Introduction

Throughout the history of humankind, various peoples migrated to parts of the world where they were visually distinct from the standards of facial appearance of local majority population. This was also the case of Vietnamese citizens who were coming to Czechoslovakia since 1956 to be trained in mechanical engineering, light industries, or to study at Czechoslovak universities1. The arrival of Vietnamese students, trainees, and guest workers was based on friendly diplomatic and economic contacts between the Czechoslovak Socialist Republic and the Democratic Republic of Vietnam. Many of these immigrants did not learn the Czech language, which somewhat isolates this older wave of immigrants who arrived during the era of state socialism from the majority Czech population. After the fall of the Communistic regime in Czechoslovakia in 1989, many more Vietnamese came from Vietnam and from other European countries. After the Velvet Revolution, persons of Vietnamese origin found their niche within the Czech society as small traders in street markets or operators of small grocery shops. The young generation, that is, usually the offspring of the Vietnamese immigrants, is better integrated into Czech society and Czech educational system and many are promising students who successfully aspire to professional careers. They call themselves "the Banana generation"—meaning yellow outside, white inside—because unlike their parents and despite their Asian appearance, they speak fluent Czech. They sometimes feel more Czech than Vietnamese: they were born in the Czech Republic or lived there for most of their life, and they often have an ambivalent relationship to the country their parents came from. It is estimated that the Vietnamese minority in the Czech Republic currently numbers around 60,000. In this study, we recruited members of this younger generation of the Vietnamese minority as "Czech Vietnamese" raters.

One can learn a lot just by looking at another person's face. Apart from the most obvious information, such as sex and age, faces also provide information about strength, health, health risks, fighting performance, social status, emotional states, and various personality traits2,3,4,5,6,7. Facial perception is thus one of the most important aspects of human social interactions, especially due to its decisive role in forming first impressions8,9,10.

It is well documented that people are better at recognizing and remembering faces and reading emotional expressions in faces of own race than in faces belonging to other races11,12,13,14,15,16. This phenomenon is known as the other-race effect. It implies that the perception of various attributes of faces belonging to other-race faces is usually less effective than the recognition of attributes of own-race faces. In short, it is more difficult to distinguish and decode particular facial features and their meaning in faces of other ethnicities. These facial perception biases evolve in very early childhood17,18,19. It has been shown that own-race faces are perceived in a more holistic manner that other-race faces20. Face perception strongly depends on the viewing context and previous exposure21. Greater amount of individuating experiences with other-race faces tend to reduce the other-race effect in holistic processing22. On the other hand, recent research had shown that some social traits may be better assessed for outgroup faces23. It has been demonstrated that racially typical individuals are more vulnerable to various forms of social discrimination and stigmatization, especially by outgroup members24. Other-race (versus own-race) perception may be mediated by nonracial categorization. The people classified as in-group (versus out-group) are perceived in a more positive manner25. Own-race bias can be minimized or eliminated by positive emotions, which help promote a broader social identity prior to interpersonal interaction26. It has been demonstrated that social categorization on its own can elicit biases in facial recognition and perception and might be thus partially responsible for the other-race effect27. It seems that both sufficient perceptual experience with other-race faces and motivation to individuate out-group faces successfully reduces the other-race effect in perception28.

Attractiveness is among the most frequently studied attributes of face perception, because it plays a key role in both mate choice and social perceptions. Positive personality traits are cross-culturally listed among the most significant factors in mate choice by both sexes and attractive people tend to be attributed more positive qualities by others29. For example, attractive individuals are treated more positively than less attractive persons and are perceived as more healthy, intelligent, competent, and more experienced in dating (for a meta-analytic review of stereotypes associated with beauty, see30. There is a cross-cultural consensus on attractiveness standards of human faces31,32 although in various cultures, there may be a different degree of emphasis on various attractiveness features such variation in skin texture, skin color, eye and hair color, facial masculinity, or facial shape33,34,35,36,37,38,39,40. Among the traits which contribute to the resulting facial attractiveness the most are averageness (proximity to a population norm), facial symmetry, and sexual dimorphism (the degree of development of sexually dimorphic features).

More average (that is, less distinct) faces are perceived as more attractive due to stabilizing selection41,42. Facial averageness reflects higher genetic diversity, greater heterozygosity, and therefore also higher biological quality29,43,44. Male and female faces manipulated to greater averageness are regarded as healthier-looking45. Faces perceived as the most attractive seem to be those which are close to the average but not the most average ones42,46,47,48.

An alternative explanation of preference for typical faces might be that faces were regarded as attractive not in virtue of their proximity to the population norm but rather due to a symmetry resulting from computational averaging that takes place during the production of facial composites49,50. That explanation, however, fails to account for the association between attractiveness and averageness calculated as the distance of natural faces from their population mean, which is a method used in this study. When averageness was computed as a Procrustes distance from the average face, higher averageness was positively linked with attractiveness51. Moreover, in studies which used composites, averageness accounted for a significant part of attractiveness even after exclusion of the effect of symmetry52.

In human faces, sexual dimorphism develops around puberty. This process is driven by increasing influence of sex steroids (such as estrogens, androgens, and progestogens, usually listed only as testosterone and estrogens). It has been reported that feminine facial shapes in females are perceived as more attractive both between and within cultures36,43,53,54,55. In virtue of its association with high estrogen levels, facial femininity in women may indicate sexual maturity and high reproductive capacity43,56 or, due to its association with youthfulness, it may serve as a cue to residual reproductive capacity57. With respect to links between attractiveness and individual expression of sexual dimorphism in male faces, on the other hand, reported results are inconsistent. Some studies reported women's preference for more masculine faces58,59 while other studies found evidence of preference for more feminine male faces60,61,62. It seems that both masculine and feminine male faces might be preferred, whereby the actual preference depends on an interaction between perceived hormone markers and the hormonal state of the female perceiver63,64. Moreover, it has been shown that preference for sexually dimorphic traits varies among human cultures and its association with attractiveness may have emerged relatively recently in human evolution, in particular with the appearance of urban Western societies65. Alternatively, the variation over preference for sexual dimorphism among different populations can also be explained by other factors such as differences in national health, income inequality, and homicide rate66,67.

Age serves as a cue to actual and residual fertility in women68 and, to a lesser extent, also in men69. In women, the decline of attractiveness with age is steeper and seems stable across cultures70,71,72. Even in young women, the current attractiveness is negatively associated with their estimated age at menopause (based on woman's mother menopause age)73. In men, the attractiveness declines with age more slowly. Middle-aged men may be attractive, as they possess higher status and more resources while keeping a sufficient level of fertility71.

The actual relative weight (body mass index) predicted a person's perceived adiposity and perceived weight74,75, though the viewing angle may bias the perception of weight from the face76. Both high and low perceived facial adiposity (underweight and overweight) lowers perceived healthiness. High perceived facial adiposity is also associated with worse actual health74. Higher BMI is negatively associated with perceived female bodily77 and facial attractiveness78. Nonetheless, there are cross-cultural differences in the most preferred relative weight79. In this study, we investigate differences in perceived attractiveness of European faces of Czech origin between Vietnamese persons living in the Czech Republic (henceforth Czech Vietnamese) and Vietnamese permanently residing in Vietnam (henceforth Asian Vietnamese). We expect that attractiveness judgments of Czech Vietnamese raters should be closer to attractiveness judgments of Czech European raters than to assessments by Asian Vietnamese raters. In other words, we hypothesize that the correlation in attractiveness judgments between Czech European and Czech Vietnamese raters is stronger than the correlation between Czech European and Asian Vietnamese raters. Moreover, we expect that this difference should be statistically significant. We also expect that Czech Vietnamese raters should be better adapted to the perception of sexually dimorphic traits of Czech faces and to local morphological typicality in general than Asian Vietnamese raters. In other words, we expect that the Czech Vietnamese raters have a better representation of both Czech facial population mean and the degree of facial masculinization/feminization and that the correlation between attractiveness and degree of shape sexual dimorphism (SShD) measured from faces will be greater between Czech and Czech Vietnamese than between Czech and Asian Vietnamese raters. Finally, we investigate the pattern of relative contribution of objectively measured level of sexual dimorphism and averageness to the perception of facial attractiveness in all three groups of raters (Czech Europeans, Czech Vietnamese, and Asian Vietnamese).

Materials and methods

Ethical statement

All the experiment protocol for involving humans was in accordance to guidelines of national/international/institutional or Declaration of Helsinki. All procedures mentioned and followed were approved by the Institutional Review Board of the Faculty of Science of the Charles University (protocol ref. number 06/2017).

Data acquisition

We took facial portraits of 100 Czech participants (50 women: mean age = 23.64; SD = 4.29; range = 19–36; 50 men: mean age = 24.04; SD = 3.88; range = 19–34). Portraits were collected in Prague, Czech Republic, during several sessions but always in the same room under the same conditions.

Facial portraits were acquired using a standardized procedure80. Participants were photographed in front of a white background with a full-frame color camera Canon 6D using a studio flash. We used an EX SIGMA 1.4f 85 mm lens and set the focus point to the left eye. Shutter speed was set to 1/100 s, exposure to ISO 100, aperture F8, and 2/3 of strobe power. Photographs were taken from a tripod adjusted to each participant's height so that the target's face was in the middle of the image. Distance between lens and target's tip of the nose was set to 1.25 m to preserve natural variability in face size in each image and to obtain the sharpest result possible with the lens used. Participants were instructed to adopt a neutral, non-smiling facial expression and refrain from any mimic muscle activity. Participants were asked not to wear any makeup, glasses, jewelry, or other decorations and they all wore a black T-shirt.

Rating of facial images

Facial portraits were assessed for attractiveness by Czech European raters, members of the Vietnamese minority living in the Czech Republic and Vietnamese persons living in Vietnam. In Vietnam, raters were students of the University of Science and Technology of Hanoi. In Czech Republic, raters were Czech university students of European origin. The Czech Vietnamese raters were attendees of the special meeting called "Banana Fest" a multi-genre festival introducing the culture of so-called "banana children", which is a colloquial name for the second generation of Vietnamese migrants living in the Czech Republic. The inclusion criteria for Czech Vietnamese were the participant's ability to fluently speak Czech and the information that they attended Czech education system. The participants from all three rater-groups were asked to report their sexual orientation (heterosexual, homosexual, other). We used only data from individuals that identify themselves as heterosexuals.

Photographs of Czech European women were rated by Czech European men (N = 34, mean age = 28.18, SD = 4.210), Czech Vietnamese men (N = 28, mean age = 23.04, SD = 6.19) and Asian Vietnamese men (N = 53, mean age = 21.18, SD = 2.01). Photographs of Czech men were rated by Czech European women (N = 89, mean age = 27.56, SD = 4.23), Czech Vietnamese women (N = 29, mean age = 24.17, SD = 7.44) and Asian Vietnamese women (N = 32, mean age = 21.9, SD = 4.33). In other words, raters of each sex rated a set of 50 facial portraits of the other sex for attractiveness: men rated photographs of women and vice versa. Raters viewed each portrait on an IPS computer screen with resolution 1980 × 1080 pixels, using a fullscreen setup of survey session, seeing only one photograph at the time, and assessed attractiveness on a 7-point scale (ranging from 1—very unattractive to 7—very attractive). There was no time limit for exposure to each portrait. In each rating session, the order of photographs was randomized.

Cronbach’s alpha showed high interrater agreement for attractiveness judgments of Czech female photographs by male Czech European raters (N = 33, α = 0.961), male Czech Vietnamese raters (N = 27, α = 0.982), and male Asian Vietnamese raters (N = 57, α = 0.984). Female raters also showed high interrater agreement when judging the attractiveness of Czech male photographs (female Czech European raters: N = 89, α = 0.971; female Czech Vietnamese raters: N = 16, α = 0.972; female Asian Vietnamese raters: N = 32, α = 0.962).

All participants provided their informed consent by clicking on the 'I agree' button to consent with their participation in the study.

Statistical analyses

Relationships between variables were preliminarily explored using Pearson's zero-order correlation coefficient. Average ratings within each rater group (Czech European, Czech Vietnamese, and Asian Vietnamese) were first calculated for individual facial images.

To analyze the data on the level of individual ratings, we built a mixed effect model using the lmer function within the lmerTest R package81, with completely specified varying intercepts and slopes among rater groups and random intercepts of raters within rater groups and random intercepts of rated targets (faces). Attractiveness ratings were set as a response variable and age, BMI, averageness, and sexual shape dimorphism as predictors. Varying intercepts and slopes were included as interaction terms between predictors and rater groups. We evaluated contrasts between pairs of rater-groups using glht function from multcomp R package82. Two separate models were built, one for men and one for women.

In addition to mixed effect models, we used path analysis to investigate directed dependencies between perceived attractiveness, sexual shape dimorphism (SShD), averageness, individual's age, and body mass index (BMI). Path analyses were conducted using the sem function within the lavaan package available in R software for statistical computing83. First, we predicted directed dependencies among the variables (no correlational associations were investigated). Dependencies were directed according to logical constraints, so that for instance age could either positively or negatively affect the BMI but BMI could not affect a person's chronological age. No latent variables were included in the model syntax.

The main dependent variable was attractiveness. SShD, BMI, and averageness were set as mediators between age and attractiveness. Averageness and sexual dimorphism were also viewed as mediators in the association between BMI and attractiveness. A direct association between age and attractiveness was also included in the model. The number of observations was relatively low, which is why we calculated a "robust" p-value using the Monte Carlo permutation procedure. We estimated a full model fit on randomized datasets based on 10,000 simulation runs, on which we derived the distribution of expected regression coefficients.

Geometric morphometrics and anthropometric measurements

A total of 72 landmarks were digitized on the faces of 50 men and 50 women. Not all aspects of facial morphology can be described by discrete homologous points that can be unambiguously identified in every face in the sample. For this reason, 36 landmarks were identified a posteriori as semilandmarks that denote curves and outlines. Facial configurations were aligned by generalized Procrustes analysis using the gpagen function in R geomorph package84. Procrustes-aligned specimens were projected onto tangent space prior to further computations. An algorithm which reduces the bending energy between each facial configuration and Procrustes mean was used to optimize the semilandmarks positions.

To measure the level of individual expression of SShD, we applied the Procrustes fit (a generalized Procrustes analysis) to all 100 faces (both men and women). Subsequently, we determined the position of each individual facial shape along the axis connecting male and female facial averages85,86. By projecting each individual face onto a vector linking the average female and male face, we obtained SShD scores which were used in subsequent analyses.

To calculate facial averageness, we performed a Procrustes fit separately for male and female faces. Average landmark configuration (consensus) was then calculated separately for male and female faces. Procrustes distances between the consensus and each facial configuration in the set were computed and used as a measure of individual averageness. Lower facial averageness scores thus indicate particular configuration's proximity to the mean shape, while higher averageness scores indicate more distinct faces.

The weight of the photographed person was taken by scales, and the height measured by tape measure fixed on the wall. We calculated the body mass index (BMI) of a given person as weight [kg] divided by squared height [m]. The age of the person was self-reported.

Results

Pearson's correlations between attractiveness ratings and anthropometric traits are summarized in Supplementary Tables S1 and S2 for photos of men and women, respectively. The results were adjusted for multiple tests.

The results of mixed effects models are summarized in Tables 1 and 2. The attractiveness ratings of Czech Europeans were used as a standard measure of Czech facial attractiveness. Attractiveness ratings of Czech European and Czech Vietnamese raters were associated more strongly that the ratings of Czech Vietnamese and Asian Vietnamese raters.

Table 1 Summary of the results of mixed effects modelling for faces of men (female raters).
Table 2 Summary of the results of mixed effects modelling for faces of women (male raters).

Post hoc comparisons of selected pairs of rater-groups (using the Tukey HSD test) indicated that the attractiveness assessment of Asian Vietnamese women was significantly different from Czech Europeans (δ CZ−AVN = − 0.406, SE = 0.151, p = 0.02) as well as from Czech Vietnamese female raters (δ CZVN−AVN = − 0.6641, SE = 0.188, p = 0.001). Czech European and Czech Vietnamese female raters did not significantly differ in their attractiveness judgment of Czech European male faces (δ CZ−CZVN = − 0.258, SE = 0.157, p = 0.224).

In similar vein, the attractiveness assessment of Asian Vietnamese was significantly different from Czech Europeans (δ CZ−AVN = − 0.487, SE = 0.1624, p = 0.008) as well as from Czech Vietnamese male raters (δ CZVN−AVN = − 0.602, SE = 0.171, p = 0.001). Czech European and Czech Vietnamese male raters did not significantly differ in their attractiveness perception of Czech European female faces (δ CZ–CZVN = − 0.115, SE = 0.188, p = 0.814).

The preference for average male faces was statistically different neither between Czech European and Czech Vietnamese (δ CZ–CZVN = 0.002, SE = 0.028, p = 0.995) nor between Czech European and Asian Vietnamese (δ CZ–AVN = 0.056, SE = 0.027, p = 0.096,) as well as between Czech Vietnamese and Asian Vietnamese (δ CZVN–AVN = 0.054, SE = 0.034, p = 0.248) female raters. In case of female faces, the preference for faces with higher averageness was significantly different between Czech European and Czech Vietnamese (δ CZ–CZVN = − 0.13, SE = 0.043, p = 0.008) and between Czech European and Asian Vietnamese (δ CZ−AVN = − 0.174, SE = 0.037, p < 0.001) male raters. Averageness preference did not differ significantly between Czech Vietnamese and Asian Vietnamese (δ CZVN–AVN = − 0.05, SE = 0.039, p = 0.475) male raters.

The preference for facial dimorphism did significantly differ between in neither of compared rater groups for both male (δ CZ–CZVN = 0.027, SE = 0.028, p = 0.61; δ CZ–AVN = − 0.049, SE = 0.027, p = 0.17; δ CZVN–AVN = − 0.022, SE = 0.034, p = 0.788) and female photos (δ CZ–CZVN = 0.009, SE = 0.033, p = 0.959; δ CZ–AVN = 0.028, SE = 0.028, p = 0.581; δ CZVN–AVN = 0.019, SE = 0.03, p = 0.797).

We used the path analysis to trace interrelations between attractiveness, age, BMI, SShD, and averageness. In total, we performed six analyses. We separately analyzed the perception of male and female Czech faces by three groups of raters, namely Czech Europeans, the Czech Vietnamese, and the Asian Vietnamese. The results are summarized in Fig. 1.

Figure 1
figure 1

Visualization of path analyses of correlations between reported age, body mass index (BMI), sexual shape dimorphism (SShD), measured averageness (AVRG), and attractiveness. Arrows denote causal directions. The number next to a significant path describes the estimate of regression coefficient of the model with standardized variables. Green color of an arrow denotes a positive coefficient, red color a negative one. Asterisks represent the level of significance (p < 0.05*, p < 0.01**, p < 0.001***) of partial regression coefficient being non-zero. Gray arrows denote absence of a significant relationship. Apostrophe (‘) denotes a nonsignificant trend (p < 0.1, p > 0.05). The higher the SShD value, the more female sex-typical is the facial shape. Lower (negative) values correspond to more masculine facial configurations. A positive association between perceived facial attractiveness and SShD in male faces therefore means that less sex-typical male faces are perceived as more attractive. In female faces, a positive association between SShD and perceived attractiveness indicates that women with more sex-typical (feminine) faces are perceived as more attractive. In both sexes, the negative association between averageness and attractiveness means that more average faces were perceived as more attractive.

Path analyses yielded a constant cross-group structure of a causal pattern of interdependencies for both male and female Czech faces. Attractive female faces were closer to the population average. BMI and age had a negative effect on perceived facial attractiveness. In general, the effect of sex-typicality (here represented by SShD) on attractiveness was not statistically significant. Only Czech European male raters tended to be sensitive to female SShD: they associated attractiveness with more feminine-shaped faces. In the case of male faces, averageness influenced the perception of attractiveness but for Czech and Asian Vietnamese female raters, this association was not statistically significant. Otherwise, we found no other differences between Czech European, Czech Vietnamese, and Asian Vietnamese rater groups.

Discussion

As expected, Czech Vietnamese raters were more accurate than Asian Vietnamese raters at predicting the attractiveness of Czech faces (whereby attractiveness of Czech faces as assessed by Czech European raters was considered a standard). In general, however, the results regarding preferences for averageness and sex-typicality did not correspond to the initial theoretical expectations.

The reason why Czech Vietnamese raters were capable of estimating facial attractiveness of Czech faces more accurately (again, with the Czech European judgments taken as a standard) than Asian Vietnamese raters may be in part due to differences in the perceptual narrowing in early childhood. Infants discriminate between faces of their own and other populations equally well until the age of six months. Then this capacity decreases and by the ninth month of life, young children are losing this ability. In later life, people thus discriminate among faces belonging to other populations less well than among faces belonging to their own population87,88,89,90. All Czech Vietnamese raters in our sample were born in the Czech Republic or moved to the Czech Republic at a very young age. Most of them should thus have some very early experience with European faces, for instance, during visits to a Czech doctor, while the Vietnamese residing in Vietnam would not be likely to have such early experiences of European faces. The better accuracy in the assessment of attractiveness might be thus due to differences in the perceptual narrowing between the Vietnamese born in the Czech Republic and those who were born and grew up in Vietnam.

Path analysis did not reveal any substantial differences between the three compared rater groups of either sex. Czech men tend to prefer more sex-typical women (r = 0.27). Still, the association was not statistically significant (at alpha = 0.05), and the mixed effects model also did not reveal any nonrandom association between SShD and attractiveness. Female facial attractiveness was negatively associated with age. Age did not significantly affect perceived attractiveness of Czech male faces when rated by women from all three rater-groups. These results support previous findings69,72. BMI and attractiveness were mediated by facial sex-typicality in female faces. Higher BMI negatively affected the morphological femininity of Czech female faces.

The degree of sexual dimorphism varies across different human populations91. In this context, it may be relevant to note that European faces express a greater level of sexual dimorphism than faces of Asian origin92. The lower degree of sexual dimorphism in Vietnamese persons in comparison to more dimorphic Czech Europeans could be the reason why for the Asian Vietnamese sexual dimorphism (facial masculinity/femininity) may be a less efficient cue for assessing overall facial attractiveness. An additional reason why Vietnamese raters of Asian origin did not tend to utilize traits associated with sexual shape dimorphism as a cue of attractiveness may be that Asian people tend to attribute relatively greater significance to facial skin color and texture than Europeans do93. Czech Vietnamese raters followed the same pattern as their Asian peers, which seems to contradict the abovementioned assumption of importance of environmental influence. One may speculate that genetic and/or parental imprinting-like effects might play a greater role in setting the preferences for sexually dimorphic facial traits.

A possible weakness of this study may be the relatively small sample size of female raters belonging to the Vietnamese minority in the Czech Republic. The Czech Vietnamese community is not very open to outsiders and especially female members of this community willing to participate in this research were hard to find. In future research, this limitation could be reduced, for instance, by monetary reward for participation to attract more Czech Vietnamese raters, although a promise of reward may be a selection criterion that could bias the understanding of attractiveness perception among this community. Ideally, future research should be undertaken by a Czech Vietnamese researcher who might be able to secure a higher level of cooperation of the Vietnamese diaspora living in the Czech Republic.

On the whole, we found that facial averageness of female European faces (in contrast to only weak or none effect in males) plays a significant role in attractiveness assessment in all three rater groups that differ in their racial background, country of residence, or both. This is partly consistent with the "average is attractive" hypothesis41,43. In both sexes, the place of long-term residence and/or upbringing was a significant factor in their perception of facial attractiveness. Vietnamese respondents living in Czech Republic were significantly better at predicting attractiveness of Czech faces than Vietnamese living in Vietnam were.