Abstract
Steady-state visual evoked potentials (SSVEPs), the brain response to visual flicker stimulation, have proven beneficial in both research and clinical applications. Despite the practical advantages of stimulation at high frequencies in terms of visual comfort and safety, high frequency-SSVEPs have not received enough attention and little is known about the mechanisms behind their generation and propagation in time and space. In this study, we investigated the origin and propagation of SSVEPs in the gamma frequency band (40–60 Hz) by studying the dynamic properties of EEG in 32 subjects. Using low-resolution brain electromagnetic tomography (sLORETA) we identified the cortical sources involved in SSVEP generation in that frequency range to be in the primary visual cortex, Brodmann areas 17, 18 and 19 with minor contribution from sources in central and frontal sites. We investigated the SSVEP propagation as measured on the scalp in the framework of the existing theories regarding the neurophysiological mechanism through which the SSVEP spreads through the cortex. We found a progressive phase shift from posterior parieto-occipital sites over the cortex with a phase velocity of approx. 8–14 m/s and wavelength of about 21 and 24 cm. The SSVEP spatial properties appear sensitive to input frequency with higher stimulation frequencies showing a faster propagation speed.
Introduction
Repetitive visual stimulation (RVS), or visual flicker, at a frequency in the range from 1 to 100 Hz elicits an oscillatory response in the electroencephalogram (EEG) at the same frequency and/or its harmonics known as steady-state visual evoked potentials (SSVEPs). This response is time and phase-locked to the driving stimulus. It appears most prominently over parietal and occipital areas as they are closer to the visual information processing centers of the brain cortex but can also be detected in central and frontal areas1.
Because of their high signal to noise ratio and robustness to artifacts, SSVEPs have proven beneficial in both research and clinical applications.
The wide spatial extent of the SSVEPs across the scalp can provide means for studying the collective behaviour of large groups of neurons2 allowing the investigation of neural processes underlying brain rhythmic activity and functional brain connectivity. When RVS is superimposed on a cognitive task, especially around the alpha frequency band (8–12 Hz), specific topographic changes in amplitude and phase of the SSVEP appear analogous to the site-specific reductions in alpha EEG amplitude associated with cognitive and motor tasks3. This technique is known as steady-state topography and has been applied for studying working memory3,4, binocular rivalry5, selective attention6,7, vigilance8 and even emotions9.
SSVEPs are also used as a diagnostic tool to study pathological brain dynamics and as an investigative probe of the cortical mechanisms of visual perception. The amplitude and phase of the SSVEP vary as a function of stimulus parameters such as temporal and spatial frequency, contrast, luminance, hue, visual angle and spatial attention1,10,11,12,13 and can be a sensitive electrophysiological index of a variety of visual-perceptual and cognitive functions14. SSVEP-based techniques such as photic driving and sweep-VEP are used for evaluating visual acuity of infants and children15, studying infant vision16, assessment of neuro-visual function17, spatial neglect18 and epilepsy19. SSVEPs are also applied for the investigation and treatment monitoring of age-related and neurodegenerative disorders, such as Alzheimer’s disease, Parkinson, Schizophrenia and others8,20,21. They could also be an effective diagnostic tool for detecting or predicting age-related cognitive decline22.
Lately the topic of controlled entrainment of perceptually relevant brain oscillations by non-invasive rhythmic stimulation in a frequency-specific manner has become increasingly popular in research of brain rhythms23. Peripheral stimulation, including sensory and transcranial stimulation, could be used to probe the causal role of oscillatory brain activity on cognition and emotion. To show causality here, we need to verify that evoked oscillatory signatures, which are already well-studied correlates of particular behaviour outcome, do give rise to a similar behaviour outcome23. A number of groups has shown a promising behavioral and electrophysiological evidence that external rhythmic stimulation protocols may change attention and perception24,25, affect memory26, enhance sleep27 and even attenuate Alzheimer’s disease-associated pathologies28 by potentially reducing amyloid load29.
SSVEPs are also widely used in overt and covert attention based brain–computer interfaces (BCIs). In BCIs visual stimuli having distinctive properties (e.g., frequency or phase) are simultaneously presented to a user who selects among several commands by focusing his/her attention on the corresponding stimulus. Most current SSVEP-based BCIs use frequencies between 5 and 40 Hz, which may cause discomfort and induce fatigue. A safety risk also exists because RVS in that range may cause epileptic seizures30 or headache and eye strain complaints31 and, therefore, higher frequencies are preferable. The signal-to-noise ratio of HF-SSVEP (high frequency-SSVEP) is as high as that of LF-SSVEP (low frequency-SSVEP), which makes HF-SSVEP an ideal candidate for practical BCIs32,33 due to the minimal or even absence of user discomfort vis-a-vis the flicker34. HF-SSVEP elicited by stimulation frequencies beyond the critical perception frequency have been successfully used for BCI applications35. SSVEPs elicited by frequencies that are only 0.2 Hz apart from each other can be distinguished in the EEG36.
The broad range of SSVEP applications in research and clinical practice come with their requirements regarding safety, comfort and ease of use. Higher stimulation frequencies and sparse electrode arrays are often preferred as a more practical alternative. This could potentially speed up system preparation, increase the amount of people tested, and avoid known side effects of prolonged visual stimulation. Here, care should be taken translating existing knowledge regarding the SSVEP phenomena and better understanding of the spread and the propagation of the response in scalp recorded EEG is needed.
Despite the years of investigation, the mechanisms behind SSVEP generation are still not fully understood. Vialatte et al.37 identify three concurrent theories behind SSVEP origin and propagation. According to the first one SSVEPs originate in the primary visual cortex and propagate by the combined activity of locally and broadly distributed sources38. The second one argues that SSVEPs are generated by a finite number of electrical dipoles that are activated sequentially in time, starting with a dipole located in the primary visual areas14. While isolated dipoles account for the strongest activity, they may not sufficiently describe the complex distribution of electrical activity in the brain, thus, the third theory2 suggests that VEPs originate in the primary visual cortex, and propagate to other brain areas through cortical travelling and standing waves. These theories, do have in common the fact that SSVEPs propagate, starting from the primary visual cortex, and involve more than one single source dipole.
Cortical activation of steady-state and transient visual stimuli appears to be similar, with a consensus that early visual components originate in the primary visual cortex (Brodmann’s area 17, striate cortex, V1)14. A visual response then generally follows the hierarchical organization of cortical areas from V1 to V2, V339. Co-localization of dipole locations with fMRI activation sites shows that the two major sources of the SSVEP are located in primary visual cortex (V1) and in the motion sensitive (MT/V5) areas with minor contributions from mid-occipital (V3A) and ventral occipital (V4/V8) areas14.
Cortical propagation of oscillatory brain activity has been investigated in various animal and human studies both with intracranial and transcranial recordings40. This wave-like dynamic behaviour has been observed for brain oscillation in the alpha (8–12 Hz), theta (4–8 Hz)40,41,42, and sleep delta (0.5–4 Hz)43,44 bands as well as visually evoked potentials2,38,45,46,47. Early reports of travelling waves state that the estimated speed of travelling over the scalp generally varies between 1 and 20 m/s48. Alpha rhythm phase velocities from 3 to 7 m/s49 have been estimated over the scalp (6–14 m/s when corrected for cortical folding50), with corrected for folding lower alpha (\(\sim\) 8 Hz) velocity around 6.4 m/s and upper alpha (\(\sim\) 12 Hz) velocity around 8.4 m/s. Alpha waves have been also observed in ECoG recordings at phase velocity between 0.7 and 2 m/s40. The propagation velocity of theta waves moving through the hippocampus in ECoG recordings seem to be between 1 and 5 m/s42. In sleep, slow waves propagation at a speed from 1 to 7 m/s43 and from 0.4 to 6.3 m/s44 have also been observed. When it comes to cortical propagation of evoked oscillations, analyzing topographical phase relationships of the ERP P1 component (evoked by the Stroop task), having a frequency characteristics primarily in the alpha range, reveals a systematic posterior to anterior traveling pattern at a speed of approx. 3 m/s (or approx. 6 m/s if corrected for cortical folding)47. EEG activity following eye movement also show a wave propagation dynamics with mean velocity from 0.2 m/s at 0.5 Hz to 14 m/s at 32 Hz51. For SSVEPs, scalp phase velocities for checkerboard stimulation around the alpha peak have been estimated between 7 and 11 m/s (corrected for cortical folding using a folding factor of 2.2)2. SSVEPs elicited by a circular black/white checkerboard pattern at frequencies between 2 and 20 Hz, have showed a traveling wave behaviour propagating from occipital to prefrontal electrodes in the upper alpha band (phase velocities ranging from about 2.4 to 3.3 m/s along the scalp), but not in beta band46. Another study38 using stimulation between 3 and 30 Hz, also presents evidence for traveling waves (at a speed of 1–3 m/s) at frequencies in the upper alpha band (11–13 Hz) but not in delta and lower alpha.
While it might be difficult to dissociate between the different theories behind SSVEP origin and propagation, understanding the cortical properties of this response as measured by the EEG is crucial for the design and implementation of the practical applications identified above. Sparse electrode montages and higher frequencies positively impact the ease of use, patient throughput, comfort and safety. In this study we aim at investigating the spatiotemporal oscillatory properties of SSVEPs. Particularly, SSVEP responses at high frequencies, e.g. in the gamma band (40–60 Hz), which have not received enough attention and little is known about their generation and factors affecting the response. We set to investigate their site of origin and their cortical propagation properties in terms of speed and phase.
Results
SSVEP response
Time frequency decomposition analysis revealed a stable SSVEP response starting within 250 ms from stimulus onset at each fundamental and its harmonic frequencies. Figure 1 shows the percentage power change (event related synchronization, ERS) for the first second of stimulation for three of the conditions at each fundamental frequency. As expected the response is stronger over parieto-occipital sites. Lower frequency conditions are characterized by a broader response as compared to higher frequencies, showing some contribution from fronto-central sites. In all cases, after the transient response the first 250 ms, we observe a sustained steady state response for the whole duration of stimulation. These observations were consistent across all stimulation conditions.
For a more comprehensive overview of the neural dynamics at the fundamental and the harmonic frequencies of this dataset as well as the interaction with ongoing oscillatory activity see Tsoneva et al.52.
Source analysis
To identify the spread of cortical sources involved in SSVEP generation we performed source localization analysis using the sLORETA software. This analysis revealed strongest activation in response to frequencies between 40 and 56 Hz at coordinates: \(X = 0 , Y = - 85 , Z = 30\), identified as the Cuneus in Brodmann area 19 of the Occipital cortex (see Fig. 2a,b). Other areas of the visual cortex such as V1 (Brodmann area 17) and V2 (Brodmann area 18) were also identified as contributors. Additionally, minor contribution from sources in central and frontal sites could be observed. These coordinates were consistent across all conditions under 56 Hz for both, the fundamental and subharmonic components (see Supplementary information Figure S1ab). We did not find a consistent activation for higher order harmonic frequencies.
The source distribution for stimulation at 58 Hz and 60 Hz was slightly different (see Fig. 2c). While activity in the visual cortex was present, the strongest activation was found in the inferior frontal gyrus (Brodmann area 47) for the fundamental frequencies and middle frontal gyrus (Brodmann area 11) for the sub-harmonic frequencies (see Supplementary information Figure S1c). Statistical testing, however, did not show a significantly higher current source density (CSD) for those coordinates when compared to the second before stimulation onset as baseline.
The average log-F-ratio statistics was quite consistent across all stimulation conditions highlighting cortical areas in the occipital, parietal, limbic and frontal lobe. The average log-F-ratio across all conditions is shown in Fig. 2d. The log-F-ratio thresholds for \(p < 0.01\) and \(p < 0.05\) for the fundamental component at each stimulation condition are listed in Table 1. The results for the sub-harmonic components of each stimulation condition can be found in the Supplementary information Figure S1d and Table S1. Higher order harmonics did not show any significant difference from baseline (the second before stimulation onset).
Low-resolution brain electromagnetic tomography (sLORETA) at the fundamental frequency component under diverse stimulation conditions: (a) 44 Hz, (b) 54 Hz, (c) 58 Hz. (d) The average log F-ratio statistics across all fundamental frequency components. The squared magnitude of the CSD and the average log F-ratio are colour coded from blue (minimum) through grey (zero) to red and bright yellow (maximum). Slices from left to right: axial (viewed from top), sagittal (viewed from left), and coronal (viewed from back). L left, R right, A anterior, P posterior.
Source of origin
While LORETA is very useful for localizing the activity, it cannot tell us about the chronology of events. Generally, the SSVEP response is time- and phase-locked to the driving stimulus. Figure 3a shows the instantaneous phase locking value (PLV) between midline electrodes and the stimulation for one of the stimulation conditions (44 Hz) averaged over all subjects. Phase locking (\(PLV > 0.5\)) occurs already within the first 150 ms and saturates after approximately 250 ms, following the transient response. The same behaviour was observed for all other conditions. Because we are particularly interested in the phase properties of the response, we use finite impulse response (FIR) filters at the stimulation frequency, which have zero phase distortion after correcting for their linear delay. The inherently high order and the equiripple nature of those filters, however, can cause discontinuities at the head and tail of their impulse response. This is mostly visible at the start and the end of stimulation where the steady-state response is still weak. To deal with this issue we only consider PLVs for samples where the phase locking statistic (PLS) show significance of the phase covariance (\(PLS < 0.05\)). Figure 3b shows the decrease of PLS shortly after the stimulus onset and its relatively low value for the whole stimulation period, especially for parieto-occipital channels.
Phase locking between the light stimulation signal and the EEG signals for RVS at 44 Hz. (a) Instantaneous PLV between electrode locations along the midline and the light stimulation signal. The vertical red dotted lines mark the onset and offset of stimulation. The black dashed line marks the PLV threshold. (b) PLS for electrode locations along the midline and the stimulation signal. The vertical red dotted lines mark the onset and offset of stimulation. The black dashed line marks the PLS threshold. (c) Spatial distribution of phase-locking delays.
To identify the initial source locking to the driving stimulus first, we look at the significant \(PLVs > 0.5\) for all channels. The locking delays for different channels at stimulation frequency of 44 Hz are visualized in Fig. 3c. Note that for some channels PLS never reaches significance level. As expected, the channels with minimum locking delays are in the parietal and occipital areas for all frequencies, with channel Pz being the first to lock for 7 out of the 10 frequencies. For consistency, henceforth, we select channel Pz as source of wave propagation.
Phase velocity
The phase difference between each electrode and the source appears to be constant for the whole stimulation period (see Fig. 4b). The value, calculated over 5 cycles for each frequency and averaged over all trials and all subjects every 250 ms shows a progressive phase shift in the EEG, suggesting spherical propagation in all directions from the source.
Phase velocity estimation. (a) Electrode positions and the selected set of electrodes for phase velocity estimation (in blue). (b) Average phase difference between the source (Pz) and all electrode locations for stimulation at 44 Hz over time starting 250 ms before stimulation until 1000 ms after stimulation onset. (c) Topography of phase differences with respect to the source location Pz, 250 ms after stimulus onset, and the phase gradient as estimated by a first order polynomial fit for the selected electrode set for each stimulation condition in different subplots. Correlation coefficients (r), p values (p) and phase velocity estimates (V) are reported for each stimulation condition.
To characterise the phase velocity of the propagation we estimate the spatial phase gradients by fitting a first order polynomial to the phase estimates for a set of electrodes covering the sites identified in the source analysis, namely O1, O2, Oz, PO3, PO4, P3, Pz, P4, CP1, CP2, Cz, FC1, FC2 and Fz as shown in Fig. 4a ordered in increasing distance from the source Pz.
The phase velocity is inversely proportional to the phase gradient and is calculated using Eq. (6) adjusting for the stimulation frequency for each condition. Figure 4c shows the first order polynomial fit and the phase velocity estimates over the area identified by the source analysis for all conditions 250 ms after stimulus onset when the steady-state response should be already stable. The correlation between phase difference and electrode distance (see Eq. 5) is significant for all conditions with mean correlation coefficient \(r = 0.85 (\pm 0.013)\) and mean p = 0.001 (± 5.5265.526e−05) across conditions. The estimated velocity ranges between 8.64 and 13.77 m/s. These estimates suggest waves propagating at wavelengths between 21.6 and 23.5 cm across all conditions.
The phase velocity estimates are by definition sensitive to stimulation frequency (see Eq. 6). Figure 5 shows a linear relationship and a significant positive correlation between our velocity estimates and frequency of stimulation (\(r = 0.99\), \(p = 3.852e-08\)). We also estimate the linear regression model parameters that describe this relationship. An increase in the stimulation frequency of 1 Hz results in about 0.24 m/s increase in phase velocity. It is important to note that the validity of this model applies to the range of frequency tested as different mechanisms might apply to oscillations in different frequency sub-systems.
Discussion
We set to investigate the SSVEP responses to periodic visual stimulation at high frequencies within the gamma band (40–60 Hz) by studying the dynamic properties of EEG measured by sparse electrode montage in 32 subjects. Not surprisingly the strongest cortical sources involved in SSVEP generation, identified by the sLORETA analysis, were found in the primary visual cortex, Brodmann areas 17, 18 and 19. Minor contribution from sources in central and frontal sites could also be observed. Looking at the phase response we found phase locking occurring already within the first 150 ms with channel Pz being the one that synchronized to the driving stimuli first for the majority of the tested conditions. While it is difficult to distinguish between the neurophysiological mechanism through which the SSVEP spreads through the cortex, being a finite number of electrical dipoles that are activated sequentially in time, or through cortical travelling waves, the SSVEP response as measured by the surface EEG shows a progressive phase shift in all directions from the source. Our results suggest that high-frequency-evoked SSVEPs propagate over the cortex with a phase velocity of approx. 8–14 m/s and wavelength of about 21 and 24 cm.
It should be emphasized that estimates of phase velocity depend on a variety of different factors. Some authors highlight the fact that the choice of EEG recording reference might play an important role with bipolar recordings giving lower estimates as compared to reference methods2,49. Other estimates are based on reference-free methods such as surface Laplacian methods38,49. MEG and ECoG recordings produce lower velocity estimates than EEG measurements40,46. Apart from the above mentioned factors affecting the phase estimates, inter-electrode distance could also vary. Some authors choose to correct for cortical folding (fissures)2,53, while others do not40,43. Furthermore, using euclidian or great-circle distance between electrodes results in different velocity estimates. All these factors inherently affect the phase velocity estimates making it difficult to compare between studies.
There is also lack of literature reporting SSVEP propagation in the gamma band. However, frequency seems to be an important factor and a number of authors show that SSVEP properties are sensitive to flicker frequency38,46. Sleep slow waves (0.5–4 Hz) are reported to be travelling at 1–7 m/s, with most of the scalp velocities being in the 2–3 m/s range43. Alpha waves seem to be travelling at 3–8 m/s, with lower alpha (8 Hz) around 6.4 m/s and upper alpha (12 Hz) around 8.4 m/s49. One of the few studies looking at higher frequencies reports phase velocities ranging from 0.2 m/s at 0.5 Hz to 14 m/s at 32Hz for eye-movement evoked responses. This progressive increase of velocity with the frequency of oscillations (spontaneous or evoked) seems to be in line with our estimates and the correlation with frequency of stimulation we found (see Fig. 5). It is, however, important to note that SSVEPs are sensitive to cortical resonances in at least three frequency subsystems10,38 and the mechanisms that apply at higher frequencies might not directly translate to oscillations at lower frequencies.
In their theoretical framework, considering the rough boundaries of propagation velocities within cortico-cortical fibers and accounting for experimental and theoretical uncertainties, some authors53 expect the phase velocity of cortical waves traveling away from primary visual areas to not exceed 30 m/s (up to a factor of three from observed estimates between 6 and 9 m/s). Our estimates of SSVEP propagation speed using Laplacian derivation and great-circle distance are within these loose boundaries.
Scalp recorded patterns of cortical activity also reflect volume conduction across the scalp, skull and other tissues. Volume conduction is especially a problem of reference EEG recordings, which are known to overestimate propagation velocities. Alternatively, close bipolar electrodes generally do better, but may be biased toward shorter wavelengths54. In this study, the spatial distortion from conductivity variations was minimized by applying a surface Laplacian, which is reference independent linear data transformation that maintains the invariant aspects of the EEG signal and provides means for obtaining continuous estimates of radial current flow at the scalp for both low- and high-density EEG montages55. It enhances the spatial information of the EEG signal by emphasizing sources within 2–3 cm of the electrode and providing good representation of the underlying current dipoles38. In our initial analysis we compared two preprocessing approaches, namely the widely used common average reference (CAR) and surface Laplacian. Signals preprocessed using CAR seem to show progressive phase shifts even outside the stimulation period (350 ms before stimulation onset) showing clearly the effects of volume conduction (see Fig. 6). Applying surface Laplacian eliminated this effect and the phase shift was only observed during the stimulation period (see Fig. 4c). Additionally, the source localization analysis, revealing neuronal current generators underlying EEG scalp topographies, confirmed the spread of the observed propagation and was used as a basis for our further analysis.
The sources identified by sLORETA for the fundamental and sub-harmonic components seemed to agree with what has been previously reported14,56. We, however, did not find a significant activation for the first harmonic component. A reason for that might be that all our stimulation condition were in the high frequency region, resulting in first harmonic component in the range from 80 to 120 Hz. The decrease in SNR for higher order harmonics may distort the LORETA analysis. Wagner et al. suggest that simultaneously active sources can only be separated well by sLORETA analysis if their fields are distinct enough and of similar strength, and in the context of a strong or superficial source, weak or deep sources remain invisible57. In our previous work we did observe a linear decrease in SSVEP strength at the first harmonic especially at higher frequencies52. It was encouraging to see that the cortical sources identified by the sLORETA analysis for the first harmonic of our lowest stimulation condition (40 Hz) were consistent with those identified by Pastor et al.56 for the same frequency (their highest frequency condition).
Regarding the differences observed for the highest stimulation conditions at 58 and 60 Hz, stimuli at those frequencies at average light level of about 130 cd/m\(^2\) and assuming an average pupil diameter of about 4 mm, would fall very close to the visual perception threshold as defined by the temporal sensitivity curve at retinal illuminance of 1000 Td and above58. The responses in the high frequency range could possibly have different neural representations and the associated neural mechanisms could be different from those governing conscious visual perception at lower frequencies11,59.
Broadband pseudo random noise sequences produce a code-modulated visual evoked potential response (cVEPs) that shares many characteristics with SSVEPs, including topology. This approach has been successful in BCIs60. A comparisons with SSVEP would boost insight in the respective mechanisms and the evidence for SSVEP propagation model may well inform research in broadband visual evoked potentials. It is not clear if the processing of periodic versus noise-like stimuli is open to attention modulation in different ways. But such propagation model may point to possible gain in detection methods, also for these stimuli.
While no conceptual framework could possibly account for the complex properties and interactions in the human brain, models considering both the spatial and temporal aspects of the cortical dynamics would provide better representation of the underlining mechanisms.
The broad range of SSVEP applications in research and clinical practice call for reduced complexity, which would positively impact the ease of use, patient throughput, comfort and safety. In this manuscript, we investigated the spatiotemporal oscillatory properties of SSVEPs using a sparse electrode montage and high stimulation frequencies. We show what are the cortical sources underlying HF-SSVEPs as well as their spread and propagation velocity. This work could help building more practical solutions, improve processing speed, reduce erroneous interpretation and enhance the patient experience. Furthermore, this can open new doors for studying the neural dynamics in the gamma band, which functional significance is only starting to be understood.
Methods
Participants
For this study we invited 32 healthy volunteers with normal or corrected to normal vision (14 male, 18 female, age range 20–30 years old). Because of the nature of the stimuli, the participants were carefully screened for a history of photosensitivity, seizures, migraine, or headaches and only healthy participants not reporting any of these conditions were enrolled in the study. They were informed about the goals of the study and asked to sign an informed consent before the start of the experiment. The study was approved by the Philips internal ethics committee on biomedical experiments and conducted in accordance with the declaration of Helsinki Guidelines for Ethics in Research.
Experimental paradigm
During the experimental session the participants were instructed to focus their attention on an LED panel with dimensions 44 \(\times\) 36 cm, and to avoid eye blinks and movement as much as possible. The panel was placed centrally on a desk at a distance of about 70 cm and spanning a visual angle of about 35\(^{\circ }\). The panel consisted of twenty 2 mm white LEDs (Philips Lumileds LXHL) evenly distributed in 4 rows and 5 columns (approx. 8 cm apart in each dimension) covered by a diffusing screen. It delivered repetitive visual stimulation in the form of square waves at 100% modulation depth and 50% duty cycle. The average light output of the stimuli was approx. 130 cd/m\(^2\). The experimental conditions comprised RVS at 10 different frequencies from 40 to 60 Hz with a step size of 2 Hz, excluding 50 Hz as it coincides with the power line frequency at the study site. Stimuli presentation was randomized and each condition was presented 10 times, which is expected to improve the SNR by a factor of \(\sqrt{(}10) \approx 3\)61. Each trial consisted of an inter-stimulus interval, where no light was present, of random duration between 3 and 6 s, followed by a 2 s long stimulation period (see Fig. 7). Trials were grouped in blocks of 10 separated by a break of 10 s during which the participants could rest, move or blink. The average experimental duration was approx. 13 min and ensured that effects of fatigue or decreased alertness, which might affect the response62, are minimized.
Data acquisition
BioSemi ActiveTwo signal acquisition system (BioSemi B.V., Amsterdam, The Netherlands) was used to record EEG via 32 sintered Ag/AgCl active electrodes at a sampling frequency of 2048 Hz. The electrodes were mounted on an elastic cap according to the 32-channel extension of the international 10–20 positioning system (see Fig. 4a). The setup used an added common mode sense and driven right leg as “ground” electrodes. The signal from a photo diode was jointly recorded with the EEG to mark the presentation of the stimuli.
Data analysis
EEG preprocessing
The recorded signals were analysed using custom MATLAB scripts and the open source EEGLAB toolbox63. First, a 50 Hz notch filter was applied to attenuate the power line interference. Eye-blinks and other ocular artifacts were removed using independent component analysis (ICA)53 by excluding the component showing the highest average spatial difference between front and back electrodes. The signals were subsampled to 256 Hz to reduce the computation time, and surface Laplacian64,65 was applied to reduce the effect of widely distributed background neuronal activity and to enhance the spatial localization of the signals.
The signals were then segmented into non-overlapping epochs time-locked to stimulus onset. The epochs started 1000 ms before stimulus onset and ended 3000 ms after, including 2000 ms of stimulation and a 1000 ms long period after stimulus offset.
Time frequency decomposition
All preprocessed epochs were subjected to a time-frequency decomposition using the fast Fourier transform (FFT) with a 126 samples (500 ms) wide Hanning window and 4.5 samples (17.59 ms) overlap (a total of 200 windows). The average power at each stimulation frequency was, then, calculated for five 250 ms-wide windows, starting with a baseline interval 250 ms before and continuing for 1000 ms after stimulation onset. Using this estimate we define the event related synchronization (ERS) as the fraction power change in each 250 ms window referenced to the 250 ms baseline interval. The resulting dynamics in each channel were then visualized on a topographic map. For this analysis the window length and baseline were optimized to improve the visual representation of the spatial maps.
Source localization
We estimated EEG cortical source current density with low-resolution brain electromagnetic tomography using the sLORETA software66. sLORETA calculates the standardized current source density (CSD) in the gray matter and the hippocampus at each of the 6239 voxels of the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) reference brain. CSD at the frequency of stimulation and its harmonics for each condition was estimated using a regularization factor of 10 (SNR = 10). For this analysis we computed the average cross spectra for each condition over 1 second long epochs starting 250 ms after stimulus onset. The results were then compared to the 1 second period before stimulation onset using sLORETA’s built-in non-parametric statistical analyses (statistical non-parametric maps, SnPM) using log-F-ratio statistic for paired groups with a threshold of \(p < 0.01\) and \(p < 0.05\) and permutation test repeated 5000 times, correcting for multiple comparisons.
Phase synchrony analysis
Phase synchrony between two signals was estimated by looking for latencies at which the phase difference between signals varies little across trials67. For this analysis, all signals including the signal from the photo diode monitoring the light stimulation, were first filtered with a band-pass Parks–McClellan finite impulse response (FIR) filters centered around each stimulation frequency (\(Fstop: F \pm 3 Hz, Fpass: F \pm 1 Hz, Dpass: 0.058, Dstop: 10^{-4}, density factor = 20\)). The filter properties were chosen such that the filter is stable, while ensuring a narrow bandpass response. FIR filters were chosen because they can be designed to have an exact linear phase response. The constant group delay after filtering was estimated and signals were corrected for it.
Next, the instantaneous phase of the signal y(t) was computed using the Hilbert transform68. The Hilbert transform \(y_h(t)\) is the convolution of the signal y(t) with the function \(h(t)=1/\pi t\) such that:
where \(\tau\) is the integration variable.
The Hilbert transform enables the analytical representation of the signal as follows:
Comparison of the Hilbert transform and the FFT-based method revealed no substantial difference in phase estimates.
The phase difference \(\Delta \phi _{12}(t)\) between two time series \(y_1(t)\) and \(y_2(t)\) with instantaneous phases \(\phi _1(t)\) and \(\phi _2(t)\), respectively, was calculated as follows:
The phase locking value (PLV) for each condition, channel and subject at time t was then defined as the average value over trials (\(n = 1,\ldots ,N\)):
The phase locking statistics (PLS)67 measures the significance of the phase covariance between the two signals. It is a statistical test based on a surrogate data aiming at differentiating significant PLVs against noise. PLS was calculated by shuffling the trials for another randomly selected stimulation condition and calculating the respective surrogate PLV. The PLS comprises the proportion of the surrogate values higher than the original PLV. We chose \(PLS \le 5\%\) to ensure the probability of having false positive rate below \(5\%\).
Distance estimates
The distance \(\Delta \delta _{12}\) between electrode locations was estimated as the great-circle distance between two points on a sphere using the haversine formula (see Eq. (5)). We assumed a spherical head model with a radius of 9 cm.
where r is the radius of the sphere; \(\phi _1, \phi _2\) are the latitudes of the two points in radians; and \(\lambda _1, \lambda _2\) are the longitudes of the two points in radians.
Phase velocity
Phase velocity v is the speed at which the phase of a wave propagates over the scalp and is calculated as follows:
where f is the stimulus frequency, and \(\Delta \phi _{12}(t)\) is the phase difference as defined in Eq. (3) and \(\Delta \delta _{12}\) is distance between electrodes as defined in Eq. (5).
Data availability
The data that support the findings of this manuscript are not publicly available, as it will potentially compromise research participant consent under the European GDRP law regarding reuse of data for secondary research and development purposes.
References
Regan, D. Human Brain Electrophysiology: Evoked Potentials and Evoked Magnetic Fields in Science and Medicine (Elsevier, New York, 1989).
Burkitt, G. R., Silberstein, R. B., Cadusch, P. J. & Wood, a. W. Steady-state visual evoked potentials and travelling waves. Clin. Neurophysiol. 111, 246–58 (2000).
Silberstein, R. B., Nunez, P. L., Pipingas, A., Harris, P. & Danieli, F. Steady state visually evoked potential (SSVEP) topography in a graded working memory task. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 42, 219–232 (2001).
Ellis, K. A., Silberstein, R. B. & Nathan, P. J. Exploring the temporal dynamics of the spatial working memory n-back task using steady state visual evoked potentials (ssvep). Neuroimage 31, 1741–1751 (2006).
Brown, R. J. & Norcia, A. M. A method for investigating binocular rivalry in real-time with the steady-state vep. Vision. Res. 37, 2401–2408 (1997).
Hillyard, S. A. et al. Combining steady-state visual evoked potentials and FMRI to localize brain activity during selective attention. Hum. Brain Mapp. 5, 287–292 (1997).
Di Russo, F. & Spinelli, D. Spatial attention has different effects on the magno-and parvocellular pathways. NeuroReport 10, 2755–2762 (1999).
Silberstein, R. B., Line, P., Pipingas, A., Copolov, D. & Harris, P. Steady-state visually evoked potential topography during the continuous performance task in normal controls and schizophrenia. Clin. Neurophysiol. 111, 850–857 (2000).
Keil, A. et al. Early modulation of visual perception by emotional arousal: Evidence from steady-state visual evoked brain potentials. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 3, 195–206 (2003).
Regan, D. Steady-state evoked potentials. JOSA 67, 1475–1489 (1977).
Tsoneva, T., Garcia-Molina, G., van de Sant, J. & Farquhar, J. Eliciting steady state visual evoked potentials near the visual perception threshold. In Neural Engineering (NER), 2013 6th International IEEE/EMBS Conference on, 93–96 (IEEE, 2013).
Bieger, J., Molina, G. G. & Zhu, D. Effects of stimulation properties in steady-state visual evoked potential based brain-computer interfaces. In Proceedings of 32nd Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society, 3345–8 (2010).
Di Russo, F., Teder-Sälejärvi, W. A. & Hillyard, S. A. Steady-state vep and attentional visual processing. The Cognitive Electrophysiology of Mind and Brain (Zani A, Proverbio AM, eds) 259–274 (2002).
Di Russo, F. et al. Spatiotemporal analysis of the cortical sources of the steady-state visual evoked potential. Hum. Brain Mapp. 28, 323–334 (2007).
Leat, S. J., Yadav, N. K. & Irving, E. L. Development of visual acuity and contrast sensitivity in children. J. Optometry 2, 19–26 (2009).
Morrone, M. C., Fiorentini, A. & Burr, D. C. Development of the temporal properties of visual evoked potentials to luminance and colour contrast in infants. Vision. Res. 36, 3141–3155 (1996).
Stock, S. C. et al. A system approach for closed-loop assessment of neuro-visual function based on convolutional neural network analysis of EEG signals. In Neurophotonics, vol. 11360, 1136008 (International Society for Optics and Photonics, 2020).
Spinelli, D., Burr, D. C. & Morrone, M. C. Spatial neglect is associated with increased latencies of visual evoked potentials. Vis. Neurosci. 11, 909–918 (1994).
Rubboli, G., Parra, J., Seri, S., Takahashi, T. & Thomas, P. Eeg diagnostic procedures and special investigations in the assessment of photosensitivity. Epilepsia 45, 35–39 (2004).
Sartucci, F. et al. Dysfunction of the magnocellular stream in Alzheimer’s disease evaluated by pattern electroretinograms and visual evoked potentials. Brain Res. Bull. 82, 169–176 (2010).
Stanzione, P. et al. An electrophysiological study of d 2 dopaminergic actions in normal human retina: A tool in parkinson’s disease. Neurosci. Lett. 140, 125–128 (1992).
Sridhar, S. & Manian, V. Assessment of cognitive aging using an SSVEP-based brain–computer interface system. Big Data Cogn. Comput. 3, 29 (2019).
Thut, G., Schyns, P. G. & Gross, J. Entrainment of perceptually relevant brain oscillations by non-invasive rhythmic stimulation of the human brain. Front. Psychol. 2, 20 (2011).
Lakatos, P., Karmos, G., Mehta, A. D., Ulbert, I. & Schroeder, C. E. Entrainment of neuronal oscillations as a mechanism of attentional selection. Science (New York, N.Y.) 320, 23–25 (2008).
Kanai, R., Chaieb, L., Antal, A., Walsh, V. & Paulus, W. Frequency-dependent electrical stimulation of the visual cortex. Curr. Biol. 18, 1839–1843 (2008).
Klimesch, W., Sauseng, P. & Gerloff, C. Enhancing cognitive performance with repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation at human individual alpha frequency. Eur. J. Neurosci. 17, 1129–1133 (2003).
Bellesi, M., Riedner, B. . a, Garcia-Molina, G. . N., Cirelli, C. & Tononi, G. Enhancement of sleep slow waves: Underlying mechanisms and practical consequences. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 8, 1–17 (2014).
Iaccarino, H. F. et al. Gamma frequency entrainment attenuates amyloid load and modifies microglia. Nature 540, 230–235 (2016).
Singer, A. C. et al. Noninvasive 40-Hz light flicker to recruit microglia and reduce amyloid beta load. Nat. Protoc. 13, 1850–1868 (2018).
Fisher, R. S., Harding, G., Erba, G., Barkley, G. L. & Wilkins, A. Photic-and pattern-induced seizures: A review for the epilepsy foundation of america working group. Epilepsia 46, 1426–1441 (2005).
Allison, B. Z. Toward ubiquitous BCIs. In Brain–Computer Interfaces 357–387 (Springer, Berlin, 2010).
Yijun, W., Ruiping, W., Xiaorong, G. & Shangkai, G. Brain-computer interface based on the high-frequency steady-state visual evoked potential. In Proceedings. 2005 First International Conference on Neural Interface and Control, 2005., 37–39 (IEEE, 2005).
Wang, Y., Wang, R., Gao, X., Hong, B. & Gao, S. A practical vep-based brain-computer interface. IEEE Trans. Neural Syst. Rehabil. Eng. 14, 234–240 (2006).
Han, C., Xu, G., Xie, J., Chen, C. & Zhang, S. Highly interactive brain–computer interface based on flicker-free steady-state motion visual evoked potential. Sci. Rep. 8, 1–13 (2018).
Sakurada, T., Kawase, T., Komatsu, T. & Kansaku, K. Use of high-frequency visual stimuli above the critical flicker frequency in a SSVEP-based BMI. Clin. Neurophysiol. 126, 1972–1978 (2015).
Chen, X., Chen, Z., Gao, S. & Gao, X. A high-ITR SSVEP-based BCI speller. Brain Comput. Interfaces 1, 181–191 (2014).
Vialatte, F.-B., Maurice, M., Dauwels, J. & Cichocki, A. Steady-state visually evoked potentials: Focus on essential paradigms and future perspectives. Prog. Neurobiol. 90, 418–438 (2010).
Srinivasan, R., Bibi, F. A. & Nunez, P. L. Steady-state visual evoked potentials: Distributed local sources and wave-like dynamics are sensitive to flicker frequency. Brain Topogr. 18, 167–187 (2006).
Vanni, S. et al. Sequence of pattern onset responses in the human visual areas: An fMRI constrained VEP source analysis. NeuroImage 21, 801–817 (2004).
Bahramisharif, A. et al. Propagating neocortical gamma bursts are coordinated by traveling alpha waves. J. Neurosci. 33, 18849–54 (2013).
Klimesch, W., Sauseng, P. & Hanslmayr, S. EEG alpha oscillations: The inhibition-timing hypothesis. Brain Res. Rev. 53, 63–88 (2007).
Zhang, H. & Jacobs, J. Traveling theta waves in the human hippocampus. J. Neurosci. 35, 12477–12487 (2015).
Massimini, M., Huber, R., Ferrarelli, F., Hill, S. & Tononi, G. The sleep slow oscillation as a traveling wave. J. Neurosci. 24, 6862–6870 (2004).
Murphy, M. et al. Source modeling sleep slow waves. PNAS 106, 1608–1613 (2009).
Wilson, H. R., Blake, R. & Lee, S.-H. Dynamics of travelling waves in visual perception. Nature 412, 907–910 (2001).
Thorpe, S. G., Nunez, P. L. & Srinivasan, R. Identification of wave-like spatial structure in the SSVEP: Comparison of simultaneous EEG and meg. Stat. Med. 26, 3911–3926 (2007).
Klimesch, W., Hanslmayr, S., Sauseng, P., Gruber, W. R. & Doppelmayr, M. P1 and traveling alpha waves: Evidence for evoked oscillations. J. Neurophysiol. 97, 1311–1318 (2007).
Hughes, J. R. The phenomenon of travelling waves: A review. Clin. Electroencephalogr. 26, 1–6 (1995).
Nunez, P. L. & Cutillo, B. A. Neocortical Dynamics and Human EEG Rhythms (Oxford University Press, USA, 1995).
Nunez, P. L. et al. Electric Fields of the Brain: The Neurophysics of EEG (Oxford University Press, USA, 2006).
Giannini, M., Alexander, D. M., Nikolaev, A. R. & Leeuwen, C. V. Large-scale traveling waves in EEG activity following eye movement. Brain Topogr. 31, 608–622 (2018).
Tsoneva, T., Garcia-Molina, G. & Desain, P. Neural dynamics during repetitive visual stimulation. J. Neural Eng. 12, 066017 (2015).
Nunez, P. L. & Srinivasan, R. A theoretical basis for standing and traveling brain waves measured with human EEG with implications for an integrated consciousness. Clin. Neurophysiol. 117, 2424–2435 (2006).
Nunez, P. L. & Srinivasan, R. Neocortical dynamics due to axon propagation delays in cortico-cortical fibers: EEG traveling and standing waves with implications for top-down influences on local networks and white matter disease. Brain Res. 1542x, 138–166 (2015).
Kayser, J. & Tenke, C. E. Issues and considerations for using the scalp surface Laplacian in EEG/ERP research: A tutorial review. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 97, 189–209 (2015).
Pastor, M. A., Valencia, M., Artieda, J., Alegre, M. & Masdeu, J. C. Topography of cortical activation differs for fundamental and harmonic frequencies of the steady-state visual-evoked responses*** An EEG and PET H215O study. Cereb. Cortex (New York, N.Y. : 1991) 17, 1899–905 (2007).
Wagner, M., Fuchs, M. & Kastner, J. Evaluation of sloreta in the presence of noise and multiple sources. Brain Topogr. 16, 277–280 (2004).
Barten, P. G. J. Formula for the contrast sensitivity of the human eye. Image Qual. Syst. Perform. 5294, 231–238 (2003).
Regan, D. A high frequency mechanism which underliers VEP. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 25, 231–237 (1968).
Thielen, J., Van Den Broek, P., Farquhar, J. & Desain, P. Broad-band visually evoked potentials: Re (con)volution in brain–computer interfacing. PLoS One 10, 1–22 (2015).
Andreassi, J. Psychophysiology: Human Behavior and Physiological Response (Lawrence Erlbaum, New Jersey, 2007).
Simonson, E. & Enzer, N. Measurement of fusion frequency of flicker as a test for fatigue of the central nervous system. J. Ind. Hygiene Toxicol. 23, 83–89 (1941).
Delorme, A. & Makeig, S. Eeglab: An open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J. Neurosci. Methods 134, 9–21 (2004).
Perrin, F., Pernier, J., Bertrand, O. & Echallier, J. Spherical splines for scalp potential and current density mapping. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 72, 184–187 (1989).
Farquhar, J. Function ’sphericalSplineInterpolate’ taken from the Brain Computer Interface toolbox. https://github.com/jadref/buffer_bci (2014–2018).
Pascual-Marqui, R. Standardized low resolution brain electromagnetic tomography (sLORETA): Technical details. Methods Find. Exp. Clin. Pharmacol. 24, 5–12 (2002).
Lachaux, J. P., Rodriguez, E., Martinerie, J. & Varela, F. J. Measuring phase synchrony in brain signals. Hum. Brain Mapp. 8, 194–208 (1999).
Zhu, D., Garcia-Molina, G., Mihajlovic, V. & Aarts, R. M. Phase synchrony analysis for ssvep-based bcis. In Computer Engineering and Technology (ICCET), 2010 2nd International Conference on, vol. 2, V2–329 (IEEE, 2010).
Acknowledgements
The authors would like to thank Jason Farquhar for the spatial filter software implementation65, Raymond van Ee and Jan Mathijs Schoffelen for their valuable feedback and our participants for their kind collaboration. This study was funded by Philips Research Europe.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
T.T. and G.G.-M. conceived and designed the experiments and analyzed the results. T.T. wrote the manuscript. P.D. supervised the work and reviewed the manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
T.T. is employed and G.G-M was employed as scientists at Royal Philips, a commercial company, which is a major innovating institution in the field of applied neuroscience. As scientists, they are mandated by their employer to make meaningful scientific contributions and always strive to ensure scientific integrity in their academic writings. P.D. has no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Informtaion
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Tsoneva, T., Garcia-Molina, G. & Desain, P. SSVEP phase synchronies and propagation during repetitive visual stimulation at high frequencies. Sci Rep 11, 4975 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-83795-9
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-83795-9
This article is cited by
-
Optimal flickering light stimulation for entraining gamma rhythms in older adults
Scientific Reports (2022)
Comments
By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.