Abstract
Measuring animal stress is fundamentally important for assessing animal emotional state and welfare. Conventional methods of quantifying stress (cortisol levels, heart rate/heart rate variability) require specialist equipment and are not instantly available. Spontaneous blink rate (SBR) has previously been used to measure stress responses in humans and may provide a non-invasive method for measuring stress in other animal species. Here we investigated the use of SBR as a measure of stress in the domestic horse. SBR was measured before and during a low-stress event (sham clipping) and compared with heart rate variability and salivary cortisol. For the entire sample, there was a reduction in SBR (startle response) during the first minute of clipping. For horses reactive to clipping, the initial reduction in SBR was followed by an increase above baseline whereas the SBR of the non-reactive horses quickly returned to baseline. For the entire sample, SBR correlated with heart rate variability and salivary cortisol. We have demonstrated that SBR is a valid fast alternative measure of stress in horses, but the initial 'startle' response must be considered when using this parameter as a measure of animal stress.
Introduction
Measuring animal stress is fundamentally important to ensuring reasonable levels of animal welfare1. Conventional methods of quantifying stress include measuring plasma and saliva cortisol levels2,3,4, eye temperature5,6, heart rate and heart rate variability7,8,9. The primary disadvantage associated with these methods is that they require specialist equipment and training in how to collect and/or analyse the data. The results are also not instantly available, requiring at least some degree of processing and analysis. Spontaneous blink rate (SBR) has previously been used to measure stress responses in humans and may provide a non-invasive method for measuring stress in a range of species10. Spontaneous blinks have several functions, they are essential for corneal lubrication and also appear to be important for resetting eye movements during the correction of fixation errors 11. Blinks also reflect cognitive processes during attention, with blink rate increasing when attentional demand is low (e.g. during punctuation pauses whilst reading12), during pauses while listening to a speech13 and also during information retrieval and memory14,15. The link between SBR and stress appears to be mediated via dopaminergic activity. SBR significantly increases in monkeys through the sub-cutaneous injection of dopamine agonists (apomorphine) and significantly blocked with dopamine antagonists (sulpiride)16 and CNS dopamine levels significantly increase during stressful events. The latter reflects this neurotransmitter’s functional role in eliciting motivation and learning processes during homeostatic behavioural responses to stress17,18.
Although the majority of evidence demonstrates that SBR increases during stress19,20 other work in horses has reported a decrease in SBR when presented with a potentially stressful event21. Human studies have shown a reduction in SBR during periods of attention and cognitive effort20,22,23,24,25. Potentially threatening stimuli may require cognitive effort to verify it as a threat thus reducing SBR at the early stage of stressor presentation. This suggests that the SBR response to stress could vary depending on (1) when it is measured in relation to the stressor exposure and (2) the type of stressor. Using the horse as a model species, the aim of this study was twofold, firstly, to validate SBR as a putative measure of stress against conventional measures of stress (HRV and salivary cortisol), and secondly, to assess the timing and consistency of the SBR response during a stress event.
Results
Spontaneous blink rates
Figure 1 shows the mean SBR (± SEM) over time with the stressor presented at the 10 min point and maintained thereafter for the duration of the experiment.
SBR over time for reactive and non-reactive horses. Mean SBR (SEM) (blinks min−1) for reactive and non-reactive horses (n = 33) during baseline (B) (minutes 1–10), initial treatment (IT) (minute 10, 1 min post-stressor presentation) and continued treatment (CT) (minutes 11–20 post-stressor presentation). P < 0.05 (*), P < 0.01 (**), P < 0.001 (***).
When partitioned into low (n = 16) and high (n = 17) reactive groups, the high reactive group showed a highly significant decrease in SBR during the IT period compared to baseline (11.1 ± 1.09 blinks per min to 6.2 ± 0.95 blinks per min, t1,30 = − 2.9, p = 0.008) and a significant increase in SBR from baseline during the CT period (11.1 ± 1.09 blinks per min to 15.6 ± 1.71 blinks per min, t1,30 = 2.7, p = 0.012). For the low reactive group, there was also a highly significant decrease in SBR during the IT compared to baseline, (11.4 ± 0.33 blinks per min to 5.8 ± 0.46 blinks per min, t1,32 = − 7.9, p < 0.001), but no significant difference in SBR during CT compared to baseline B (11.4 ± 0.33 blinks per min to 10.0 ± 0.67 blinks per min, t1,32 = − 1.9, p = 0.068).
Correlation of SBR with standard measures of stress
For all animals (low reactive and high reactive) combined, there was a highly significant moderate positive correlation between the change in SBR between baseline (B) and the continued treatment (CT) (ΔSBR) and change in cortisol (ΔCortisol) (r = 0.56, p < 0.001) (Fig. 2) and a highly significant strong negative correlation between ΔSBR and ΔRMSSD (r = − 0.63, p < 0.001) (Fig. 3).
The HRV and cortisol results for each horse at each time-point can be found in Supplementary Table S1 online.
Discussion
A stress response was seen in both the HPA axis and SAM system demonstrating a combined axes response to a psychological stressor. It has been suggested that activation of the SAM axis predominates during times of physical stress whereas the HPA axis is activated during psychological stress26,27,28. The results presented here demonstrate that a predominantly psychological stressor elicits a response in both the HPA and SAM axes, with significant elevation in saliva cortisol and reduction in heart rate variability respectively.
The increased SBR correlating with an increased physiological stress response agrees with previous studies in humans19,20 but conflicts with the previous study in horses21 that reported a reduced SBR during stressful situations. One of the key differences between the work of Merkies et al.21 and the present study is that the former used three stressors, one of which was a simple startle test and the other two (separation and withholding food) were only recorded for the first 3 min. Here we report that during the first minute of the stressor (sham clipping), for both low and high reactive animals, there was a highly significant reduction in SBR. The reduction was larger in the non-reactive group and from the video analysis it could be seen that many of the high reactive horses had a very distinct period of non-blinking/startle response at the initial presentation of the clippers, but this only lasted a few seconds before rapidly increasing. As previously discussed, the initial SBR response may reflect an initial increase in concentration/vigilance response to the stressor20,22,23 which would explain why in the early stages of a stressful event (particularly a startle test), a reduced SBR was observed21.
It was of note that low reactive horses (that did not become stressed by the sham clipping stressor event) spent longer in a state of low SBR-high concentration response (1 min or more verses a few seconds for the highly reactive group). This strongly suggests that more stress reactive animals may also be prone to making faster environmental assessments in relation to potential threats. However, it must also be considered that none of the animals in the current study were naïve to the stressor, so for highly reactive horses, this may also have been a learned response that did not need lengthy consideration. Overall, the data demonstrate that the initial startle response, as shown by the initial significant reduction in SBR, needs to be taken into consideration when using SBR as potential measure of animal stress.
After the first minute following stressor presentation and the startle response, high reactive horses (n = 16) produced a highly significant increase in SBR above baseline, which was maintained for the duration of the experiment, whilst low reactive animals' SBR returned to baseline levels after the first minute. SBR therefore appeared to be a valid parameter of stress measurement. One of the primary aims of this study was to validate SBR as a measure of stress through correlation against conventional measures of stress centred around activation of the HPA and SAM axes. The Pearson’s correlation between ΔSBR and the change in stress parameters for the whole sample (n = 33), found a highly significant (p ≤ 0.001) correlation between ΔSBR and HRV (as a measure of SAM system activation—more specifically, RMSSD as an implied measure of parasympathetic/vagal tone) and salivary cortisol (as a measure of HPA axis activation), with a higher SBR correlating with a higher physiological stress response. The correlation between SBR and RMSSD was marginally stronger (0.63) than the correlation between SBR and salivary cortisol (0.53). This may suggest that SBR reflects greater activation of one axis over the other. SBR is mediated by activation of the D1 and D2 dopamine receptors in the striatum16 and stress increases levels of dopamine (DA) in this region of the brain17,18,29,30. Although there are glucocorticoid receptors at both ends of the messoaccumbens pathway (ventral tegmentum area and nucleus accumbens) and increased levels of glucocorticoids initiate DA release along this mesoaccumbens pathway31, there is also a temporal lag (approximately 30 min) between stressor exposure and peak plasma glucocorticoid levels2,3,4,26. Thus, the release of glucocorticoids into the peripheral blood system to then stimulate glucocorticoid receptors in the striatum cannot be attributed as the causal factor for increased SBR. However, Corticotropin Releasing Hormone (CRH) (which initiates the neuroendocrine signalling in the HPA axis) can also stimulate DA release in the ventral striatum32 suggesting that the SBR may be mechanistically related to the co-activation of the HPA axis.
In relation to the SAM axis, the sympathetic nervous system (SNS) is activated during periods of high vigilance33 and thus could be responsible for the reduced SBR reported during the IT phase in this and other studies21,34. However, dopaminergic innervation of the central nucleus of the amygdala from substantia nigra appears to be critical for the enhanced attention response during presentation of unexpected stimuli via its control over the parietal cortex35. It is well established that the posterior parietal cortex is a key structure during attentional response and mediates this functional role through direct influence over the frontal eye fields and superior colliculus to control volitional eye movement36 as well as blink rate37. This may therefore be the primary candidate mechanism underlying both the reduced blink rate during the startle response and the heightened blink rate during more prolonged periods of stress-inducing elevation of central dopaminergic systems.
A limitation of the study is that we were unable to specifically quantify the SNS activity of the ANS. Previously, this has been assumed from measures of LF Power and LF/HF ratio of HRV, but this method has been widely criticised38,39,40. As outlined in the review by Stucke, et al.41, it would be inappropriate to draw any inference about SNS activity from the time domain parameter reported in this study, although this does not detract from the application of SBR as a welfare assessment tool. Measurement of electrodermal activity (EDA) is a renowned marker of sympathetic activity40 and use of this measure may further elucidate the relationship of the autonomic nervous system in the blink response to stress.
Conclusion
SBR is a simple, cost-free, instantaneous measure of acute equine stress that can support welfare assessment, as demonstrated through the significant increase from baseline during exposure to a known stressor. This is further validated through the significant correlation of SBR with conventional measures of stress associated with the activation of the HPA and SAM axes. The startle response must be taken into consideration when using SBR as a measure of stress and the duration of the startle response may vary significantly between low and high stress reactivity animals. As a final point and caveat to the use of SBR as a measure of animal stress, dopamine also increases in the striatum during the presentation of reward substrates42. Thus theoretically, reward or positive events may also induce a significant increase in SBR. Understanding the applied use of the SBR response, both in horses and other animal species, will be improved by future research assessing the effects of positive stimuli on changes in SBR and also further validation of SBR against conventional measures of stress based around the SNS and HPA axis.
Methods
Animals
Thirty three horses (mean ± SEM age 13 ± 0.7 years; 21 geldings, 12 mares) stabled at the Moreton Morrell campus of Warwickshire College were used for the study. All horses were physically fit in regular exercise as general riding school horses for up to 4 h per day and none were on any medication during sampling or for the 2 weeks prior to sampling. Diets were individually tailored depending on size and workload but generally the dietary intake was divided 70–80% haylage, 20–30% cool mix (9.8 MJ/kg / 10% protein). All horses were housed in an American barn style stable block. The upper half of the stable partitions were bars, allowing the horses to see conspecifics on either side of them as well as those opposite. Four hours of group turn-out was available every other day. Of the 33 horses used in the study, 16 were categorised by animal staff as reactive to the exposed stressor (sight and sound of hair clippers) (high reactive) and the 17 remaining animals were categorised non-reactive to the stressor (low reactive). This assessment was based on the horses’ previous reactions to being clipped and human safety during the clipping process (in their role as teaching horses at the equine college); the 17 non-reactive horses were designated as safe for students to clip, whereas the 16 reactive horses required a member of staff to clip them or had to be sedated during the clipping process. Ethical approval for this study was granted by the Veterinary Ethical Review Committee for the Royal (Dick) School of Veterinary Studies and the Research Ethics Committee for Warwickshire College Group. All methods were performed in accordance with the guidelines and regulations of these two institutions.
Study design
Data were collected during a period of experimentally-induced mild stress following a protocol similar to Yarnell et al.6, whereby horses were subjected to a controlled ‘sham clipping’ (sight and sound of hair clippers) for a period of 10 min. This entailed holding a set of clippers (Liveryman Arena C130, UK) against the horses’ left shoulder in a way that the hair was not actually cut. Ten minutes of clipper exposure has previously been reported to cause a significant elevation in salivary cortisol6 and thus this same duration was used in this experiment. Each horse acted as its own control to assess the relationship between variables (within-horse) during periods of stress and no stress, with measurements taken during the sham clipping being compared with the ‘at rest’ measurements for that horse. The ‘at rest’ measurements were always taken before exposure to the stressor in order to avoid any carry-over effects and each horse was only sampled once. All measurements were taken between 1100 and 1300 each day to avoid any confounding influences of diurnal variation in physiological recordings43. Each procedure was carried out in the horses’ own stable.
Heart rate variability
Each horse was fitted with a heart rate monitor (Polar Equine V800 Science, Polar Electro Oy, Kempele, Finland) and given a 5 min acclimatisation period. Heart rate and video recording was then initiated at the same time to ensure that the time stamp on the video would relate directly to the time on the HR trace. The horse was monitored for 10 min at rest before the clippers were started and the sham clipping procedure conducted for the next 10 min. This provided a continuous 20 min video and HR trace.
Heart Rate Variability (HRV) time domain analysis (Root mean square of the successive differences of the beat to beat interval, recorded in milliseconds [RMSSD]) of HRV data were sampled for 10 min using a Polar Equine V800 Science heart rate monitor. The earlier model (Polar S810) has been validated in horses44,45 and although Parker et al.44 found discrepancies existed during movement, both Parker et al.44 and Ille et al.45 found better agreement when the horse was static (as they were during the current study). Randle et al.46 proposed that recent technological improvements (i.e. the newer Polar V800) could improve the reliability of the data generated and Giles et al.47 demonstrated that (in humans) the V800 was indeed an improvement over the earlier models. Although this specific model has not been validated in horses, it has demonstrated good interclass correlations with a clinical ECG in humans47.
RMSSD and SDNN are both measures of PNS activation, but SDNN is a more suitable metric for long-term changes (minimum of 5 min required)39. SD1 is identical to RMSSD48, although some authors still report these as separate measures. SD2 closely correlates to SDNN and is therefore only suitable for assessing long-term variance. We initially anticipated reporting the LF/HF ratio as cautiously suggested in the review by Stucke et al.41 as a measure of SNS activity in horses, but this (and LF power) are now seen as unreliable measures of SNS activity (in humans)40. There is president in applying the findings of human studies to other mammals as outlined in the review by Von Borell et al.9, primarily because the neural basis of an emotional response is similar in all mammals, as is the control of vagal tone. It could be argued that the uniquely high resting vagal tone of horses makes them a special case41, but in the interests of robustness, the LF/HF ratio and LF Power where excluded from this analysis.
Data were analysed using Kubios software (Kubios HRV Standard version 3.1.0, Kubios Oy, Kuopio, Finland49), using a low threshold (0.35 s) for artifact correction. Any data with more than 5% errors were discarded50.
Salivary cortisol
Salivary cortisol samples were taken whilst the horse was at rest 15 min prior to the start of recording and again at 60 min after the start of the clipping procedure. This took into account the time lag of the peak increase in salivary cortisol following a stressful event2,3 and avoided sampling (itself a potential stressor) during the recording period.
Saliva samples were taken using Salivette cortisol swabs (Sarstedt, Nümbrecht, Germany). The swabs were mounted in Foerster 9.5 inch straight sponge forceps to avoid accidental loss and to ensure the safety of the operator. Using these forceps, the swab was held under the horses’ tongue for 90 s. In accordance with the manufactures’ instructions, the Salivettes were stored at 4 °C immediately after collection and then centrifuged at 1000 g for 2 min (Sigma 3K30 Centrifuge, Sigma Laboratory Centrifuges, Osterode am Harz, Germany). The extracted saliva was frozen in the Salivette collection tube at − 20 °C within 1 h of collection. The frozen samples were then transported, on dry ice, for analysis within 3 months of collection. The cortisol competitive immunoassay was carried out using a Salimetrics salivary cortisol enzyme immunoassay kit (Stratech Scientific Ltd., Ely, UK) in accordance with the manufacturer’s instructions.
Both the Sarstedt swabs and the Salimetrics ELISA have been previously been validated for use in the horse2,3,51,52.
Spontaneous blink rate
The method for measuring SBR in horses has previously been described by Roberts et al.53. In brief, the horses were lightly restrained using a head-collar and lead-rope to prevent them from moving around and observations were made by a single observer with a clear view of the horses’ left eye. Only one eye can be observed at any one time due to their lateral positioning. The left eye was selected following the experimental protocol adopted by Roberts et al.53 and also because horses demonstrate a left gaze bias in relation to stressful events54. Thus, measuring the left eye was considered to give a more accurate indication of the stress response and was kept consistent throughout the project. Furthermore, it is convention to approach and handle horses on their left-hand side, so filming from the left was potentially less likely to cause a stress response than if the horses’ head was approached from the right-hand side.
The area of the eye was filmed using a High Definition digital camera (Fujifilm Finepix S6800) at 60 frames s−1 to allow post hoc measurements to be made. Blink rate was reported as blinks per minute by the same observer for each sample. In contrast to the reporting of eye flutters and half closures by Merkies et al.21, this study counted the binary eye fully open or eye fully closed. As this was not a subjective measure, it was felt that the addition of a second, blinded observer would have only offered a minimal improvement in reporting. A clip of the video collected can be found in the Supplementary Information online.
Statistical analysis
All statistical analysis and data visualisation were carried out using R version 3.5.355.
A linear mixed effects model (with individual horse as the random effect) was fitted to the data to compare the SBR at baseline (B) (minutes 0–10) with the initial treatment (IT) (minute 10) and with the continued treatment (CT) (minutes 11–20). The model assumptions were tested by plotting the normal probability of the residuals. The change in each parameter was calculated (during stress minus before stress) and then all the parameters were compared using a Pearson Correlation. When performing the correlations, the data were analysed as the complete sample (N = 33). For analysis of the SBR, the data were partitioned into the two groups (low reactive [N = 16] and high reactive [N = 17]).
References
Broom, D. M. & Johnson, K. G. in Stress and Animal Welfare Animal Behaviour (eds D. M. Broom & K. G. Johnson) 57–86 (Chapman and Hall, 1993).
Peeters, M., Sulon, J., Beckers, J. F., Ledoux, D. & Vandenheede, M. Comparison between blood serum and salivary cortisol concentrations in horses using an adrenocorticotropic hormone challenge. Equine Vet. J. 43, 487–493 (2011).
Scheidegger, M. D. et al. Repeatability of the ACTH stimulation test as reflected by salivary cortisol response in healthy horses. Domest. Anim. Endocrinol. 57, 43–47 (2016).
Videlock, E. J. et al. The effect of sex and irritable bowel syndrome on HPA axis response and peripheral glucocorticoid receptor expression. Psychoneuroendocrinology 69, 67–76 (2016).
Squibb, K., Griffin, K., Favier, R. & Ijichi, C. Poker Face: Discrepancies in behaviour and affective states in horses during stressful handling procedures. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 202, 34–38 (2018).
Yarnell, K., Hall, C. & Billett, E. An assessment of the aversive nature of an animal management procedure (clipping) using behavioral and physiological measures. Physiol. Behav. 118, 32–39 (2013).
Mohr, E., Witte, E. & Voss, B. Heart rate variability as stress indicator. Archiv fur Tierzucht 43, 171–176 (2000).
Sleigh, J. & Henderson, J. Heart rate variability and preoperative anxiety. Acta Anaesthesiol. Scand. 39, 1059–1061 (1995).
Von Borell, E. et al. Heart rate variability as a measure of autonomic regulation of cardiac activity for assessing stress and welfare in farm animals—a review. Physiol. Behav. 92, 293–316 (2007).
Roberts, K., Hemmings, A. J., Moore-Colyer, M., Parker, M. O. & McBride, S. D. Neural modulators of temperament: A multivariate approach to personality trait identification in the horse. Physiol. Behav. 167, 125–131 (2016).
Khazali, M. F., Pomper, J. K. & Thier, P. Blink associated resetting eye movements (BARMs) are functionally complementary to microsaccades in correcting for fixation errors. Sci. Rep. 7, 16823. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-17229-w (2017).
Orchard, L. N. & Stern, J. A. Blinks as an index of cognitive activity during reading. Integr. Physiol. Behav. Sci. 26, 108–116. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf02691032 (1991).
Nakano, T. & Kitazawa, S. Eyeblink entrainment at breakpoints of speech. Exp. Brain Res. 205, 577–581. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-010-2387-z (2010).
Dejong, P. J. & Merckelbach, H. Eyeblink frequency, rehearsal activity, and sympathetic arousal. Int. J. Neurosci. 51, 89–94. https://doi.org/10.3109/00207459009000513 (1990).
Bentivoglio, A. R. et al. Analysis of blink rate patterns in normal subjects. Mov. Disord. 12, 1028–1034. https://doi.org/10.1002/mds.870120629 (1997).
Karson, C. N. Spontaneous eye-blink rates and dopaminergic systems. Brain 106, 643–653 (1983).
Belujon, P. & Grace, A. A. Regulation of dopamine system responsivity and its adaptive and pathological response to stress. Proc. R. Soc. B: Biol. Sci. 282, 20142516 (2015).
Pruessner, J. C., Champagne, F., Meaney, M. J. & Dagher, A. Dopamine release in response to a psychological stress in humans and its relationship to early life maternal care: a positron emission tomography study using [11C] raclopride. J. Neurosci. 24, 2825–2831 (2004).
Tichon, J. G., Wallis, G., Riek, S. & Mavin, T. Physiological measurement of anxiety to evaluate performance in simulation training. Cogn. Technol. Work 16, 203–210 (2014).
Giannakakis, G. et al. Stress and anxiety detection using facial cues from videos. Biomed. Signal Process. Control 31, 89–101 (2017).
Merkies, K., Ready, C., Farkas, L. & Hodder, A. Eye blink rates and eyelid twitches as a non-invasive measure of stress in the domestic horse. Animals 9, 562 (2019).
Berguer, R., Smith, W. & Chung, Y. Performing laparoscopic surgery is significantly more stressful for the surgeon than open surgery. Surg. Endosc. 15, 1204–1207 (2001).
Joiner, T. E. et al. Staring down death: Is abnormally slow blink rate a clinically useful indicator of acute suicide risk?. Crisis: J. Crisis Interv. Suicide Prevent. 37, 212 (2016).
Kwee-Meier, S. T., Mertens, A. & Schlick, C. M. Age-related differences in decision-making for digital escape route signage under strenuous emergency conditions of tilted passenger ships. Appl. Ergonom. 59, 264–273 (2017).
25Newman, M. & Tattersall, A. J. 33 Physiological measures and data load: a direct measure of situation awareness? Engineering Psychology and Cognitive Ergonomics: Volume 1: Transportation Systems, 307 (2017).
Skoluda, N. et al. Intra-individual psychological and physiological responses to acute laboratory stressors of different intensity. Psychoneuroendocrinology 51, 227–236 (2015).
Schwabe, L., Haddad, L. & Schachinger, H. HPA axis activation by a socially evaluated cold-pressor test. Psychoneuroendocrinology 33, 890–895 (2008).
Potts, S. R., McCuddy, W. T., Jayan, D. & Porcelli, A. J. To trust, or not to trust? Individual differences in physiological reactivity predict trust under acute stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology 100, 75–84 (2019).
Finlay, J., Zigmond, M. & Abercrombie, E. Increased dopamine and norepinephrine release in medial prefrontal cortex induced by acute and chronic stress: effects of diazepam. Neuroscience 64, 619–628 (1995).
Puglisi-Allegra, S., Imperato, A., Angelucci, L. & Cabib, S. Acute stress induces time-dependent responses in dopamine mesolimbic system. Brain Res. 554, 217–222 (1991).
Oswald, L. M. et al. Relationships among ventral striatal dopamine release, cortisol secretion, and subjective responses to amphetamine. Neuropsychopharmacology 30, 821 (2005).
Lemos, J. C. et al. Severe stress switches CRF action in the nucleus accumbens from appetitive to aversive. Nature 490, 402 (2012).
Godoy, L. D., Rossignoli, M. T., Pereira, P. D., Garcia-Cairasco, N. & Umeoka, E. H. D. L. A comprehensive overview on stress neurobiology: basic concepts and clinical implications. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 12, 127 (2018).
MacRae, A. M., Makowska, I. J. & Fraser, D. Initial evaluation of facial expressions and behaviours of harbour seal pups (Phoca vitulina) in response to tagging and microchipping. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 205, 167–174 (2018).
Lee, H. J., Youn, J. M., Mary, J. O., Gallagher, M. & Holland, P. C. Role of substantia nigra-amygdala connections in surprise-induced enhancement of attention. J. Neurosci. 26, 6077–6081. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1316-06.2006 (2006).
Gottlieb, J., Balan, P. F., Oristaglio, J. & Schneider, D. Task specific computations in attentional maps. Vision. Res. 49, 1216–1226. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2008.03.023 (2009).
Shibutani, H., Sakata, H. & Hyvarinen, J. Saccade and blinking evoked by microstimulation of the posterior parietal association cortex of the monkey. Exp. Brain Res. 55, 1–8. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf00240493 (1984).
Billman, G. E. The LF/HF ratio does not accurately measure cardiac sympatho-vagal balance. Front. Physiol. 4, 26 (2013).
Teckenberg-Jansson, P. et al. Effects of live music therapy on heart rate variability and self-reported stress and anxiety among hospitalized pregnant women: a randomized controlled trial. Nordic J. Music Therapy 28, 7–26 (2019).
40Thomas, B. L., Claassen, N., Becker, P. & Viljoen, M. Validity of commonly used heart rate variability markers of autonomic nervous system function. Neuropsychobiology, 1–13 (2019).
Stucke, D., Ruse, M. G. & Lebelt, D. Measuring heart rate variability in horses to investigate the autonomic nervous system activity–Pros and cons of different methods. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 166, 1–10 (2015).
Schultz, W., Dayan, P. & Montague, P. R. A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science 275, 1593–1599 (1997).
Barbato, G. et al. Diurnal variation in spontaneous eye-blink rate. Psychiatry Res. 93, 145–151 (2000).
Parker, M., Goodwin, D., Eager, R. A., Redhead, E. S. & Marlin, D. J. Comparison of Polar® heart rate interval data with simultaneously recorded ECG signals in horses. Comp. Exercise Physiol. 6, 137–142 (2009).
Ille, N., Erber, R., Aurich, C. & Aurich, J. Comparison of heart rate and heart rate variability obtained by heart rate monitors and simultaneously recorded electrocardiogram signals in nonexercising horses. J. Vet. Behav. 9, 341–346 (2014).
Randle, H., Steenbergen, M., Roberts, K. & Hemmings, A. The use of the technology in equitation science: A panacea or abductive science?. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 190, 57–73 (2017).
Giles, D., Draper, N. & Neil, W. Validity of the Polar V800 heart rate monitor to measure RR intervals at rest. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 116, 563–571 (2016).
Ciccone, A. B. et al. Reminder: RMSSD and SD1 are identical heart rate variability metrics. Muscle Nerve 56, 674–678 (2017).
49Kubios HRV Standard, <https://www.kubios.com/hrv-standard/>
Schöberl, I., Kortekaas, K., Schöberl, F. F. & Kotrschal, K. Algorithm-supported visual error correction (AVEC) of heart rate measurements in dogs, Canis lupus familiaris. Behav. Res. Methods 47, 1356–1364. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-014-0546-z (2015).
Ellis, A. D., Stephenson, M., Preece, M. & Harris, P. A novel approach to systematically compare behavioural patterns between and within groups of horses. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 161, 60–74 (2014).
Schmidt, A., Aurich, J., Möstl, E., Müller, J. & Aurich, C. Changes in cortisol release and heart rate and heart rate variability during the initial training of 3-year-old sport horses. Horm. Behav. 58, 628–636 (2010).
Roberts, K., Hemmings, A., Moore-Colyer, M. & Hale, C. Cognitive differences in horses performing locomotor versus oral stereotypic behaviour. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 168, 37–44 (2015).
Smith, A. V., Proops, L., Grounds, K., Wathan, J. & McComb, K. Functionally relevant responses to human facial expressions of emotion in the domestic horse (Equus caballus). Biol. Let. 12, 20150907 (2016).
55The R Project for Statistical Computing, <https://www.r-project.org/>
Acknowledgements
We thank Bryony Lancaster and the MSc Distance Learning team of the Royal (Dick) School of Veterinary Studies, the University of Edinburgh, for their support during this project. We thank the yard staff at the Moreton Morrell Campus of Warwickshire College Group for the provision of the horses used in the study and Tony Shakespeare for his assistance with filming. RM received funding from the International Society for Equitation Science (Student Research Grant). IBERS receives strategic funding from BBSRC.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
R.M. designed the experiments, acquired and interpreted the data and drafted the manuscript. S.M. contributed to the design of the experiment, the interpretation of the data and revised the manuscript critically for important intellectual content. S.H. performed and interpreted the cortisol ELISAs. All authors discussed the results, commented on the manuscript and gave final approval of the version to be submitted.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
Supplementary Information 2.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Mott, R.O., Hawthorne, S.J. & McBride, S.D. Blink rate as a measure of stress and attention in the domestic horse (Equus caballus). Sci Rep 10, 21409 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-78386-z
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-78386-z
This article is cited by
-
Going Deeper than Tracking: A Survey of Computer-Vision Based Recognition of Animal Pain and Emotions
International Journal of Computer Vision (2023)
-
The effect of stress and exercise on the learning performance of horses
Scientific Reports (2022)
Comments
By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.