Abstract
Sleep deprivation significantly impairs a range of cognitive and brain function, particularly episodic memory and the underlying hippocampal function. However, it remains controversial whether one or two nights of recovery sleep following sleep deprivation fully restores brain and cognitive function. In this study, we used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and examined the effects of two consecutive nights (20-hour time-in-bed) of recovery sleep on resting-state hippocampal connectivity and episodic memory deficits following one night of total sleep deprivation (TSD) in 39 healthy adults in a controlled in-laboratory protocol. TSD significantly reduced memory performance in a scene recognition task, impaired hippocampal connectivity to multiple prefrontal and default mode network regions, and disrupted the relationships between memory performance and hippocampal connectivity. Following TSD, two nights of recovery sleep restored hippocampal connectivity to baseline levels, but did not fully restore memory performance nor its associations with hippocampal connectivity. These findings suggest that more than two nights of recovery sleep are needed to fully restore memory function and hippocampal-memory associations after one night of total sleep loss.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
Insufficient sleep is a widespread problem in contemporary societies. Millions of people sleep less than 7 hours per night, which is the minimum sleep duration to prevent cumulative deterioration in neurobehavioral performance1,2,3,4,5. Sleep loss destabilizes the wake state, impairs cognition and behavior, increases the risk for multiple diseases, and incurs considerable social, financial, and health-related costs6,7,8,9.
Although the exact function of sleep remains to be elucidated, it is well known that sleep facilitates memory retention10,11,12,13, whereas sleep deprivation degrades memory performance and brain function14,15,16,17,18,19,20,21,22,23. Animal studies have consistently demonstrated the impairing effects of sleep loss on several neural circuits involved in learning and memory, in particular the hippocampal complex16,17,18. Accumulating evidence also suggests that newly encoded information temporarily stored in the hippocampus is reactivated during sleep and integrated with existing long-term memories stored in the neocortex24,25,26. In humans, sleep deprivation attenuates hippocampal function at rest as well as during memory encoding, consolidation, and recognition tasks14,15,22,23,27. A growing body of research suggests that functional connectivity between the hippocampus, prefrontal cortex, and default mode network (DMN) regions may play critical roles in memory function28,29,30,31,32, which are disrupted by sleep deprivation15,20,22,23.
Although the detrimental effects of sleep loss on memory and brain function are well established, whether and how recovery sleep restores memory and brain function after sleep loss are not well understood. To date, only a limited number of studies have examined the restoring effects of recovery sleep on cognitive and brain function. For example, an animal study suggested that several hours of recovery sleep restored hippocampal synaptic plasticity and connectivity in mice after brief sleep deprivation33. In humans, some studies have indicated that one or two nights of recovery sleep might be able to restore cognitive performance and brain adenosinergic system to baseline levels34,35,36,37,38,39,40, while others have suggested that neurobehavioral deficits, self-monitoring abilities, and brain metabolic decreases after total or chronic sleep loss might not be fully recovered by one or two nights of recovery sleep1,3,41,42,43,44,45,46. A recent study found that a 90-min recovery nap restored hippocampus-dependent learning during the day of sleep deprivation, and the structural morphology of hippocampal subfields predicted the success of learning restoration47. However, it remains unclear about how much amount of recovery sleep people would need to fully restore both cognitive and brain function following sleep deprivation.
The present study used resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and aimed to examine both the impairing effects of one night of total sleep deprivation (TSD) and the restorative effects of two consecutive nights (12-hours and 8-hours time-in-bed) of recovery sleep following TSD on episodic memory and hippocampal functional connectivity in a well-controlled 5-day and 4-night in-laboratory study (see Fig. 1). We also studied an additional cohort of control participants using the same protocol but without sleep loss to exclude the potential confounding effects of in-laboratory experimental procedures on brain connectivity and memory performance. While a number of literature has demonstrated the alteration of brain connectivity in different resting-state networks after sleep deprivation20,21,22,23,93, we focused on the effects of recovery sleep following sleep deprivation on hippocampal functional connectivity and episodic memory performance in this study. Based on previous literature, we hypothesized that one night of acute TSD would significantly disrupt episodic memory performance (H1a) and the underlying hippocampal connectivity (H1b) as compared to baseline levels, whereas two nights of recovery sleep after one night of acute TSD would restore both episodic memory performance (H2a) and hippocampal connectivity (H2b) to baseline levels.
Results
We first compared behavioral performance on the episodic memory task before and after TSD to determine the effects of one night of acute TSD on memory function. As shown in Fig. 2, memory performance was significantly impaired after TSD (day 3) when compared to baseline (day 2), including significantly decreased hit rate (p < 0.05), increased false alarm (p < 0.01), and reduced d-prime (p < 0.001), while no differences were found in the memory performance between the corresponding day 2 and day 3 in the control subjects (all p > 0.05). These findings support our hypothesis (H1a) that one night of acute TSD would disrupt episodic memory function. We then compared memory performance at baseline (day 2) and after recovery sleep (day 5) to examine whether two nights of recovery sleep could fully restore episodic memory function. Contradicting with our initial hypothesis H2a, memory performance was still significantly impaired in hit rate (p < 0.05), false alarm (p < 0.05), and d-prime (p < 0.001), suggesting that participants may need more than two nights of recovery sleep to restore memory function after one night of sleep loss.
For brain imaging data, we compared resting-state functional connectivity patterns before and after TSD to determine the effects of one night of acute TSD on hippocampal connectivity. As shown in Fig. 3a and listed in Table 1, hippocampal connectivity was significantly reduced in multiple DMN and prefrontal regions, including the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), medial superior frontal gyrus (mSFG), right ventromedial prefrontal cortex (R.vmPFC), and right precentral gyrus (R.PreCG), and was significantly increased in bilateral cerebellum and right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (R.dlPFC) after one night of TSD when compared to baseline condition. All of these brain regions survived whole brain family-wise error rate (FWE) corrected p < 0.01. These regions were defined as the regions of interest (ROIs) for further analyses to determine the restoration effects of two nights of recovery sleep on hippocampal connectivity. In contrast, no brain connectivity differences were found in the control group (see Fig. 3b). These findings support our hypothesis (H1b) that one night of acute TSD would significantly disrupt hippocampal connectivity.
The results from the ROI analyses are shown in Fig. 4. When comparing hippocampal connectivity at baseline (day 2) and after recovery sleep (day 5), no differences were observed in the TSD group (all p > 0.05). These findings support our hypothesis (H2b) and suggest that two nights of recovery sleep would restore hippocampal connectivity to baseline levels after one night of sleep loss. Moreover, the ROI analyses also demonstrated that the increased connectivity in the R.dlPFC from the whole-brain analysis was due to reduced negative connectivity (anti-correlations) between the hippocampus and this region (p < 0.001), and the increased connectivity in the cerebellum was due to reversed connectivity (from negative to positive correlations) to the hippocampus during TSD (day 3) as compared to baseline (day 2). No connectivity differences were found among any scans from the three corresponding days in the control subjects (all p > 0.05).
We also examined the relationships between hippocampal connectivity and episodic memory performance (d-prime) separately during each scan day (day 2, day 3, and day 5, see Fig. 5) using correlation analyses and non-parametric permutation tests. A significant positive correlation was observed between resting-state hippocampal connectivity and memory performance (d-prime) in the R.vmPFC (Fig. 5a, Pearson r = 0.419, p = 0.001; Spearman rho = 0.336, p = 0.036; permutation test p = 0.003). However, this correlation was disrupted during TSD (Fig. 5b, Pearson r = 0.208, p = 0.20; Spearman rho = 0.144, p = 0.38; permutation test p = 0.11) and following recovery sleep (Fig. 5c, Pearson r = 0.176, p = 0.28; Spearman rho = 0.126, p = 0.26; permutation test p = 0.13). Although the comparisons of correlation coefficients did not reach significant differences between conditions, these findings suggest that TSD impaired the associations between hippocampal connectivity and episodic memory performance and such impairments might not be fully restored after two nights of recovery sleep.
Discussion
To our knowledge, this study demonstrated for the first time both the detrimental and restoring changes in resting-state hippocampal connectivity and episodic memory after one night of acute total sleep deprivation and following two consecutive nights of recovery sleep. As expected, we found significantly impaired connectivity between hippocampus and multiple brain regions after TSD, which is in line with previous memory and sleep deprivation studies14,15,20,22,23,38,47,48,49. However, we also found that two nights of recovery sleep following TSD did not restore episodic memory performance to baseline, even though hippocampal connectivity was restored to baseline levels. These findings only partly support our hypothesis concerning the restoring effects of recovery sleep but are nevertheless consistent with several previous studies showing insufficient restoration of cognitive performance and/or brain function after one or two nights of recovery sleep following sleep loss1,3,41,42,43,44,45. In addition, the associations between hippocampal connectivity and episodic memory performance were also disrupted during TSD and did not fully restore after two nights of recovery sleep, suggesting that intact hippocampal functional connectivity may not be sufficient to support normal episodic memory function after two nights of recovery from sleep deprivation.
Numerous previous studies have undoubtedly demonstrated the impacts of sleep loss on multiple neurobehavioral domains, particularly attention, learning, and memory7,8,10,11,21,41,50,51. Impaired neurobehavioral performance includes slowed response times, concentration lapses, increased errors of commission and omission, and mitigated learning ability and memory consolidation. For example, using a picture recognition memory task, Yoo and colleagues15 found that a single night of total sleep deprivation produced significant deficits in hippocampus-dependent episodic memory encoding and subsequent memory retention, while Sterpenich and colleagues14 showed that one night of total sleep deprivation impaired recollection of positive and neutral pictures (but not negative pictures). Moreover, Van Der Werf and colleagues27 reported that even a mild sleep disruption was sufficient to affect hippocampal-dependent memory encoding and performance. Using a free recall test of word lists, de Almeida and colleagues38 reported that both episodic memory and short-term memory were significantly spared after two nights of sleep deprivation. In our study, we found that one night of TSD significantly reduced hit rate, increased false alarm, and reduced d-prime, which are consistent with previous studies and further support the key role of sleep in hippocampus-dependent episodic memory function. Memory performance did not show any significant changes across three scan days in the control protocol without sleep deprivation, suggesting that hindered memory performance was specific to sleep loss and not due to other aspects of the experimental protocol that the control participants also experienced.
TSD altered functional connectivity between the hippocampus and multiple DMN and prefrontal regions, including the PCC, vmPFC, and mSFG. These findings replicate previous studies showing that TSD impaired hippocampal connectivity at rest and/or during memory tasks14,15,20,22,23. Both the PCC and vmPFC are the core nodes of DMN, which is thought to be involved in autobiographical and episodic memory processing52,53,54. Enhanced connectivity between the hippocampus and DMN has been linked to successful memory formation in normal individuals55,56 while reduced connectivity has been observed in patients with episodic memory deficits57,58. However, we did not find increased hippocampal connectivity to subcortical regions after TSD as reported in several previous studies15,22. The discrepancy may be due to differences in the experimental design, study protocol, characteristics of participants, as well as data acquisition parameters and analysis pipelines. For example, the study by Yoo et al.15 employed a between-subject design and compared hippocampal connectivity between a cohort of sleep-deprived subjects and another independent cohort of non-sleep-deprived controls during the performance of a memory encoding task. In contrast, both the current study and the study by Chengyang et al.22 employed a within-subject design and compared resting-state hippocampal connectivity in the same cohort of healthy adults at rested wakefulness and after one night of sleep deprivation. The Chengyang et al. study22 included two scans from 20 healthy young male adults (age range 18–24 years) while the current study included three scans from 70 male and female healthy adults with a much larger age range (21–50 years). In addition, all subjects in the current study were required to stay in the laboratory across 5 days and 4 nights and carefully monitored for their activities and food intake, while subjects in previous studies were monitored in the laboratory during the TSD period. However, it remains unknown how the laboratory environment and specific activities would affect hippocampal connectivity and memory function.
We also found that TSD significantly increased hippocampal connectivity to the right dlPFC, a key node of the task-positive dorsal attention network (DAN). However, ROI analyses indicated that such increased connectivity was actually due to reduced anti-correlation between the dlPFC and the hippocampus. Since the hippocampus is a core region of the DMN54,59,60, this finding is consistent with the literature demonstrating reduced anti-correlations between the DMN and DAN after sleep loss20,61,62,63,64,65. During rested wakefulness after normal sleep, the task-negative DMN is usually activated whereas the task-positive DAN is suppressed at rest in the absence of task requirements, suggesting a functional decoupling between these two networks66,67. The reduction of anti-correlation during TSD suggests that task-negative and task-positive networks may fail to remain functionally distinct from each other after sleep loss, which may lead to an altered allocation of cognitive resources to brain networks and poor modulation of attention processes in response to shifting cognitive demands21,68,69.
One night of TSD not only impaired resting-state hippocampal connectivity, but also disrupted the associations between hippocampal connectivity and episodic memory performance. Specifically, resting-state hippocampal connectivity to prefrontal regions (R.vmPFC) correlated with episodic memory performance following baseline sleep, but not during sleep deprivation or after recovery sleep. These findings are consistent with the view that hippocampus-prefrontal communications are important for optimal redistribution of temporal memory traces to more permanent cortical storage, and that disruptions of such communications may compromise the cognitive capacity for committing new experiences to memory15.
Consistent with our hypothesis, we found that disrupted hippocampal connectivity after TSD returned to baseline levels after two consecutive nights (20 hours of time-in-bed) of recovery sleep. However, we did not observe full restoration of memory performance and its associations with hippocampal connectivity to baseline levels after two consecutive nights of recovery sleep. These findings on memory restoration failure appear to contradict several prior studies where it was suggested that one night of recovery sleep restored cognitive performance34,35,36,38,39,70 to baseline levels. Specifically, a previous study reported that a 90-min recovery nap restored hippocampus-dependent learning ability and memory function in a mnemonic similarity task during the day of acute total sleep deprivation47. However, the failure of memory restoration after two nights of recovery sleep is in line with many other previous studies where it was indicated that more than one night of recovery sleep was needed to fully restore cognitive performance1,3,37,41,43,44,45,46 and brain metabolic changes42 after total or chronic sleep loss. For example, Dinges and colleagues41 reported that one night of recovery sleep was not sufficient to reverse cognitive deficits resulting from chronic sleep restriction. Similarly, Belenky and colleagues1 showed that three nights of recovery sleep were not able to fully restore performance in the psychomotor vigilance task (PVT) after seven nights of chronic sleep restriction. Pejovic and colleagues44 showed that PVT performance deficits persisted after two nights of recovery sleep following six nights of chronic sleep restriction (6-h). Lo and colleagues45 also reported that sustained attention performance did not return to baseline levels after two nights of recovery sleep following seven nights of chronic sleep restriction (5-h). Taken together, our findings provide further evidence supporting that two nights of recovery sleep are insufficient to fully restore cognitive and brain function from one night of TSD.
There are several limitations in our study. First, this study only involves overnight total sleep deprivation, thus we could not determine the specific contribution of different types of sleep, such as slow wave sleep (SWS) or rapid eye movement (REM) sleep to the detrimental effects of sleep loss on hippocampal connectivity and episodic memory. Moreover, we were not able to examine the sleep stage changes during the two night of recovery sleep. However, the dual-process hypothesis suggests that SWS is more important for declarative memory (including episodic memory and semantic memory) whereas REM sleep is essential for procedural memory71,72,73,74,75. Increased functional coupling between the hippocampus and neocortical regions has been found in stage-2 sleep76, while decreased coupling between the hippocampus and PCC has been found during SWS compared with the waking state77. Furthermore, increases in hippocampal connectivity correlate with the amount of prior SWS78. These findings suggest that different types of memories may occur during different sleep stages. Future studies should replicate the current findings and determine the potential different roles of stage-2 sleep, SWS, and REM sleep in maintaining and restoring brain network integrity and memory function.
Second, although both acute TSD and chronic sleep restriction can induce similar impairments in cognitive performance, some studies suggest that the recovery following acute TSD may be faster than that following chronic sleep restriction3,34,70. Therefore, acute TSD and chronic sleep restriction might have somewhat different physiological processes1,79,80. The present study focuses on the effects of acute TSD and their restoration, thus our findings cannot be generalized to the effects of chronic partial sleep restriction on brain function and its associations with memory performance. Future studies are needed to determine whether acute total sleep deprivation and chronic partial sleep restriction and their recovery would share the same or separable neural mechanisms, and whether neural recovery would occur across the same time period or over a different temporal duration following different forms and doses of sleep deprivation21.
Third, we only used fMRI to measure resting-state hippocampal connectivity and did not image memory task in the scanner at the same time. Previous task-related fMRI studies have reported significant effects of sleep deprivation and recovery sleep on memory performance and hippocampal function14,15,27,81. However, because sleep deprivation simultaneously impairs brain function and memory performance, it is difficult for task-related fMRI studies to dissociate the effects of sleep loss on brain function per se from the effects of sleep loss on task performance that subsequently affects brain function. Moreover, the use of resting-state fMRI may eliminate confounds of differences in task performance, effort, practice, task strategy82,83, and reduce the potential effects of fatigue on brain connectivity. Of note, sleep deprived participants may easily become fatigued when performing tasks, therefore, the use of resting-state fMRI outperforms the use of task-based fMRI with respect to sleep deprivation studies. Nonetheless, future studies may need to combine both resting-state and task-related fMRI to more comprehensively understand the impairing effects of sleep deprivation and the restoring effects of recovery sleep on brain function and memory performance.
Finally, the final analyses only included 39 subjects with an age range between 21 and 50 years. Although this sample size is not small in the sleep deprivation literature, we were not able to find significant differences in the correlation coefficients between memory performance (d-prime) and the hippocampal connectivity across baseline, TSD, and recovery sleep conditions. Thus, our results of the disrupted relationships between memory performance and hippocampal connectivity after TSD and recovery sleep need to be regarded as preliminary and future studies with a larger sample size are needed to validate our findings. In addition, because memory degradation is highly prevalent in elderly individuals, future studies are necessary to examine how sleep loss and recovery sleep impact memory function in this vulnerable population. However, our findings may have important implications for understanding memory dysfunction in aging and neurodegenerative diseases. For example, hippocampal connectivity measured by resting-state fMRI may offer a noninvasive approach for monitoring the effects of sleep loss on the episodic memory system in the human brain. Disturbed hippocampal connectivity has been consistently observed in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s Disease (AD), for which episodic memory degradation is a hallmark symptom57,58. Both animal and human studies have demonstrated that sleep loss exacerbates amyloid-beta (Aβ) plaque aggregation in the brain, a fundamental process leading to AD84,85,86,87. Moreover, a very recent study suggests that quantitative and qualitative features of human sleep may represent non-invasive biomarkers of AD pathology88. Since reduced sleep duration and increased sleep disturbances are concomitant symptoms of aging and AD89,90,91,92, disruption in hippocampal connectivity and its associations with memory performance may provide a plausible mechanism for the deleterious effects of sleep loss in this population.
In summary, here we used resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging in a well controlled in-laboratory sleep deprivation protocol and demonstrated both the impairing effects of one night of acute total sleep deprivation and the restoring effects of two consecutive nights of recovery sleep on hippocampal connectivity and episodic memory performance. We found that sleep deprivation significantly impaired hippocampal connectivity to multiple prefrontal and default mode network regions, reduced episodic memory performance, and disrupted the associations between them. Although hippocampal connectivity was fully restored to baseline levels after two nights of recovery sleep, episodic memory performance and its associations with hippocampal connectivity did not return to baseline. These findings support the critical role of sleep in maintaining the integrity of hippocampal circuits and suggest that people may need more than two nights of recovery sleep to fully restore both memory performance and their associations with brain function after one night of total sleep loss. Optimization of sleep time may represent a potential effective intervention for the deleterious effects of sleep loss on memory and hippocampal function, with therapeutic opportunities for aging and AD.
Methods
Participants
A total of 70 healthy adults (age range 21–50 years, 41 males) participated in a 5-day and 4-night in-laboratory controlled sleep deprivation experiment93,94, including 54 adults in the experimental group with one night of TSD (Fig. 1a) and 16 adults in the control group without sleep loss (Fig. 1b). Fifteen participants in the TSD group and one participant in the control group were excluded due to head motion, MR hardware problem, falling asleep during the scans, and/or missing behavioral data. Thus, fifty-four participants (age range 21–50 yrs, 28 males) were included in the present study, including 39 in the TSD group (mean age = 33.5 ± 8.8 yrs, 22 males) and 15 in the control group (mean age = 34.5 ± 9.0 yrs, 7 males). The sleep-wake schedule of each participant was assessed by at least one week actigraphy, sleep-wake diaries, sleep and circadian rhythm questionnaires, and a night of laboratory polysomnography and oximetry measurements during the weeks preceding the study. All participants had a regular sleep-wake schedule of nocturnal sleep duration between 6.5 and 8.5 hours, regular bedtime between 2200–0000 h and wake time of 0600–0900 h, and no evidence of habitual napping or sleep disorders. Participants could not have participated in trans-meridian travel or shift work, nor had irregular sleep-wake routines for 60 days prior to the in-lab study. Caffeine, alcohol, tobacco, medications (except 4 subjects took oral contraceptives and one subject took Mirena for birth control) or other psychoactive substances were prohibited from the week prior to and during the in-laboratory study. All participants were right-handed, not current smokers, and had no history of chronic physical or mental illness, as established by interviews, questionnaires, physical examinations, and blood and urine tests. This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the University of Pennsylvania and was carried out in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. All participants provided written informed consent before enrollment and were compensated for participating in the study.
In-laboratory study protocol
The in-laboratory study lasted 5 consecutive days and 4 nights (see Fig. 1a for the study protocol). All participants stayed in a laboratory room of the Clinical Translational Research Center at the Hospital of the University of Pennsylvania. Participants arrived at the laboratory on the afternoon of day 1 and were provided 9–10 hours time-in-bed (TIB) sleep opportunity for baseline sleep on night 1. The first fMRI scan session took place on the morning of day 2 (0700–1000 hours). Participants were then randomized to either a TSD or a control condition. On night 2, participants in the SD group were kept awake and then underwent the second fMRI scan on the morning of day 3 (0700–1000 hours). On nights 3 and 4, these participants received 12 and 8 hours recovery sleep, respectively. We used two consecutive nights of recovery sleep because previous studies have suggested that neurobehavioral and brain activity deficits after sleep loss may not be fully recovered after one night of sleep3,42. The third fMRI scan took place on the morning of day 5 (0700–1000 hours). Participants in the control group (Fig. 1b) also had the same three fMRI scans on the mornings of days 2, 3, and 5, but they were provided 8 hours TIB sleep on nights 2 to 4. Because previous studies have suggested that time-of-day may modulate brain activity and memory performance95,96,97,98,99,100,101,102,103,104, the time of fMRI scans and cognitive tests were fixed for all subjects and kept constant across the whole protocol to minimize the potential confounding time-of-day effects.
Throughout the 5-day protocol, participants were continuously behaviorally monitored by trained staff in a semi-isolated living area. They were provided meals at regular, pre-specified hours, as well as snacks within reasonable limits. When not testing during the study, they were permitted to engage in non-strenuous activities such as reading and watching television. They were not allowed to do any physically demanding tasks, work on laptops, or leave the study environment.
During the afternoons (1400–1800 hours) of days 2 to 5, all participants completed a scene encoding and recognition task to measure episodic memory function after SD. For the episodic memory task, participants were asked to view 90 or 150 color pictures during the encoding session and told to remember them for later recognition testing. Each day participants learned a different set of picture stimuli consisting of scenes, landscapes, objects, and non-famous people, which were pseudo-randomly selected from the International Affective Pictures System (IAPS)105 and online, The number of pictures was kept the same across different days. Pictures with extreme emotional valence were excluded. The pictures were matched in terms of overall visual complexity, brightness, contrast, and emotional valence. Each picture was displayed on the computer screen for 1 second with 0.5 second inter-stimulus interval. A few minutes after the encoding task, participants performed the recognition tests, in which they viewed 30 pictures from the encoding session mixed with 60 new pictures as foils. Participants were required to make a forced-choice response as to whether they remembered the picture from the encoding session (‘old’) or believed they had not viewed the picture before (‘new’). There were four types of responses: (i) old pictures correctly recognized as old (‘hits’); (ii) new stimuli correctly recognized as new (‘correct rejections’); (iii) old pictures incorrectly judged to be new (‘misses’); and (iv) new pictures incorrectly judged to be old (‘false alarms’). Task performance was calculated on the basis of hit rate [hits/(hits + misses)], false alarm (FA) rate [false alarms/(false alarms + correct rejections)], and signal detection accuracy d-prime (d’). According to the signal detection theory, d-prime is calculated as the normalized distance between the probability distributions of signal and noise and noise alone106. Greater d-prime indicates participants have better performance to discriminate target from nontarget therefore a better memory in the task. Differences in episodic memory performance across the four consecutive days (days 2–5) were analyzed using repeated measures one-way analyses of variance (ANOVA).
Imaging data acquisition and analysis
All MRI scans were performed using a 3 T Siemens Trio system (Siemens Medical Systems, Erlangen, Germany). A multiband gradient-echo EPI sequence was used for resting-state BOLD fMRI data acquisition with the following parameters: TR = 2 s, TE = 24 ms, FOV = 220 × 220 mm, matrix = 64 × 64 × 36, slices thickness = 4 mm, 36 interleaved slices without gap. A total of 210 images were acquired from about 7 min scanning for each participant. Participants were instructed to remain still in the scanner, keep their eyes open and look at a cross fixation on the center of a screen. An eye-tracking camera was used to monitor participants’ eyes to confirm that they did not fall asleep during the scan. After the functional scans, high-resolution (1 × 1 × 1 mm3) T1-weighted anatomic images were obtained using a standard 3D MPRAGE sequence for structural reference.
Image data processing and analyses were carried out with the Statistical Parametric Mapping software (SPM 12, Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, UK) and the REST 2.0 toolbox (http://resting-fmri.sourceforge.net/), implemented in Matlab 14 (Math Works, Natick, MA). The 3 translational/rotational motion parameters and the framewise displacement (FD) were calculated. To control for the effect of head motion, the threshold of head motion parameters were set as 2 for translational/rotational motion and 0.5 for mean FD. Data from subjects whose head motion parameters were over the threshold were discarded. After head motion correction, scrubbing, and co-registration, functional images were smoothed using an isotropic Gaussian kernel with a full-width at half-maximum (FWHM) of 4 mm and then normalized to the standard Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space. Linear trends were also removed. All functional volumes were finally band pass filtered at 0.01–0.08 Hz to reduce low-frequency drift and physiological high-frequency respiratory and cardiac noise. Nuisance covariates including six head motion parameters, global mean signal, white matter signal and CSF signal were regressed out before the seed-based functional connectivity (FC) analysis107.
The hippocampal seed was defined as the bilateral hippocampus from an automated anatomical labeling region of interest library108. The Pearson’s correlation coefficients between the mean BOLD fMRI signal time series of the seed region and all other voxels within the brain were calculated, generating a correlation map for each participant. Fisher’s r-to-z transformation {z = 0.5 Ln [(1 + r)/(1 − r)]} was then applied on these correlation maps to improve the normality of the correlation coefficients. A whole-brain voxel-wise group-level paired t-test was performed on these z-transformed correlation maps to compare resting-state brain connectivity patterns before and after TSD and determine the effects of one night of acute TSD on hippocampal connectivity. The threshold was defined as whole brain family-wise error (FWE) corrected p < 0.05. The regions showing significant changes in connectivity to hippocampus were then defined as the regions of interest (ROIs) to determine the restoration effects of two nights of recovery sleep on hippocampal connectivity. Spearman and Pearson correlation analyses, as well as permutation tests were performed to examine the relationships between hippocampal connectivity and episodic memory performance (d-prime) separately during each scan day using SPSS Statistics for Windows (Version 18.0, IBM, Chicago, IL). Specifically, we used a permutation testing procedure to test the significance of the Pearson correlation: (i) the observed Pearson correlation was computed between the hippocampus-R.vmPFC connectivity and the d-prime; (ii) the values of d-prime were permuted relative to the connectivity values; (iii) Pearson correlation was then re-computed after each permutation; (iv) (i) and (ii) were repeated 10,000 times to build a null distribution of Pearson correlations for comparison with the observed Pearson correlation. Moreover, we used a correlation comparison method suggested by Diedenhofen and Musch109 to evaluate whether correlation coefficients between memory performance (d-prime) and the hippocampal connectivity were statistically different across baseline, TSD, and recovery sleep conditions.
References
Belenky, G. et al. Patterns of performance degradation and restoration during sleep restriction and subsequent recovery: a sleep dose-response study. J. Sleep Res. 12, 1–12 (2003).
Van Dongen, H. P. A., Maislin, G., Mullington, J. M. & Dinges, D. F. The Cumulative Cost of Additional Wakefulness: Dose-Response Effects on Neurobehavioral Functions and Sleep Physiology From Chronic Sleep Restriction and Total Sleep Deprivation. Sleep 26, 117–126 (2003).
Banks, S., Van Dongen, H. P. A., Maislin, G. & Dinges, D. F. Neurobehavioral dynamics following chronic sleep restriction: Dose-response effects of one night for recovery. Sleep 33, 1013–1026 (2010).
Ford, E. S., Cunningham, T. J. & Croft, J. B. Trends in Self-Reported Sleep Duration among US Adults from 1985 to 2012. Sleep 38, 829–832 (2015).
Watson, N. F. et al. Recommended Amount of Sleep for a Healthy Adult: A Joint Consensus Statement of the American Academy of Sleep Medicine and Sleep Research Society. Sleep 38, 843–844 (2015).
Banks, S. & Dinges, D. F. Behavioral and Physiological Consequences of Sleep Restriction. J. Clin. Sleep Med. 3, 519–528 (2007).
Lim, J. & Dinges, D. F. Sleep deprivation and vigilant attention. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1129, 305–322 (2008).
Goel, N., Rao, H., Durmer, J. S. & Dinges, D. F. Neurocognitive Consequences of Sleep Deprivation. Semin Neurol. 29, 320–339 (2009).
Spaeth, A. M., Dinges, D. F. & Goel, N. Effects of Experimental Sleep Restriction on Weight Gain, Caloric Intake, and Meal Timing in Healthy Adults. Sleep 36, 981–990 (2013).
Walker, M. P. & Stickgold, R. Sleep-dependent learning and memory consolidation. Neuron 44, 121–133 (2004).
Diekelmann, S. & Born, J. The memory function of sleep. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 114–126 (2010).
Inostroza, M. & Born, J. Sleep for Preserving and Transforming Episodic Memory. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 36, 79–102 (2013).
Fernandes, C. et al. Detrimental role of prolonged sleep deprivation on adult neurogenesis. Front. Cell. Neurosci. 9, 140 (2015).
Sterpenich, V. et al. Sleep-related hippocampo-cortical interplay during emotional memory recollection. PLoS Biol. 5, e282 (2007).
Yoo, S. S., Hu, P. T., Gujar, N., Jolesz, F. A. & Walker, M. P. A deficit in the ability to form new human memories without sleep. Nat. Neurosci. 10, 385–392 (2007).
Havekes, R., Vecsey, C. G. & Abel, T. The impact of sleep deprivation on neuronal and glial signaling pathways important for memory and synaptic plasticity. Cell. Signal. 24, 1251–1260 (2012).
Havekes, R. et al. Sleep deprivation causes memory deficits by negatively impacting neuronal connectivity in hippocampal area CA1. Elife 5, e13424 (2016).
Abel, T., Havekes, R., Saletin, J. M. & Walker, M. P. Sleep, plasticity and memory from molecules to whole-brain networks. Curr. Biol. 23, R774–R788 (2013).
Menz, M. M. et al. The role of sleep and sleep deprivation in consolidating fear memories. Neuroimage 75, 87–96 (2013).
Yeo, B. T. T., Tandi, J. & Chee, M. W. L. Functional connectivity during rested wakefulness predicts vulnerability to sleep deprivation. Neuroimage 111, 147–158 (2015).
Krause, A. J. et al. The sleep-deprived human brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 18, 404–418 (2017).
Chengyang, L. et al. Short-term memory deficits correlate with hippocampal-thalamic functional connectivity alterations following acute sleep restriction. Brain Imaging Behav. 11, 954–963 (2017).
Zhao, R. et al. Disrupted Resting-State Functional Connectivity in Hippocampal Subregions After Sleep Deprivation. Neuroscience 398, 37–54 (2019).
Sirota, A., Csicsvari, J., Buhl, D. & Buzsa´ki, G. Communication between neocortex and hippocampus during sleep in rodents. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 100, 2065–2069 (2003).
Ji, D. & Wilson, M. A. Coordinated memory replay in the visual cortex and hippocampus during sleep. Nat. Neurosci. 10, 100–107 (2007).
Logothetis, N. K. et al. Hippocampal-cortical interaction during periods of subcortical silence. Nature 491, 547–553 (2012).
Van Der Werf, Y. D. et al. Sleep benefits subsequent hippocampal functioning. Nat. Neurosci. 12, 122–123 (2009).
Marshall, L. & Born, J. The contribution of sleep to hippocampus-dependent memory consolidation. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 442–450 (2007).
Schlichting, M. L. & Preston, A. R. Memory reactivation during rest supports upcoming learning of related content. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 111, 15845–15850 (2014).
Voets, N. L. et al. Aberrant Functional Connectivity in Dissociable Hippocampal Networks Is Associated with Deficits in Memory. J. Neurosci. 34, 4920–4928 (2014).
Cooper, R. A. et al. Reduced Hippocampal Functional Connectivity During Episodic Memory Retrieval in Autism. Cereb. cortex 27, 888–902 (2017).
Tompary, A. & Davachi, L. Consolidation Promotes the Emergence of Representational Overlap in the Hippocampus and Medial Prefrontal Cortex. Neuron 96, 228–241 (2017).
Vecsey, C. G. et al. Sleep deprivation impairs cAMP signalling in the hippocampus. Nature 461, 1122–1125 (2009).
Drummond, S. P. A., Paulus, M. P. & Tapert, S. F. Effects of two nights sleep deprivation and two nights recovery sleep on response inhibition. J. Sleep Res. 15, 261–265 (2006).
Jay, S. M. et al. The characteristics of recovery sleep when recovery opportunity is restricted. Sleep 30, 353–360 (2007).
Mander, B. A. et al. EEG measures index neural and cognitive recovery from sleep deprivation. J. Neurosci. 30, 2686–2693 (2010).
Tucker, A. M., Whitney, P., Belenky, G., Hinson, J. M. & Van Dongen, H. P. A. Effects of Sleep Deprivation on Dissociated Components of Executive Functioning. Sleep 33, 47–57 (2010).
de Almeida, V. Z. G. et al. Free Recall of Word Lists under Total Sleep Deprivation and after Recovery Sleep. Sleep 35, 223–230 (2012).
Philip, P. et al. Acute Versus Chronic Partial Sleep Deprivation in Middle-Aged People: Differential Effect on Performance and Sleepiness. Sleep 35, 997–1002 (2012).
Elmenhorst, D. et al. Recovery sleep after extended wakefulness restores elevated A1 adenosine receptor availability in the human brain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 114, 4243–4248 (2017).
Dinges, D. F. et al. Cumulative Sleepiness, Mood Disturbance, and Psychomotor Vigilance Performance Decrements During a Week of Sleep Restricted to 4-5 Hours per Night. Sleep 20, 267–277 (1997).
Wu, J. C. et al. Frontal Lobe Metabolic Decreases with Sleep Deprivation not Totally Reversed by Recovery Sleep. Neuropsychopharmacology 31, 2783–2792 (2006).
Ikegami, K. et al. Recovery of cognitive performance and fatigue after one night of sleep deprivation. J. Occup. Health 51, 412–422 (2009).
Pejovic, S. et al. Effects of recovery sleep after one work week of mild sleep restriction on interleukin-6 and cortisol secretion and daytime sleepiness and performance. Am. J. Physiol. Metab. 305, E890–E896 (2013).
Lo, J. C., Ong, J. L., Leong, R. L. F., Gooley, J. J. & Chee, M. W. L. Cognitive Performance, Sleepiness, and Mood in Partially Sleep Deprived Adolescents: The Need for Sleep Study. Sleep 39, 687–698 (2016).
Boardman, J. M. et al. The ability to self-monitor cognitive performance during 60 h total sleep deprivation and following 2 nights recovery sleep. J. Sleep Res. 27, 1–8 (2018).
Saletin, J. M. et al. Human Hippocampal Structure: A Novel Biomarker Predicting Mnemonic Vulnerability to, and Recovery from, Sleep Deprivation. J. Neurosci. 36, 2355–2363 (2016).
Hu, P., Stylos-allan, M. & Walker, M. P. Sleep faciliates consolidation of emotional declarative memory. Psychol. Sci. 17, 891–898 (2006).
Chuah, L. Y. M. et al. Donepezil Improves Episodic Memory in Young Individuals Vulnerable to the Effects of Sleep Deprivation. Sleep 32, 999–1010 (2009).
Drummond, S. P. A. et al. Altered brain response to verbal learning following sleep deprivation. Nature 403, 655–657 (2000).
Killgore, W. D. S. Effects of sleep deprivation on cognition. Progress in Brain Research 185, (Elsevier B.V., 2010).
Gusnard, D. A., Akbudak, E., Shulman, G. L. & Raichle, M. E. Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: Relation to a default mode of brain function. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 98, 4259–4264 (2001).
Vincent, J. L. et al. Coherent Spontaneous Activity Identifies a Hippocampal-Parietal Memory Network. J Neurophysiol 96, 3517–3531 (2006).
Buckner, R. L., Andrews-Hanna, J. R. & Schacter, D. L. The brain’s default network: Anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1124, 1–38 (2008).
Ranganath, C., Heller, A., Cohen, M. X., Brozinsky, C. J. & Rissman, J. Functional connectivity with the hippocampus during successful memory formation. Hippocampus 15, 997–1005 (2005).
Schott, B. H. et al. The relationship between level of processing and hippocampal-cortical functional connectivity during episodic memory formation in humans. Hum. Brain Mapp. 34, 407–424 (2013).
Greicius, M. D., Srivastava, G., Reiss, A. L. & Menon, V. Default-mode network activity distinguishes Alzheimer’s disease from healthy aging: evidence from functional MRI. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 101, 4637–4642 (2004).
Sperling, R. A. et al. Functional alterations in memory networks in early alzheimer’s disease. NeuroMolecular Med. 12, 27–43 (2010).
Raichle, M. E. et al. A default mode of brain function. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 95, 676–682 (2001).
Vincent, J. L., Kahn, I., Snyder, A. Z., Raichle, M. E. & Buckner, R. L. Evidence for a Frontoparietal Control System Revealed by Intrinsic Functional Connectivity. J. Neurophysiol. 100, 3328–3342 (2008).
Sämann, P. G. et al. Increased sleep pressure reduces resting state functional connectivity. Magn. Reson. Mater. Physics, Biol. Med. 23, 375–389 (2010).
Havas, J. A. D., Parimal, S., Soon, C. S. & Chee, M. W. L. Sleep deprivation reduces default mode network connectivity and anti-correlation during rest and task performance. Neuroimage 59, 1745–1751 (2012).
Bosch, O. G. et al. Sleep deprivation increases dorsal nexus connectivity to the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 110, 19597–19602 (2013).
Lei, Y. et al. Large-Scale Brain Network Coupling Predicts Total Sleep Deprivation Effects on Cognitive Capacity. PLoS One 10, e0133959 (2015).
Nilsonne, G. et al. Intrinsic brain connectivity after partial sleep deprivation in young and older adults: Results from the Stockholm Sleepy Brain study. Sci. Rep. 7, 9422 (2017).
Fox, M. D. et al. The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 102, 9673–9678 (2005).
Fox, M. D. & Raichle, M. E. Spontaneous fluctuations in brain activity observed with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 8, 700–711 (2007).
Clapp, W. C., Rubens, M. T., Sabharwal, J. & Gazzaley, A. Deficit in switching between functional brain networks underlies the impact of multitasking on working memory in older adults. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 108, 7212–7217 (2011).
Turner, G. R. & Spreng, R. N. Executive functions and neurocognitive aging: Dissociable patterns of brain activity. Neurobiol. Aging 33, 826.e1–826.e13 (2012).
Lamond, N. et al. The dynamics of neurobehavioural recovery following sleep loss. J. Sleep Res. 16, 33–41 (2007).
Smith, C. Sleep states and memory processes. Behav. Brain Res. 69, 137–145 (1995).
Plihal, W. & Born, J. Effects of early and late nocturnal sleep on declarative and procedural memory. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 9, 534–547 (1997).
Ficca, G. & Salzarulo, P. What in sleep is for memory. Sleep Med. 5, 225–230 (2004).
Stickgold, R. Sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Nature 437, 1272–1278 (2005).
Rasch, B. & Born, J. About Sleep’s Role in Memory. Physiol. Rev. 93, 681–766 (2013).
Andrade, K. C. et al. Sleep Spindles and Hippocampal Functional Connectivity in Human NREM Sleep. J. Neurosci. 31, 10331–10339 (2011).
Sämann, P. G. et al. Development of the brain’s default mode network from wakefulness to slow wave sleep. Cereb. Cortex 21, 2082–2093 (2011).
Mander, B. A. et al. Prefrontal atrophy, disrupted NREM slow waves and impaired hippocampal-dependent memory in aging. Nat. Neurosci. 16, 357–364 (2013).
Alhola, P. & Polo-Kantola, P. Sleep deprivation: Impact on cognitive performance. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 3, 553–567 (2007).
Basner, M., Rao, H., Goel, N. & Dinges, D. F. Sleep deprivation and neurobehavioral dynamics. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 23, 854–863 (2013).
Gujar, N., Yoo, S. S., Hu, P. & Walker, M. P. The unrested resting brain: Sleep deprivation alters activity within the default-mode network. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22, 1637–1648 (2010).
Fox, M. D. & Greicius, M. Clinical applications of resting state functional connectivity. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 4, 19 (2010).
Sanchez Panchuelo, R. M., Stephenson, M. C., Francis, S. T. & Morris, P. G. Neural brain activation imaging. Biomedical Imaging: Applications and Advances, https://doi.org/10.1533/9780857097477.2.112 (2014).
Kang, J.-E. et al. Amyloid-β Dynamics Are Regulated by Orexin and the Sleep-Wake Cycle. Science (80-.). 326, 1005–1008 (2009).
Jack, C. R. et al. Tracking pathophysiological processes in Alzheimer’s disease: An updated hypothetical model of dynamic biomarkers. Lancet Neurol. 12, 207–216 (2013).
Gouras, G. K., Olsson, T. T. & Hansson, O. β-amyloid Peptides and Amyloid Plaques in Alzheimer’s Disease. Neurotherapeutics 12, 3–11 (2015).
Shokri-Kojori, E. et al. β-Amyloid accumulation in the human brain after one night of sleep deprivation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 201721694, https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1721694115 (2018).
Winer, J. R. et al. Sleep as a potential biomarker of Tau and β-amyloid burdern in the human brain. J. Neurosci. 39, 6315–6324 (2019).
Vitiello, M. V. & Borson, S. Sleep disturbances in patients with alzheimer’s disease: Epidemiology, pathophysiology and treatment. CNS Drugs 15, 777–796 (2001).
Gagnon, J.-F., Petit, D., Latreille, V. & Montplaisir, J. Neurobiology of Sleep Disturbances in Neurodegenerative Disorders. Curr. Pharm. Des. 14, 3430–3445 (2008).
Pace-Schott, E. F. & Spencer, R. M. C. Age-related changes in the cognitive function of sleep. Progress in Brain Research 191, (Elsevier B.V., 2011).
Harand, C. et al. How aging affects sleep-dependent memory consolidation? Front. Neurol. 3, 1–6 (2012).
Fang, Z. et al. Altered salience network connectivity predicts macronutrient intake after sleep deprivation. Sci. Rep. 5, 1–8 (2015).
Yang, F. N. et al. Sleep deprivation enhances inter-stimulus interval effect on vigilant attention performance. Sleep 41, 1–12 (2018).
Davies, J. A., Navaratnam, V. & Redfern, P. H. A 24-hour rhythm in passive-avoidance behaviour in rats. Psychopharmacologia 32, 211–214 (1973).
Folkard, S. Time of day and level of processing. Mem. Cognit. 7, 247–252 (1979).
Folkard, S. & Monk, T. H. Circadian rhythms in human memory. Br. J. Psychol. 71, 295–307 (1980).
Dunne, M. P., Roche, F. & Hartley, L. R. Effects of Time of Day on Immediate Recall and Sustained Retrieval from Semantic Memory. J. Gen. Psychol. 117, 403–410 (1990).
Testu, F. & Clarisse, R. Time-of-day and day-of-week effects on mnemonic performance. Chronobiol. Int. 16, 491–503 (1999).
Gorfine, T. & Zisapel, N. Melatonin and the human hippocampus, a time dependant interplay. J. Pineal Res. 43, 80–86 (2007).
Martin, B., Buffington, A. L. H. & Welsh-Bohmer, K. A. & Brandt. J. Time of day affects episodic memory in older adults. Aging, Neuropsychol. Cogn. 15, 146–164 (2008).
Wright, K. P., Lowry, C. A. & Lebourgeois, M. K. Circadian and wakefulness-sleep modulation of cognition in humans. Front. Mol. Neurosci. 5, 1–12 (2012).
Maylor, E. A. & Badham, S. P. Effects of time of day on age-related associative deficits. Psychol. Aging 33, 7–16 (2018).
Puttaert, D., Adam, S. & Peigneux, P. Subjectively-defined optimal/non-optimal time of day modulates controlled but not automatic retrieval processes in verbal memory. J. Sleep Res. 28, e12798 (2019).
Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M. & Cuthbert, B. N. International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Instruction manual and affective ratings, Technical Report A-8. Gainesv. Cent. Res. Psychophysiology, Univ. Florida. (2008).
Macmillan, N. A. & Creelman, C. D. Detection Theory A User’s Guide. Cambridge Univ Press New York. (1991).
Fair, D. A. et al. The maturing architecture of the brain’s default network. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 105, 4028–4032 (2008).
Tzourio-Mazoyer, N. et al. Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage 15, 273–289 (2002).
Diedenhofen, B. & Musch, J. cocor: A comprehensive solution for the statistical comparison of correlations. PLoS One 10, e0121945 (2015).
Acknowledgements
This research was supported in part by the grants from the National Institutes of Health (NIH R01 HL102119, R01 MH107571, R21 AG051981, P30 NS045839, CTRC UL1RR024134), a pilot grant from the Institute for Aging (IOA) of the University of Pennsylvania, Sun Yat-sen University, the Program for Professors of Special Appointment (Eastern Scholar) at Shanghai Institutions of Higher Learning, and Shanghai International Studies University Major Research Projects. The funders had no role in the study design, data collection and analysis, data interpretation, writing of the manuscript, or the decision to submit the article for publication.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
H.R., J.A.D. and D.F.D. designed the study and supervised the research. Y.C., Z.F., Y.F., S.X., Y.D., J.W., A.R., and H.R. carried out the research, collected the data, and contributed to data analyses. Y.C., M.Y., and H.R. prepared the figures and wrote the manuscript, J.J.K. and D.A.W. edited the manuscript. All authors discussed the results and commented on the manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Chai, Y., Fang, Z., Yang, F.N. et al. Two nights of recovery sleep restores hippocampal connectivity but not episodic memory after total sleep deprivation. Sci Rep 10, 8774 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-65086-x
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-65086-x
This article is cited by
-
Sleep-associated insulin resistance promotes neurodegeneration
Molecular Biology Reports (2023)
-
Sleep deprivation reorganizes the dynamic configurations of default mode network activity during recovery sleep
Science China Technological Sciences (2022)
-
Altered hippocampal transcriptome dynamics following sleep deprivation
Molecular Brain (2021)