Abstract
The association of immobility and camouflage is widespread as a defensive mechanism in prey from varied taxa. However, many experiments assessing the reaction of prey to predator cues are conducted under artificial laboratory conditions. In a previous experiment we observed the tadpoles of Ololygon machadoi (Hylidae) to respond to predator visual and/or chemical cues by choosing backgrounds that improve their disruptive properties, but detected no associated reduction of movement. Here we experimentally demonstrate this response in the species' natural habitat, on backgrounds where the tadpoles are likely to achieve their best camouflage. We also tested whether previous experiences could influence both background choice and immobility in O. machadoi tadpoles. These novel experimental results suggest that a defensive behavior—i.e., reduction of movement—in these tadpoles is more strongly expressed under the natural conditions where they evolved, compared to laboratory conditions where prey and predator were brought into closer contact. Besides, previous experiences are likely to play an important role in expressed defensive responses.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
Prey species are under constant selection to escape predation. They can develop predator specific responses, but some responses seem to be widespread, such as spatial avoidance of predators and reduction of activity1. In this context prey species that count on camouflage to reduce the probability of being detected by predators tend to remain motionless, aiding to the protective properties of their colouration2,3. Reduction of movement is likely to reduce detection probability and can emerge as a spontaneous response when the detected threat is at a distance (background threat), but an approaching (immediate) threat may otherwise elicit an escape response4. Previous experience can also determine the reaction of prey to specific predators5, however there is evidence that evolutionary pressures may have led prey species to develop different defensive mechanisms and express each one in appropriate contexts determined by the assessed threat4. Both innate and learned defensive behaviours can be expressed even in very early life stages6,7.
Tadpoles, the larvae of biphasic anuran amphibians, are frequently employed in experiments conducted to study the reaction of prey to predator cues8, due to their broad range of predators and defensive mechanisms1. It has been shown that many species of tadpoles reduce movement in the presence of predators1,9. However, these studies are usually conducted under laboratory or mesocosm conditions, sometimes within reduced spaces in artificial standardized containers where predator cues are manipulated, other factors kept equal, to record tadpole reaction to particular cues1,10. Under such circumstances, both visual and chemical stimuli have been shown to elicit anti-predator responses in tadpoles11,12. In natural habitats, on the other hand, tadpoles should be selected to identify and react to predator cues within a more complex context of stimuli, representing a more realistic situation.
Although tadpole neurons may have little sensitivity in early life stages, they can quickly evolve to deliver sensory-driven spikes to the developing brain when exposed to natural visual stimuli, such plasticity being adaptive to the animal’s sensory environment13,14. Xenopus tadpoles can respond to visual stimuli as soon as retinal ganglion cells innervate in the brain, different from other vertebrates, who develop this response days or even weeks later, what may be an adaptation to the great need to detect and avoid predators15. On the other hand, the visual acuity of tadpoles may at least in some cases not be sharp enough to detect predators visually at distances beyond their close vicinities, as studies conducted on some species show that they are myopic16. However, as the aforementioned studies were conducted on single species, the data available is still too limited to take conclusions about the accuracy of tadpole vision in general.
The treefrog Ololygon machadoi has conspicuously coloured larvae: especially the young tadpoles have a dark brown body with an anterior and a posterior transversal yellow band, and this pattern is expected to be disruptive on yellowish backgrounds17. In laboratory experiments, these tadpoles increased the use of yellow backgrounds in the presence of visual and/or chemical cues of water bugs (Belostoma testaceopallidum Latreille, 1807)18, but did not show decreased movement as would be expected in association with background matching3. In the Melo et al.18 study, visual, chemical and both stimuli were provided by addition of a water bug inside a transparent container, a black container with holes, and a transparent container with holes, respectively, to the trays where tadpoles were tested. The tadpoles used in that study were collected at the same location of the present study and were immediately taken to the laboratory inside plastic bags with water from the original stream in polystirene foam boxes. At the laboratory, they were kept under natural light, in the shade, and tested within the next 10 days.
Although laboratory experiments can be carefully planed to avoid stress to the experimental subjects, they increase precision in detriment of realistic conditions19. The lack of a decreased movement response of the O. machadoi tadpoles observed under such experimental conditions may have been due to the proximity of the predator eliciting an escape response. Otherwise, the effects of previous experiences may have induced varied individual reactions, masking the effects of tested predator cues. Thus, we decided to replicate this experiment in situ, in the natural habitat where the tadpoles occur and using the same native predators, in order to check whether tadpoles would reduce their movement rate under these more realistic conditions when exposed to predator cues at a greater distance.
In order to evaluate whether previous experience can influence expression of defensive behaviours, we also tested whether tadpoles could perceive backgrounds as safer and increase their use in response to previous experiences. Because movement may be important to elicit defensive responses14, we tested whether tadpoles of O. machadoi would respond differently when subject to aversive stimuli (an approaching object) on different backgrounds by using an alternative (“safer”) background. We also tested for decreased mobility after exposure to such aversive stimuli. In this case, because the needed background manipulation could not be done in situ, we conducted the experiments in trays with pictures of natural backgrounds on the bottom, with stream water and natural light and temperature conditions at the study site.
Results
Tadpoles of Ololygon machadoi reduced their movement in the presence of predator cues in their natural habitat. The best model included only phase (before, during, or after the presence of the water bug) as explanatory variable to tadpole level of activity (p < 0.0001; Table 1). Weather had no influence on tadpole behaviour regarding number of movements, as the model including only weather had lower explanatory power than the null model (Table 1). Tukey post hoc tests indicated that tadpoles moved significantly more before predator presentation than both during predator presence (estimate = 0.371, SD = 0.093, z = 4.000, p = 0.0002) and after predator removal (estimate = 0.376, SD = 0.093, z = 4.050, p = 0.0002; Fig. 1). However, number of movements did not differ during predator presence and after its removal (estimate = 0.005, SD = 0.100, z = 0.051, p = 0.998; Fig. 1). Although we did not monitor tadpole position in the enclosures, we noticed no tendency of tadpoles to move away from the predator.
For the experiment conducted to evaluate the effect of previous experience on background choice by tadpoles, the best model included phase, treatment and their interaction (p = 0.017, Table 1, Fig. 1). Dark background usage frequency differed significantly between the two treatments after the aversive stimulus was applied: tadpoles used significantly more the dark backgrounds after they experienced aversive stimuli on the yellow background than on the dark background itself (Tukey estimate = − 0.248, z = − 3.40, p = 0.009). As expected, no differences were observed within the control and among the control and the phases before the aversive stimuli in the treatments.
For tadpole activity, the best model also included phase, treatment and their interaction (p < 0.001, Table 1, Fig. 2). Tadpoles moved less after the treatments with aversive stimuli were applied on the dark backgrounds (Tukey estimate = − 0.299, z = − 4.234, p < 0.001) but no changes in activity levels happened in the control (Tukey estimate = − 0.11391, z = − 1.724, p = 0.505) or in the treatment with aversive stimuli on the yellow background (Tukey estimate = − 0.10114, Z = − 1.477, p = 0.669; Fig. 2).
Discussion
In a previous study under laboratory conditions18, no reduction of activity levels was observed for Ololygon machadoi tadpoles in response to visual and / or chemical cues from water bugs (Belostoma testaceopallidum). On the contrary, here we show that the tadpoles reduce their activity levels in situ when confronted with predators at considerable distances (up to 35 cm). They also maintained the reduced mobility response for the next 15 min phase of the experiment. This is in accordance with observations on Xenopus laevis tadpoles, that maintained a learned danger avoidance response even after 180 min of an experienced aversive stimulus20.
Tadpoles are likely selected to detect their predators before they are detected10, and this may explain the ability of tadpoles to respond to the predator under natural conditions where predator cues are less noticeable. Our results suggest that the artificial conditions of laboratory experiments may sometimes alter animal behaviours, so extrapolation of results to in situ conditions should be done carefully. It is possible that tadpoles assessed their natural backgrounds as safer (regarding camouflaging properties) than the artificial backgrounds used in laboratory experiments and thus expressed their associated immobility behaviour more efficiently. It is also possible that tadpoles did not respond to predator cues properly under laboratory conditions due to additional stress caused by the unfamiliar artificial surroundings.
An alternative explanation for our results could be a higher initial movement rate to explore new surroundings followed by tadpole settling down / getting tired and moving less. Although we did not test for this possibility in the in situ experiment, it seems unlikely based on the results of the experiment conducted in the trays. Tadpoles were placed in trays with a similar area (1,255 cm2 compared to 1,290 cm2 in the in situ experiment) and in the controls, where tadpoles received no aversive stimuli, as well as in treatments aimed at associating the yellow background with threat, movement rates did not change over time. These experiments lasted 105 min, a time period longer than the duration of the experiments conducted in the stream (54 min) and still not enough for tadpoles to decrease movement rates without interference (an aversive stimulus or predator).
Our results show that tadpoles of Ololygon machadoi were able to learn to avoid backgrounds where they are in danger based on colour. The ability to associate aversive stimuli with specific habitats and avoid them may be an important adaptation to avoid predation21. Xenopus laevis tadpoles learned to avoid specific wavelengths and intensities associated to aversive stimuli under laboratory conditions20, indicating that tadpoles can recognize colour and learn from their experiences. Ololygon machadoi tadpoles also showed a significantly decreased movement response when presented to aversive stimuli on dark backgrounds and moved towards yellow backgrounds (see Fig. 3), what may reinforce the protective effect of camouflage. Innate preferences are likely to influence on the results of learning processes20, and an innate (or learned) preference for yellow backgrounds seems to prevail in tadpoles of O. machadoi17. When moving towards yellow backgrounds, the tadpoles may be more likely to express immobility if they have an innate or learned ability to associate yellow backgrounds with protection (through camouflage).
Reduction of movement rates is a very widespread defensive mechanism among tadpoles1,22,23, and the association between camouflage and immobility for defense occurs not only in tadpoles, but also in adult frogs2 and other animals3. Movement is likely to undo the protective effect of camouflage, so that the effects of camouflage and immobility are synergistic in reducing detection rates and, expectedly, predation2,3. The widespread association of camouflage and immobility in varied prey species corroborates the selective value of the movement decrease response for prey, what may explain why tadpoles were able to react to predator presence in the natural habitat, even with likely subtle cues and other potentially influencing variables (e.g. food availability). In the natural habitat they were exposed to the trade-off between moving to eat and standing still in the presence of food (that was not available during the experiments in the laboratory), so they would have an additional reason to move24. The reduction of movement in the presence of the predator (and shortly after it) reinforces the adaptive importance of this behaviour.
In summary, our results show that tadpoles of Ololygon machadoi can adjust their defensive behaviour based on surrounding conditions and previous experiences, fine-tuning their anti-predator strategies. Similar abilities have also been shown for other species25. This flexibility should be taken into account in studies on tadpole behaviour in order to best adjust experimental design to aims and to interpret results with care.
Methods
Study site
Experiments were conducted in the Reserva Particular do Patrimônio Natural (RPPN) Santuário do Caraça, a private conservation unit in the southern portion of the Espinhaço Mountain range, Minas Gerais state, Brazil. The climate is seasonal, with a rainy period from October to March, and a dry period from April to September. Mean air temperatures vary between 13 and 29oC26.
The focal species of this study was Ololygon machadoi. The tadpoles of this treefrog have been previously shown to react to both visual and chemical predator cues (from Belostoma testaceopallidum; Melo et al.18) by positioning themselves preferentially on yellow backgrounds where they are disruptive17,18. Ololygon machadoi breeds year-round in many streams in the RPPN, and we used one of these streams (20o05′37″S, 43o29′59″W; 1293 m above sea level), where its tadpoles are abundant, to conduct the experiments. It is a small first order stream27 with sandy or rocky bottoms. Stream bank vegetation is dense, composed of herbs, shrubs and trees. In the vicinities of the point where we conducted the experiments (and up to 150 m upstream) stream width ranges from 2 to 6 m, and stream depth, from just a few centimeters to about 1 m28.
Response to predator cues in the natural habitat
We built three enclosures measuring 40 × 35 cm with a plastic mesh around a metal frame that limited two compartments, one measuring 35 × 35 cm, and another contiguous to it measuring 5 × 35 cm. Both compartments were 15 cm high and were open both below and above. We set the enclosures at a stream section of shallow water and flat rocky bottom, where water filled the enclosures up to about 5 cm (Fig. 4). We placed the enclosures with the small compartment upstream, so that the tadpole in the larger compartment would be exposed to both visual and chemical cues of the predator to be introduced in the smaller compartment. We collected three water bugs (Belostoma testaceopallidum) to be used in the experiments in a nearby stream from the same water basin (20o06′40″S, 43o28′48″W; 1,254 m above sea level). We collected the tadpoles very close to the enclosures (up to 5 m upstream).
Before we started each trial, we inspected the area covered by the enclosures to make sure no tadpoles or other animals remained inside. We then sealed the bottom carefully with sand from the same stream and placed one tadpole within each cage, in the larger compartment. We waited 3 min, sufficient for the tadpole to return to normal activity levels after translocation to the enclosures17, and then we recorded whether each tadpole was moving or standing still in 30 s intervals, during 15 min for a total of 30 observations. When tadpoles moved, they always moved on the bottom, never through the water column. After this, we waited another 3 min and repeated the 30 movement records for the next 15 min. We then removed the water bug and repeated another observation turn (waited 3 min, then made 30 movement observations separated by 30 s intervals). After each individual tadpole was tested, we released it downstream, to avoid using the same individual more than once. Tadpoles were all in developmental stage 2529 and measured 20.2 ± 2.4 mm (n = 33 tadpoles measured). We maintained the water bugs in individual recipients with clean stream water and used them randomly in the three enclosures. After all the experiments we collected them for identification.
We tested 3 tadpoles simultaneously, then restarted the whole experiment with other 3 tadpoles, and so on, until we tested 54 tadpoles during three consecutive days (3–5 October, 2018). The weather was sunny with some clouds and short periods of light rain, during which we did not conduct experiments.
Defensive responses based on previous experiences
For this experiment, we collected stage 25 tadpoles at the same stream section and kept them for no more than 2 h in polystyrene boxes with stream water by the nearby (about 1.3 km) lodging of the RPPN Santuário do Caraça, where we conducted the experiments to test the influence of previous experience on tadpole background choice and immobility. We placed individual tadpoles in plastic trays measuring 43 cm length, 30 cm width, 9 cm height, with half the bottom covered by a picture of a natural yellow background (rocks in its natural habitat). The other half was covered with the same picture manipulated digitally to match the hues and luminance of natural dark backgrounds, as in17. We filled the trays with tap water that comes straight from the main stream at the reserve, replacing the water at every trial. For each trial, we waited 3 min. after tadpole placement in the center of the tray, then we observed tadpoles for 30 min, recording their background every minute. After that, we applied one of three treatments during a 5-min interval: (1) an aversive stimulus was applied to the tadpole every time it positioned itself on the yellow background, or (2) on the dark background or (3) no stimulus was applied (control). The aversive stimulus consisted in one person approaching a wood stick slowly towards the tadpole until it reacted fleeing. After the treatments, we conducted another 30 min of observations recording tadpole background every minute. The tadpoles were all returned to their original stream after the experiments. We tested 2 tadpoles in each treatment simultaneously and then repeated the trials until tests of 30 tadpoles for each treatment (total 90 tadpoles) were completed. Experiments were performed from 4 to 6 October 2016. Experiments were conducted in the shade with natural light, and all days were sunny.
All the procedures were performed in accordance with relevant guidelines/regulations adopted by the responsible institutions: Sisbio/ICMBio (45302-1, 62316-1) authorized animal manipulation in situ and the Ethical Committee of the Pontifícia Universidade Católica de Minas Gerais (032/2016, 003/2018) approved the experimental procedures in accordance with animal welfare guidelines. The water bugs were identified as Belostoma testaceopallidum Latreille, 1807, and the collected specimens were deposited in the collection of aquatic insects of the Parasitology Department of the Institute of Biological Sciences (DPIC) of the Federal University of Minas Gerais, Belo Horizonte, Minas Gerais state, Brazil, under the accession number 9498.
Statistical analyses
We compared the level of activity of tadpoles (given by the number of instant positive records of movement) before, during, and after the presence of the water bug in the enclosures in the stream. We also evaluated a possible effect of direct sun incidence or shade on the enclosures30, and its interaction with tadpole activity levels. We built Generalized Linear Mixed Models (GLMM) with the packages “car”31 and “MASS”32 in R33. We considered number of movement records + 1 (to adjust to distributions that must be non-zero) as the dependent variable, phase (before, during, or after the presence of the water bug) and light (sun or shade) as explanatory variables, and individual tadpole as a random variable. Tadpoles might present different reactions to predators based on their previous experiences34. Considering individual as a random variable would also account for possible differences among times of the day and cages on individual behaviour. We built models including each one or both explanatory variables, with or without their interaction. We compared these models with a null model that included only the random variable, in order to identify the variable(s) with the strongest explanatory power.
We also used GLMMs to test for the ability of tadpoles to avoid a background colour after an aversive experience on it. Since tadpoles had to choose between dark and yellow, we arbitrarily used the number of records on dark backgrounds as dependent variable, because the records on yellow would represent the alternative situation (not on dark). We used treatment and phase (before and after the treatments were applied) as fixed variables and tadpole as a random variable. In order to test for the expression of immobility after the aversive stimuli, we considered the number of times tadpoles changed background colour in consecutive observations as a surrogate for tadpole movement (our dependent variable), treatment and phase as fixed variables and tadpole as a random variable.
We used the package MuMIn35 for R33 to select the best models, a procedure recommended to control the overall type I error rate36. We conducted Tukey post hoc tests with the package emmeans37.
References
Relyea, R. A. Morphological and behavioral plasticity of larval anurans in response to different predators. Ecology82, 523–540 (2001).
Cooper, W. E. Jr., Caldwell, J. P. & Vitt, L. J. Effective crypsis and its maintenance by immobility in Craugastor frogs. Copeia2008, 527–532 (2008).
Ioannou, C. C. & Krause, J. Interactions between background matching and motion during visual detection can explain why cryptic animals keep still. Biol. Lett.5, 191–193 (2009).
Hébert, M., Versace, E. & Vallortigara, G. Inexperienced preys know when to flee or to freeze in front of a threat. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A.46, 22918–22920 (2019).
Chivers, D. P., Mitchell, M. D., Lucon-Xiccato, T., Brown, G. E. & Ferrari, M. C. O. Background risk influences learning but not generalization of predators. Anim. Behav.121, 185–189 (2016).
Ferrari, M. C. O., Manek, A. K. & Chivers, D. P. Temporal learning of predation risk by embryonic amphibians. Biol. Lett.6, 308–310 (2010).
Mezraï, N., Arduini, L., Dickel, L., Chiao, C. C. & Darmaillacq, A. S. Awareness of danger inside the egg: Evidence of innate and learned predator recognition in cuttlefish embryos. Learn Behav. https://doi.org/10.3758/s-13420-020-00424-7 (2020).
Hossie, T., Landolt, K. & Murray, D. L. Determinants and co-expression of anti-predator responses in amphibian tadpoles: a meta-analysis. Oikos126, 173–184 (2017).
Preston, D. B. & Forstner, M. R. Houston Toad (Bufo (Anaxyrus) houstonensis) tadpoles decrease their activity in response to chemical cues produced from the predation of conspecifics and congeneric (Bufo (Incilius) nebulifer) tadpoles. J. Herpetol.49, 170–175 (2015).
Takahara, T. et al. Inducible defense behavior of an anuran tadpole: cue-detection range and cue types used against predator. Behav. Ecol.23, 863–868 (2012).
Hettyey, A., Rölli, F., Thürlimann, N., Zürcher, A. C. & Van Buskirk, J. Visual cues contribute to predator detection in anuran larvae. Biol. J. Linn. Soc.106, 820–827 (2012).
Polo-Cavia, N. & Gomez-Mestre, I. Learned recognition of introduced predators determines survival of tadpole prey. Funct. Ecol.28, 432–439 (2014).
Van Rheede, J. J., Richards, B. A. & Akerman, C. J. Sensory-evoked spiking behavior emerges via an experience-dependent plasticity mechanism. Neuron87, 1050–1062 (2015).
Dong, W. et al. Visual avoidance in Xenopus tadpoles is correlated with the maturation of visual responses in the optic tectum. J. Neurophysiol.101, 803–815 (2009).
Demas, J. A., Payne, H. & Cline, H. T. Vision drives correlated activity without patterned spontaneous activity in developing Xenopus retina. Dev. Neurobiol.72, 537–546. https://doi.org/10.1002/dneu.20880 (2012).
Hoff, K. V. S., Blaustein, A. R., McDiarmid, R. W. & Altig, R. Behavior Interactions and their consequences. In Tadpoles: the Biology of Anuran Larvae (eds McDiarmid, R. W. & Altig, R.) 215–239 (The University of Chicago press, Chicago, 1999).
Eterovick, P. C. et al. Tadpoles respond to background colour under threat. Sci. Rep.8, 4085. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-22315-8 (2018).
Melo, G. R., Solé, M. & Eterovick, P. C. Invisible or fearless: tadpole response to predator cues depends on color. Ethol. Ecol. Evol. (in press).
Underwood, A. J. Components of design in ecological field experiments. Ann. Zool. Fennici46, 93–111 (2009).
Rothman, G. R., Blackiston, D. J. & Levin, M. Color and intensity discrimination in Xenopus laevis tadpoles. Anim. Cogn.19, 911–919 (2016).
Mathis, A. & Unger, S. Learning to avoid dangerous habitat types by aquatic salamanders Eurycea tynerensis. Ethology118, 57–62 (2011).
Skelly, D. K. Activity level and the susceptibility of anuran larvae to predation. Anim. Behav.47, 465–468 (1994).
Casillas-Barragán, I., Costa-Pereira, R. & Peixoto, P. E. C. Perceived predation risk decreases movement and increases aggregation of Amazon milk frog (Anura, Hylidae) tadpoles throughout ontogeny. Hydrobiologia765, 379–386 (2016).
Bridges, C. M. Tadpoles balance foraging and predator avoidance: effects of predation, pond drying, and hunger. J. Herpetol.4, 627–634 (2002).
Mitchell, M. D., Chivers, D. P., Brown, G. E. & Ferrari, M. C. O. Living on the edge: how does environmental risk affect the behavioural and cognitive ecology of prey?. Anim. Behav.115, 185–192 (2016).
Viveros, R. S. Pteridófitas da Serra do Caraça, Minas Gerais, Brasil. Thesis, Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais, Belo Horizonte, Brazil (2010).
Strahler, A. N. Quantitative analysis of watershed morphology. Trans. Am. Geophys. Union38, 913–920 (1957).
Wachlevski, M., De Souza, P. H. C., Kopp, K. & Eterovick, P. Microhabitat use and feeding habits of Crossodactylus bokermanni Caramaschi and Sazima, 1985 (Anura, Hylodidae) at a site in south-eastern Brazil. J. Nat. Hist.42, 1421–1434 (2008).
Gosner, K. L. A simplified table for staging anuran embryos and larvae with notes on identification. Herpetologica16, 183–190 (1960).
Ding, G.-H., Lin, Z.-H., Zhao, L.-H., Fan, X.-L. & Wei, L. Effects of light intensity on activity in four sympatric anuran tadpoles. Zool. Res.35, 332–337 (2014).
Fox, J. & Weisberg, S. An R Companion to Applied Regression 2nd edn. (Sage, Thousand Oaks CA, 2011).
Venables, W. N. & Ripley, B. D. Modern Applied Statistics with S 4th edn. (Springer, New York, 2002).
R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Version 3.6.3 (2020). https://cran.r-project.org/.
Teplitsky, C. et al. Escape behaviour and ultimate causes of specific induced defences in an anuran tadpole. J. Evol. Biol.18, 180–190 (2005).
Bartoń, K. MuMIn: Multi-Model Inference. R package version 1.43.6. (2019) https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=MuMIn.
Hothorn, T., Bretz, F. & Westfall, P. Simultaneous inference in general parametric models. Biom. J.50, 346–363. https://doi.org/10.1002/bimj.200810425 (2008).
Russell, L. emmeans: Estimated Marginal Means, aka Least-Squares Means. R package version 1.4.5 (2020). https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=emmeans.
Acknowledgements
We are thankful to Dr. Alan Lane de Melo for water bug identification, the Pontifícia Universidade Católica de Minas Gerais (PUC Minas) and the Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais (UFMG) for logistic support, the infra-structure staff of PUC Minas for confection of experimental enclosures, the Conselho Nacional de Desenvolvimento Científico e Tecnológico (CNPq) for a Research Productivity Grant provided to P. C. Eterovick (310162/2017-3), the Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior (Capes), the Fundação de Amparo à Pesquisa do Estado de Minas Gerais (Fapemig), and the CNPq for finantial support. We also acknowledge support by the German Research Foundation and the Open Access Publication Funds of the Technische Universität Braunschweig.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
P.C.E. planned and supervised the experiment, conducted statistical analyses and leaded manuscript writing, P.I.M.V., M.G., D.T.M., M.B., I.M.M., L.T.P., R.P.Q., T.K.F.M., R.A.M., C.M.C., V.M., and A.L.O. participated in field work and manuscript writing, J.S.K., C.C.F., and M.V. participated in data analyses and interpretation and contributed to discussions and manuscript writing.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Eterovick, P.C., Kloh, J.S., Figueredo, C.C. et al. Background choice and immobility as context dependent tadpole responses to perceived predation risk. Sci Rep 10, 13577 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-70274-w
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-70274-w
This article is cited by
-
Growing a fin: wetland and upland effects on tadpole morphology of Scinax squalirostris (Anura: Hylidae)
Zoomorphology (2022)
-
Detectability is in the eye of the beholder—the role of UV reflectance on tadpole detection and predation by a passerine bird
Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology (2021)
-
Effects of hydroperiod on morphology of tadpoles from highland ponds
Aquatic Ecology (2020)
Comments
By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.