlier than x, indicating that information is flowing from y to x. For a similar approach, see Yordanova et al.12.

Connectivity analysis of EEG signals

The imaginary part of coherency is only sensitive to functional coupling between two signals which are time-lagged to each other. Therefore, as far as volume conduction does not cause a time-lag, this measure is less sensitive to common sources effects52. ICoh was obtained for all electrode combinations for each condition and each participant. In this study, we investigated the absolute value of ICoh. Additionally, before submitting it to statistical analysis, ICoh was three-point smoothed and, in order to normalize distribution, Fisher z-transformation was applied. Fisher z-transformation is given by the equation:

$$z\, = \,1/2 \, \ln \left( {\left( {1\, + \,r} \right)/\left( {1 - r} \right)} \right)\, = \,\arctan h\left( r \right),$$

where ln is the natural logarithm, arctanh is the inverse hyperbolic tangent function, and r is the actual value. Measurable parameters were ICoh values for frequency ranges theta (4.1–7.1 Hz), slow alpha (8.8–12 Hz), fast alpha (12.2–14.9 Hz), and beta (15–20 Hz)12.

Statistical analysis of FP and MF networks

The statistical evaluation of FP and MF networks were performed for selected electrode pairs using analysis of variance with repeated measures (ANOVA), analysis of covariance with Mediation Expertise as a covariate (ANCOVA), correlation and linear and quadratic regression analyses, as detailed in the results. The difference values for all variables included in the ANOVAs and ANCOVAs were tested for normality of distribution using Kolmogorov–Smirnov and Shapiro–Wilk tests. For none of the variables was p < 0.05 indicating normality of distribution (Shapiro–Wilk test, Statistic > 0.9, df = 22).

Results

Effects of meditation states on fronto-parietal networks

To assess the effects of meditation on FP networks, specific frontal-parietal electrode pairs were selected for analysis. As depicted in Fig. 1, the pairs included four frontal (F5, F3, F4, F6) and four parietal (P5, P3, P4, P6) left- and right-hemisphere electrodes. The resulting 16 frontal-parietal pairs captured the connections of each frontal electrode with each of four parietal ones and vice versa. These electrodes were chosen to cover a possible distinction between dorsal and ventral attention networks28,42,56. To assess specific effects of meditation states on FP connectivity, difference ICoh values were computed by subtracting ICoh during rest from ICoh in a given meditation condition in a pair-wise manner. Hence, positive values reflect meditation-related increases. Difference ICoh values were subjected to a 3 × 4 × 4 repeated-measures analysis of variance with within-subjects factors Meditation Condition (FAM vs. OMM vs. LKM), Frontal Nodes (4 levels corresponding to 4 frontal electrodes) and Parietal Nodes (4 levels corresponding to 4 parietal electrodes). This analysis design was chosen to assess the possible contribution of specific nodes in the fronto-parietal networks, which would produce main Frontal or Parietal effects, in addition to the contribution of single pairs, which would be reflected by Frontal × Parietal interactions. In a second analysis-step, we included Meditation Expertise (in hours) as a covariate to test if expertise influenced the involvement of FP networks during specific meditation states. Analyses were performed for theta, slow alpha, fast alpha, and beta frequency bands following previous research according to which FP networks may operate in these frequency ranges40,41,42.

Figure 1
figure 1

Schematic presentation of attentional and cognitive monitoring networks: fronto-parietal (left) and medial frontal (right). Respective electrodes are labelled according to the International 10–20 system. Electrodes used for analyses are presented in black.

As depicted in Fig. 2, in the theta range FP networks manifested a left-hemisphere increase in connectivity guided by left parietal nodes (Parietal Node, F(3/63) = 4.9, p = 0.03, \({\eta }_{p}^{2}=\) 0.19). This effect did not depend on the type of meditation (no interaction with Meditation Condition, F(6/126) = 1.1, p > 0.3). Adding Meditation Expertise as a covariate did not yield any significant interactions.

Figure 2
figure 2

Connectivity of fronto-parietal theta networks during meditation. Left: Group mean ± SE of ICoh difference (meditation minus rest) in the theta frequency band (4–7 Hz); FAM, OMM and LKM pooled together. ICoh difference for each fronto-parietal electrode pair is presented, e.g., P5-F5, P5-F3, P5-F4, P5-F6, etc. Parietal nodes P5, P3, P4 and P6 are marked in yellow—X-axis, whereas the frontal nodes are in different colours as indicated in the figure (F5, F3, F4, F6). Y-axis—non-dimensional ICoh difference values. Right: Graphical presentation of same results. Thickness of connections codes the value of ICoh difference, as indicated. Colours of nodes correspond to the colours used to denote parietal and frontal electrodes in the left panel. The meditation-related intra-hemispheric increase in theta-ICoh in the left hemisphere, common to FAM, OMM and LKM, is demonstrated.

No significant results emerged for slow alpha FP networks, nor were there any interactions with Meditation Expertise. Fast alpha FP networks were not modified by Meditation Condition, but depended on Meditation Expertise, as revealed by a significant ANCOVA Frontal Nodes × Parietal Nodes × Meditation Expertise interaction (F(9/180) = 3.5, p = 0.03, \({\upeta }_{\mathrm{p}}^{2}=\) 1.15). Specifically, for all meditation conditions, fast alpha synchronization increased with increasing expertise for the connections linking frontal and parietal electrodes of opposite hemispheres. Multivariate ANCOVA (MANCOVA) used to test the effect of Meditation Expertise for each single frontal-parietal pair verified that hours of practice predicted the increased fast alpha ICoh only for inter-hemispheric FP connections. This effect was most prominent for pairs linking right frontal (F4, F6) with left parietal (P5, P3) nodes (Meditation Expertise, F(1/20) > 4.5, p < 0.05, \({\upeta }_{\mathrm{p}}^{2}\) > 0.19)—Fig. 3 (left). Accordingly, only for these pairs, Pearson correlation coefficients were r = − 0.4 ÷ − 0.5, p < 0.01. Importantly, intra-hemispheric frontal-parietal connections were not modulated by expertise (p > 0.2)—Fig. 3 (right).

Figure 3
figure 3

Correlation between fast alpha (12–15 Hz) ICoh and meditation expertise. Individual difference ICoh values are presented (meditation minus rest); FAM, OMM and LKM pooled together for each individual difference ICoh value. Y-axis—non-dimensional ICoh difference values. The meditation-related inter-hemispheric linear increase of fast alpha ICoh as a function of meditation expertise is depicted on the left for right frontal (F4, F6)—left parietal (P3, P5) electrode pairs. The lack of correlation between meditation-related intra-hemispheric modulation of fast alpha ICoh as a function of meditation expertise is shown on the right.

Likewise, no main or interaction effects of Meditation Condition were found for FP networks in the beta range. The significant Frontal Node × Parietal Node × Meditation Expertise interaction yielded by ANCOVA (F(9/180) = 3.5, p = 0.05, \({\upeta }_{\mathrm{p}}^{2}=\) 0.17), reflected a practice-dependent increase in right frontal—left parietal connections in the beta range, similar to those found in the fast alpha range (MANCOVA, F(1/20) > 4.3, p < 0.05, \({\upeta }_{\mathrm{p}}^{2}\) > 0.18; Pearson correlation coefficients, r = − 0.4 ÷ − 0.5, p < 0.05).

These results indicate that the specific types of meditation did not induce distinctive involvement of FP networks: Intra-hemispheric FP theta connectivity was enhanced in the left hemisphere in all three types of meditation (FAM, OMM, and LKM) relative to REST. A second common feature of all three meditation types was that inter-hemispheric frontal-parietal connections in fast alpha and beta bands linking mostly right-frontal regions with left-parietal regions manifested increased connectivity as a function of meditation expertise.

Effects of meditation on the medial frontal network

As shown in Fig. 1, the cognitive monitoring (MF) network was assessed using ICoh measures of all pairs of electrodes guided by the FCz electrode. It has been demonstrated that the midfronto-central region represents a fulcrum capturing and controlling activity from the dorsal ACC and medial frontal lobe responsible for performance monitoring and re-adjustment48, where the connections with dorsolateral frontal and parietal regions are of specific relevance47. As in the previous analysis, difference ICoh values were used. They were obtained by subtracting measures during REST from measures during each of the three meditation states, separately for theta, slow alpha, fast alpha, and beta frequency bands. In the present analysis, five frontal (F5, F3, Fz, F4, F6), central (C5, C3, Cz, C4, C6), centro-parietal (CP5, CP3, CPz, CP4, CP6), parietal (P5, P3, Pz, P4, P6), parieto-occipital (P7, PO3, POz, PO4, P8) and occipital (PO7, O1, Oz, O2, PO8) electrodes were used to form the Region (6 levels) and Laterality (5 levels) within-subjects variables of a repeated measures ANOVA. A third within-subjects factor was Meditation Condition with three levels (FAM, OMM, LKM). As a second step, ANCOVA was run by including Meditation Expertise as a covariate.

No significant main or interaction effects were found for slow and fast alpha frequency bands. As detailed below, MF networks in the theta and beta frequency ranges were involved in sustaining specific meditation states.

Figure 4 demonstrates that during all meditation conditions, MF theta networks manifested increased synchronization at posterior regions (centro-parietal, parietal and parieto-occipital) of the left hemisphere (Laterality, F(4/84) = 5.9, p = 0.005, \({\upeta }_{\mathrm{p}}^{2}=\) 2.2; Region × Laterality, F(20/420) = 2.98, p < 0.02, \({\upeta }_{\mathrm{p}}^{2}=\) 0.12). Adding Meditation Expertise as a covariate revealed that Meditation Expertise predicted theta synchronization in specific regions depending on the meditation condition (Meditation Condition × Region × Laterality × Meditation Expertise, F(40/800) = 2.1, p = 0.04, \({\upeta }_{\mathrm{p}}^{2}=\) 0.1). MANCOVA tests of the effect of Meditation Expertise for single electrode pairs in each meditation condition demonstrated that during FAM, theta synchronization between FCz and left parietal regions (P7, P5, P3, CP5) increased as a function of expertise (F(1/20) > 4.4, p < 0.05, \({\upeta }_{\mathrm{p}}^{2}\) > 0.18), whereas during LKM, Meditation Expertise co-varied with the increased theta synchronization between FCz and right frontal regions F6, F4, Fz (F(1/20) > 8.5, p < 0.008, \({\upeta }_{\mathrm{p}}^{2}\) > 0.29). These relationships are shown in Fig. 5.

Figure 4
figure 4

Connectivity of the medial frontal network during meditation. Topography maps of grand average ICoh difference are presented (FAM, OMM and LKM minus REST). Upper row presents theta frequency range (4–7 Hz), and lower row—beta frequency range (15–20 Hz). Red colour indicates meditation-related increase.

Figure 5
figure 5

Linear associations between theta (4–7 Hz) ICoh of the medial frontal network (presented as individual difference values, meditation minus rest) and meditation expertise. Upper panel: Graphical maps indicating relevant electrodes where the correlations between theta ICoh and meditation expertise were significant (red circles and connections). Lower panel: Linear increase of theta ICoh as a function of meditation expertise. Positive ICoh difference values reflect meditation-related increase. The linear increase of theta ICoh for FCz-guided left parieto-occipital pairs as a function of meditation expertise is demonstrated for FAM (left); the linear increase of theta ICoh for FCz-guided right frontal pairs as a function of meditation expertise is demonstrated for LKM (right). Y-axis presents non-dimensional ICoh difference values.

Figure 4 further demonstrates that in the beta range FAM was associated with increased synchronization of MF connections in the right hemisphere, whereas OMM was associated with increased synchronization in the left posterior hemisphere (Meditation Condition × Laterality, F(8/168) = 3.4, p = 0.03, \({\upeta }_{\mathrm{p}}^{2}=\) 0.14). Accordingly, there was a significant difference between FAM and OMM over the left hemisphere (F(1/21) = 6.4, p = 0.02, \({\upeta }_{\mathrm{p}}^{2}=\) 0.23), and at posterior regions of the right hemisphere (F(1/21) = 3.7, p = 0.05, \({\upeta }_{\mathrm{p}}^{2} =\) 0.18). During LKM, left posterior and right anterior increases in synchronization were observed (Meditation Condition × Region × Laterality, F(40/840) = 2.2, p = 0.04, \({\upeta }_{\mathrm{p}}^{2}=\) 0.1; Region × Laterality in LKM, F(20/420) = 2.5, p = 0.05, \({\upeta }_{\mathrm{p}}^{2}=\) 0.11). However, no significant main or interaction effects of the covariate Meditation Expertise were found.

Regression analyses

To further explore the effects of the Meditation Expertise covariate in light of previous results57, measures of FP and MF networks were included in linear and quadratic regression models to test for the existence of linear or non-linear, U-shaped dependencies between meditation-specific changes in connectivity and Meditation Expertise. Confirming results of MANCOVAs, the analyses yielded significant solutions of linear regression models for inter-hemispheric FP synchronization in fast alpha and beta bands (F(1/21) = 3.8 ÷ 6.8, p = 0.05 ÷ 0.01). Specifically, increasing practice duration was associated with increased synchronization in the fast alpha and beta ranges between frontal and parietal regions of opposite hemispheres (Fig. 3). Likewise, for MF networks, only synchronization in the theta frequency range was linearly associated with Meditation Expertise such that long-duration practice predicted increased theta synchronization between the medial fronto-central region and the left parietal hemisphere during FAM, and the right frontal regions during LKM (F(1/21) = 4.5 ÷ 8.5, p = 0.05 ÷ 0.008)—Fig. 5.

Notably, only for FP theta synchronization, there were significant solutions in the quadratic models. As illustrated in Fig. 6, intra-hemispheric connections in the right hemisphere manifested a U-shape dependency on Meditation Expertise indicating more pronounced increase in synchronization in least and most experienced meditators (F(2/19) = 3.5 ÷ 10.9, p = 0.05 ÷ 0.001). These quadratic models proved significant for all meditation states, pointing to a generalized effect of Meditation Expertise on changes of FP theta synchronization, irrespective of the type of meditation.

Figure 6
figure 6

Regression analyses for intra-hemispheric connections in the right hemisphere for theta frequency range vs. meditation expertise. Individual difference ICoh values are included for FAM, OMM and LKM (meditation minus rest). Positive ICoh difference values reflect meditation-related increase. Right hemisphere pairs are indicated above each graph. Y-axis—non-dimensional ICoh difference values. U-shape dependency on meditation expertise indicating a most pronounced increase in synchronization in least and most experienced meditators is demonstrated.

In contrast, as depicted in Fig. 7, significant inverted U-shape solutions were yielded for the left-hemisphere intra-hemispheric connections during LKM indicating a least pronounced synchronization for least and most experienced meditators (F(2/19) = 3.5 ÷ 6.5, p = 0.05 ÷ 0.007). These inverted U-shape associations between intra-hemispheric FP connections in the left hemisphere and Meditation Expertise were also found during FAM and OMM but they did not reach significance.

Figure 7
figure 7

Results from all left and right within-hemisphere fronto-parietal (F-P) pairs for LKM. Clear opposite tendencies in the two hemispheres compared to Fig. 6 are illustrated. Designations are the same as in Fig. 6.

Control analyses

As control analyses, ICoh values were computed for 11 individuals (mean age = 45.5 ± 12.6 years, 9 females) with less than 250 h of meditation experience in secular mindfulness or Buddhist traditions. These short-term meditators further practiced the instructions of FAM, OMM and LKM for 10 days before the study, 20 min per day for each form of meditation. EEG records and analyses were conducted following the same design and procedures used for data acquisition and analysis in experienced meditators. The only significant result that was yielded was an increased involvement of frontal nodes of slow alpha FP network in the right hemisphere (Frontal Node, F(3/30) = 4.8, p = 0.03), which was similar across three meditation states (Meditation Condition × Frontal Node, F(6/60) = 0.1, p > 0.9).

In another control analysis, we tested if the common involvement of left posterior theta FP and MF networks may reflect a generalized network activity induced by the integrated meditation training of participants or carry over effects from one meditation state to another in the current experimental design. To check for the existence of such a generalized network we performed analyses, in which we used the original (un-subtracted) ICoh values in the theta band. It was hypothesized that a common connectivity pattern would be shared across all three meditation styles. Although non-significant, the observations provided evidence that despite the integrated training of participants, and despite the integrated experimental session including the three meditation styles, the connectivity patterns of FP and MF networks were distinct across meditation states (see Supplementary Information). Therefore, a generalized network may not be responsible for the specific left-lateralized increase in FP and MF connectivity common for all states of mediation. Rather, an enhanced involvement of left-lateralized nodes, irrespective of style-specific network properties, appears as a common activation induced by any type of meditation.

Discussion

Executive top-down processes guiding voluntary allocation of attention are associated with the fronto-parietal brain networks26,29,58,59,60, whereas executive processes of cognitive monitoring are mainly linked to the medial frontal networks27,44,45,49. The present study characterized the functional connectivity of these FP and MF networks in major forms of meditation (focused attention, open monitoring and loving kindness meditation) in long-term practitioners, as contrasted with a non-meditative resting state. The main objective was to explore the role of executive brain processes in sustaining different forms of meditation as a function of meditation expertise6,15,22. Toward this end, we analysed functional connectivity patterns of FP and MF networks in terms of coherent theta, slow and fast alpha, and beta oscillations, and explored their correlation with meditation expertise.

The results demonstrate that, compared to unexperienced practitioners, meditation states in long-term practitioners induced highly specific connectivity patterns of fronto-parietal and medial frontal networks relative to rest. This observation generally indicates that the executive processes of attentional control and cognitive monitoring have a specific role in supporting brain states of meditation.

Common and distinct patterns of FP and MF networks in FAM, OMM and LKM

The first major finding of this study is that the synchronization of fronto-parietal networks did not differentiate FAM, OMM, and LKM conditions in any of the analyzed frequency bands. Yet, FP networks in long-term meditators manifested an identifiable pattern of organization appearing as a common activation feature of all three types of meditation, which did not exist in novice meditators. In particular, the left parietal nodes of the FP theta networks exhibited increased synchronization with frontal regions in the same (left) hemisphere. Medial frontal networks in long-term practitioners manifested a similar common theta synchronization pattern. These findings demonstrate that the state of meditation in meditation experts engages common executive attention and cognitive monitoring mechanisms supported by synchronized theta networks in the left hemisphere. Meditation style was only distinguished by cognitive monitoring mechanisms associated with lateralized synchronization of beta networks.

We specifically targeted FP and MF networks by focusing on established nodes of these networks26,29,42,43,48,61,62. The observed patterns of enhanced theta connectivity of the left parietal region, which was common to FP and MF networks, and the distinctive lateralized beta pattern, which differentiated the MF network in FAM and OMM, resemble the more general picture of connectivity extracted in an initial analysis of the same data set12. In that study, a wide range of connections (not limited to FP and MF electrode pairs), was used. Similar to the present results, the functional connectivity of multiple theta connections was increased in the left hemisphere in all forms of meditation, and lateralized beta connectivity differentiated the FAM and OMM styles.

The current targeted analysis of FP and MF networks indicates that the previously identified unique centre of increased theta connectivity in the left posterior regions is associated with the executive attentional and cognitive monitoring systems when experts meditate. In support, theta has previously been identified as a major operating rhythm of cognitive control47, which supports the controlled signalling from prefrontal to medial frontal regions63, the cognitive monitoring and coordinating functions of the MF system43,48,49,62,64, and the guided voluntary allocation of focused attention mediated by FP networks40,42,60. The common engagement of theta frequency is also consistent with previously found shared involvement of medial frontal structures (pre/supplementary motor cortices, dorsal anterior cingulate cortex) and fronto-polar cortex in different meditation styles including FAM, OMM and LKM20.

Localizing the theta synchronization hub in parietal regions is in line with existing neurocognitive models regarding the role of these regions. These models propose that the core function of the lateral parietal activity either is to support bottom-up attentional capture65,66, or to mediate a “cross-modal integrative hub” combining bottom-up multimodal inputs with top-down controlling signals67,68. More specifically, this multi-modal hub is assumed to integrate information in order to enhance awareness (comprehension and meaning), manipulate mental events, and reorient attention to relevant information68. In a similar manner, the lateral parietal area has also been implicated in the formation of attentional “priority maps” which represent environmental bottom-up features selected by the top-down focus of attention and are dynamically shaped by task-specific motor, cognitive and motivational variables69. The present results suggest that in experienced meditators such an integrative cognitive control hub in the left hemisphere is enhanced during all three meditation types (Figs. 4 and 5). This suggestion is in line with (1) fMRI and MEG findings of a prominent cluster of left-hemispheric activation across different meditation types16,70,71, (2) evidences about more expressed structural differences in the left hemisphere of long-term meditation practitioners than in novices72,73,74, (3) current theoretical developments emphasizing on the leading role of the left hemisphere in executive top-down regulation of meditation states22.

The currently found hemispheric asymmetry can be understood within several perspectives. One account is a possible lateralized involvement of dorsal and ventral attention networks26, whereby the dorsal network controlling top-down focusing, orienting and sustaining attention is bi-lateral, and the ventral network controlling attention re-orienting to distracting but behaviourally relevant or salient events is right-lateralized26,28,29,75. Since the ventral network has been found to be suppressed during top-down guided focused attention to protect goal-driven behaviour56,76,77, the observed here left-hemisphere dominance might emerge. Second, Posner and Rothbart27 posit that executive influences on awareness involve differentially the two hemispheres: The right hemisphere may be engaged in top-down controlled sensory awareness, whereas the left hemisphere may be responsible for directing motor attention in relation to voluntary initiation and control of behaviour78,79,80,81. Although the leftward lateralization during meditation may have a complex origin, the present results demonstrate its dominant role in top-down executive functions during all three forms of meditation in long-term practitioners.

Regarding the neural coupling in the beta range, medial-frontal connections were more strongly synchronized in the left hemisphere for OMM and in the right hemisphere for FAM. This condition-dependent lateralization of beta synchronization corresponds to the broader pattern reported in our previous analysis of the data12. However, the current analysis qualifies this, in that the lateralized beta connectivity was only observed for the MF cognitive monitoring network and not for the attentional FP networks. This observation helps to refine the functional involvement of beta oscillations during meditation. It implies that the lateralized beta synchronization may be associated with a top-down amplification of attended information82, or with mediating the link between top-down attentional selection and awareness42,83,84,85,86. Also, since medial frontal beta activity has been associated with monitoring of conflicts and subsequent behavioural adjustments and adaptation64, the right- vs. left lateralization of MF beta connectivity in FAM vs. OMM may reflect different types of conflict monitoring, distracter inhibition and adaptation: sensory during FAM and behavioural during OMM78,80.

Modulation of FP and MF networks by meditation expertise: common and distinct patterns of modulation

The present results demonstrate that connectivity of FP and MF networks depend on the extent of meditation expertise. Importantly, this expertise-dependent modulation was different for fronto-parietal and medial frontal networks, suggesting an overall difference between neuroplastic dynamics of these executive networks in the course of meditation practice.

Fronto-parietal modulations by expertise were observed for all three meditation types, suggesting a generalized effect of meditation training on FP functions. Also, the effects of expertise on FP networks were different according to network frequency. Specifically, intra-hemispheric FP theta connections depended non-linearly on expertise following a U-shape model. In contrast, inter-hemispheric FP connections in fast alpha and beta bands manifested a linear increase in connectivity as a function of expertise.

Since the expertise-dependent U-shaped trajectory of FP theta connectivity was present in all three meditation states (FAM, OMM, LKM), it seems unlikely that practice-related neuroplasticity reflects changes in the control of attentional focus (narrow vs. wide), or in the amount or specificity of the content of meditation (e.g., object-specific, objectless, emotional/mental state). Rather, with regard to the previously reported domain-unspecific effects of task difficulty on the activity in lateral parietal sub-regions67, non-linear modulations by expertise may reflect training-related changes in mental/neurocomputational effort. From this cognitive effort perspective, an initial phase of increased mobilization of neuronal resources in response to training, wouldbe followed by a more efficient use of (less) neuronal resources57,87. This predictethrough continued practice—be followed by a more efficient use of (less) neuronal resources57,87. This predicted non-linear dependency of FP theta connectivity was only observed in the left hemisphere, appearing stable in LKM (Fig. 7), possibly in relation to the added load of emotional/motivational processing in this condition88,89.

A further intriguing observation is the opposing pattern of expertise-dependent FP theta synchronization in the two hemispheres. These co-existing opposite effects suggest that top-down attentional regulation of effort is under dynamic inter-hemispheric control in terms of a counter-balanced involvement of the left- and the right-hemisphere FP networks. Long-term meditation training may therefore disentangle the two hemispheres leading to alternating dominance of either the left or the right hemisphere (i.e., a dominating role of left-lateralized FP networks in least and most experienced meditators, with a reverse pattern—a dominating role of right-lateralized FP networks—in intermediate-level meditators). In other words, neuroplasticity due to meditation training may be linked to dynamic shifts of attentional effort control across hemispheres. The results further suggest that attentional effort control can be guided predominantly by either the left or the right hemisphere depending on the level of attentional training and/or simultaneously activated processes26,28,29,39. Importantly, these non-linear modulations of theta FP networks by expertise coexisted with a progressive increase of inter-hemispheric fast-frequency FP connections. From a neuro-functional perspective, this result may reflect facilitation of conscious access42,82,84,85,86 and a transference of attention-dependent conscious access to higher-frequency networks, as suggested previously17,25. It may be further assumed that these differential frequency-dependent effects of expertise capture specific meditation-related neuroplasticity of FP networks associated with attentional control of effort and conscious access.

In parallel, effects of expertise were found for medial frontal theta networks. In contrast to FP networks, these expertise-dependent modulations were linear and also depended on the type of meditation: during FAM, theta synchronization increased as a function of meditation expertise at left parietal regions, whereas during LKM, it increased at right frontal regions, with no specific regions involved in OMM (Fig. 5). Since theta oscillations represent a major signal supporting the maintenance/monitoring of internal representations48,64,90,91,92, these observations imply that meditation training facilitates access to specific contents (mental representations) that require monitoring and adaptation depending on the type of meditation. Indeed, consistent with observations for LKM, functional imaging studies have demonstrated that the right prefrontal cortex may be particularly critical for voluntary regulation of emotions, and for suppression of negative emotions in particular, as well as for self-regulation and self-control93,94,95. Also, the right ventromedial cortex has been shown to have a unique role in integrating cognition and affect to produce the empathic response96. Likewise, consistent with current results for FAM, attention priority maps of sensorimotor goals appear to be supported by the parietal regions68,69. Accordingly, the lack of an intended mental object in OMM corresponds to the lack of any specific region of expertise-dependent increase of MF theta connectivity in this condition. In combination with practice-based modulations of FP networks, these region-specific effects of meditation expertise on MF theta connectivity may be linked to the progressively increasing subjective feeling of “effortless” maintenance of meditation states in experienced meditators.

Future longitudinal studies on long-term practice of FAM, OMM and LKM may provide further insights and sharpen the present findings, also considering the intrinsic limitations of cross-sectional studies and of a self-reported retrospective assessment of lifetime meditation expertise, which however in our study was enhanced by a careful recollection by the monastic participants, performed over several days, as well as by the practice regularities in monastic life.

Conclusions

In contrast to unexperienced meditators, the maintenance of meditation states in long-term practitioners is associated with specific connectivity patterns of FP and MF networks.

  1. (1)

    First, executive brain processes of both attention regulation and cognitive monitoring during meditation are supported by networks operating in two major frequency bands—theta and beta—consistent with major operating rhythms of conventional executive networks.

  2. (2)

    Theta and beta connectivity patterns of both FP and MF networks manifest a pronounced lateral asymmetry implying that a focused lateralization of executive systems may represent a major functional mechanism which supports the state of meditation in general, as well as specific meditation states.

  3. (3)

    Connectivity patterns of FP networks were common to all three meditation states, indicating a similar involvement of attentional control across meditation conditions in the course of meditation training. Only the connectivity of medial frontal networks differentiated FAM, OMM and LKM conditions indicating a specific functional relevance for cognitive monitoring in sustaining a particular meditation state.

  4. (4)

    Training of executive systems during long-term meditation practice was accompanied by both non-linear and linear modulations of FP and MF connectivity revealing that shifts in lateralization co-existed with facilitated interactions among lateralized patterns of executive functions. Such practice-based neuroplasticity of executive functions may subserve the emergence of unique mental states in meditation in highly experienced meditators.