Abstract
Although the fighting behaviour in gamecocks has evolved because of artificial selection, it is unknown whether the selection for aggressiveness affects neurotransmitter levels in the avian central nervous system. We sought to identify the source and origin of this trait. We collected the brain samples from 6 female Shamo gamecocks and 5 Shaver Brown chickens (control; bred for egg production). The midbrain levels of norepinephrine (NE) were significantly higher in Shamo gamecocks (P = 0.0087) than in the controls. Moreover, alleles encoding adrenergic receptors differed between the breeds in terms of response to NE. Gene mutations specific to Shamo and potentially associated with fighting behaviour were in sites T440N of ADRα1D; V296I of ADRα2A; and T44I, Q232R, and T277M of ADRβ2. The evolutionary analysis indicated that the ADRβ2 (T44I and Q232R) mutations were heritable in all Galliformes, whereas the T440N mutation of ADRα1D and V296I mutations of ADRα2A were unique to Shamo and originated by artificial selection. A high NE level may confer a selective advantage by enabling gamecocks to be aggressive and pain tolerant. Therefore, the strong fighting behaviour of Shamo has resulted from a combination of naturally inherited and mutant genes derived by artificial selection.
Introduction
Seventeen varieties of domesticated chicken (Gallus gallus domesticus) known as Japanese ornamental chickens were developed by artificial selection over a long period for their cultural entertainment value. They are characterised by different body colours and shapes1,2,3,4,5. Shamo gamecocks were bred for fighting. The birds were artificially selected under extreme stress to isolate desirable traits such as strength, aggression, and anxiety and pain endurance during cockfighting3,4. The shape and colour characteristics of modern-day Japanese Shamo have been depicted in wildlife caricatures approximately 1000 y ago. Shamo have body shapes and fighting styles distinct from those of other chickens5,6. After a match, losing Shamo males are euthanised by the breeders, whereas the victorious males are bred with healthy Shamo females. These pedigreed lines have been passed down from generation to generation and are highly valuable to breeders2.
The objective of the present study was to elucidate the source of the fighting behaviour in Shamo gamecocks. Our previous molecular evolutionary studies based on the mitochondrial D-loop region revealed that the Shamo gamecock breed originated from red junglefowl2,6. As Japanese ornamental chickens phenotypically differ from each other, intensive artificial selection may have been conducted before these varieties diverged from ancestral Shamo in Okinawa2,6. We also investigated the degree of genetic differentiation among Shamo chickens by focusing on the genes encoding dopamine receptors D2, D3, and D4 in domesticated chicken populations7. The dopamine receptors receive the neurotransmitter when it is released from presynaptic nerve terminals. They trigger crucial physiological responses regulating movement, cognition, reward, and hormone release. Genetic differentiation was evaluated using the nucleotide differentiation (NST) index. We found that the NST of DRD4 (dopamine receptor D4 gene) in Shamo (0.072) was significantly higher than that of the other DRD genes. The genes responsible for aggressiveness, behaviour, and other traits were analysed by array comparative genomic hybridisation (aCGH) in culturally domesticated chickens, gamecocks, and ornamental chickens1. The assay revealed 782 gene probe candidates for artificial selection pressure in culturally domesticated chickens.
Here, the aim was to examine the effects of neurotransmitters and their receptors in the brain on Shamo strength and aggression under high-stress conditions used for artificial selection. To the best of our knowledge, no previous research has explored the stress response in Shamo from different perspectives. Moreover, there has been no study on the neurotransmitters in the brains of Shamo gamecocks. This information will increase our understanding of the mechanism underlying the aggressive behaviour of Shamo gamecocks. It may also help elucidate the roles of neurotransmitters in the human brain and under stress-related conditions such as panic disorder, depression, syncope, and anxiety8,9,10,11,12.
Results
Analysis of monoamines in the chicken brain
We examined four neurotransmitters and six metabolites that are believed to influence aggressive behaviour. We compared Shamo cocks selected for cockfighting with Shaver Brown cocks selected for egg laying. The neurotransmitters examined were dopamine (DA), epinephrine (Epi), norepinephrine (NE), and 5-hydroxytryptamine (5-HT) as well as their metabolites 3,4-dihydroxyphenylacetic acid (DOPAC), 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid (5-HIAA), homovanillic acid (HVA), 3-methoxy-4-hydroxyphenylglycol (MHPG), 3-methoxytyramine (3-MT), and normethanephrine (NM). These monoamines are important substances for identifying the metabolic pathway of epinephrine13,14,15,16.
The levels of NE and NM were significantly higher in Shamo than in Shaver Brown as determined using the Mann–Whitney U test (Figs. 1A–C and S1D–J; Tables S1–S3). The striatum (St; P = 0.0303) and midbrain (Mid; P = 0.0087) levels of NE in Shamo were significantly higher (~1.7× and ~1.6×, respectively) than those in Shaver Brown. The NE level in the central cortex (Cx) was also approximately 1.4 times higher in Shamo than in Shaver Brown; however, this difference was not significant (P = 0.0823) (Fig. 1A). In contrast, the Cx (P = 0.0303) and Mid (P = 0.0519) levels of Epi in Shaver Brown were significantly higher (~1.5× and ~1.9×, respectively) than those in Shamo (Fig. 1B). There were no significant differences between Shamo and Shaver Brown in terms of the levels of the other monoamines in the St, Cx, and Mid. The Mid of the Shamo brain contained a significantly higher NM level than that of the Shaver Brown brain (P = 0.0087) (Fig. 1C). Thus, NE biosynthesis from dopamine proceeds via dopamine-β-hydroxylase (Fig. 2), and Shamo has a higher dopamine-β-hydroxylase activity than Shaver Brown.
Analysis of polymorphisms related to adrenergic receptor genes
There were significant differences between Shamo and Shaver Brown in terms of their brain Epi and NE levels. Therefore, we analysed the genes encoding the receptors for these neurotransmitters. We sampled three Shamo individuals with the highest NE levels and three Shaver Brown birds with the lowest NE levels (Fig. 1A). We then sequenced these samples to localise mutation sites in the ADRα1A, ADRα1B, ADRα1D, ADRα2A, ADRα2B, ADRα2C, ADRβ1, ADRβ2, and ADRβ3 receptor genes (Tables 1 and S4).
The results confirmed the presence of 54 ADR mutations in the three Shamo and three Shaver Brown birds, among which 33 were identified in Shamo and 21 in Shaver Brown. Many of the mutations were localised to ADRα2A, ADRα2B, and ADRβ2. The Shamo-specific mutations included the following: V296I in ADRα2A (bird S9), R138Q and R210H (bird S6), V292M in ADRα2B (birds S7 and S9), and L58W in ADRα1D (bird S7) (Table 1). In addition, the S6, S7, and S9 Shamo birds carried the original T440N mutation site in ADRα1D (exon 2). For ADRβ2, Shamo-specific mutations A15T (G43R), T44I (C131Y and T132Y), Q232R (A695R), and T277M (C830Y) were detected (Table 1). Therefore, Shamo-specific mutations were found in ADRα1D (T440N), ADRα2B (V292M), and ADRβ2 (A15T, T44I, and Q232R).
The N5, N6, and N7 Shaver Brown birds carried the original mutation S365G in ADRα1A (exon 2). Bird N6 had the R258Q site in ADRα1B and D273E site in ADRα2A. Bird N7 had the original R68Q mutation site in ADRα2B. Shaver Brown-specific mutations were detected in ADRα1A (S365G), ADRα1B (Q258R), and ADRβ1 (Q403R).
Shamo and Shaver Brown had the same original mutations, namely, V494M in ADRα1B and V58I and D273E in ADRα2a. R342C, S396P, and Q404L of ADRβ3 were observed in both breeds.
Prediction of transmembrane helices in proteins encoded by the ADR genes
We analysed transmembrane helices in the proteins encoded by each mutant allele using TMHMM server. The ADR genes encode membrane proteins. Each of these proteins has seven transmembrane structures (Fig. 3A–C; Table S5A-C). The TMHMM analysis indicated that the most highly mutated gene was ADRβ2: T277M, T44I (Fig. 3C; Table S5C). The mutated sectors were located in the TMhelix (transmembrane region), whereas Q232R was intracellular on the cytoplasmic side. A15T was extracellular and not directly related to behaviour.
Prediction of transmembrane helices in proteins encoded by ADRα2A, ADRα1D, and ADRβ2. Transmembrane helices in the proteins encoded by ADRα2A (A), ADRα1D (B), and ADRβ2 (C) in Shamo were predicted using the TMHMM secondary structure. Segments of the cytoplasmic side (intracellular), exterior (extracellular), and transmembrane region (TMhelix) are displayed.
Several mutations were observed in D273E and V296I of ADRα2A (Fig. 3A; Table S5A). In ADRα2B, mutations were detected at the R138Q and R210H sites in bird S6 and the V292M site in birds S7 and S9. The mutations at the R138Q and R210H sites were intracellular on the cytoplasmic side and possibly related to behaviour. As the V292M site was extracellular, it was probably not associated with bird behaviour. The T440N site of ADRα1D was also found in Shamo (Fig. 3B; Table S5B). The differences in the number of mutations in this membrane protein suggest that ADRβ2 is more closely associated with fighting behaviour than ADRα2. T 441 I (TMhelix) and Q232R (inside) of ADRβ2 may regulate fighting behaviour (Fig. 3C; Table S5C). The TMHMM analysis disclosed that Shaver Brown birds N5, N6, and N7 had mutations on intracellular ADRα1A (S365G) and ADRβ1 (Q403R). These mutations conferred the birds a selective advantage as egg layers. Furthermore, ADRα1B (V494M), ADRα2A (V58I and D273E), and ADRβ3 (R342C, S396P, and Q404L) mutated simultaneously with breeder domestication.
Molecular phylogeny analysis of aggressiveness in Shamo based on ADRα1D, ADRα2A, and ADRβ2
We conducted an evolutionary analysis to identify the origin of aggressiveness. We constructed phylogenetic trees using ADRα1D(1536 bp): T440N, ADRα2A(1332 bp): D273E, V296I, and ADRβ2(1170 bp): T44I, Q232R. We also extracted sequences of red junglefowl, wild turkey, Guinea fowl, and Japanese quail from the NCBI database17,18. These species exhibit strong aggressive behaviours. For example, pheasants can be territorial19,20,21. The evolutionary analysis indicated that these birds have similar mutations. The ADRα1D: T440N, ADRα2A: D273E and V296I mutations appeared exclusively in Shamo and not in any other Phasianidae species. In contrast, ADRβ2: T44I and Q232R were confirmed in wild turkey, Guinea fowl, Japanese quail, and Shamo (Fig. 4). Hence, red junglefowl was the only species that lacked these two mutations.
Phylogenetic tree of ADRβ2 in Galliformes members. ADRβ2: T44I and Q232R were confirmed in wild turkey, Guinea fowl, and Japanese quail (Fig. 4). Red junglefowl was the only species lacking these three mutations. Diamonds indicate the T44I and Q232R mutations.
Discussion
Here, we examined the effects of brain neurotransmitters on Shamo gamecock behaviour. In this breed, artificial selection under extreme stress has improved bird strength and aggression. We analysed 10 brain neurotransmitters and the ADR genes that may either regulate aggression in Shamo used for cockfights or determine calmness of Shaver Brown bred for egg production under high population densities.
We observed significant differences between Shamo and Shaver Brown in terms of Mid NE level. The Mid NE levels were found to be closely related to Shamo aggressiveness. The Mid NE level in S6 was four times higher than that in N5 (Fig. 1A). NE is secreted from the ends of the sympathetic nerves in response to fighting stimulation. It serves as an alarm system in the brain and attenuates stress and tension caused by sudden unpleasant internal and external stimuli. The NE alarm system reduces anxiety and fear22,23,24,25,26. NE increases attention, concentration, judgment, and motivation. It is also known as the ‘angry hormone’ as it induces corporeal tension or excitement in response to stress23,27,28. Aggressiveness increases as negative emotions intensify. NE also increases the heart rate and blood flow to the brain and skeletal muscles29. Therefore, excess NE increases nerve activity, and thus, the animal becomes restless and aggressive30. A portion of the Mid is related to the pathway of sensory and motor neurons31,32,33,34, maintains reflex eyeball movements, and adjusts iris contraction35.
We analysed the receptor genes (ADR) regulated by Epi or NE (Table 1)36,37,38,39. In both breeds, 54 amino acid mutation sites were confirmed in eight ADR genes (Shamo, 33 sites; Shaver Brown, 21 sites). Prediction of transmembrane helices in proteins (TMHMM) suggested an association between fighting behaviour and specific mutations, namely, T440N of ADRα1D; V296I and D273E of ADRα2A; and T44 I, Q232R, and T277M of ADRβ2 (Fig. 3A–C). Several mutations may be related to domestication including ADRα1B (V494M), ADRα2A (V58I), and ADRβ3 (R342C, S396P, and Q404L) in domesticated chickens originally selected from the wild type (G. gallus). The mutation sites in ADRα1A (S365G) may be important for selecting traits associated with calmness and non-aggressiveness. Thus, these mutations may not be related to fighting behaviour, but are advantageous for housed birds raised for human food production. Moreover, T44I and Q232R of ADRβ2 have been detected in other Galliformes members using the molecular phylogeny analysis. We could not confirm these mutations in the wild type (G. gallus) used in our previous study1,7.
The wild type still maintains a substantial proportion of ancestral polymorphisms at the genomic level40. Our phylogenetic analysis revealed that Shamo are distinctively aggressive and that this trait evolved by combining mutations derived from artificial selection and natural adaptation in Galliformes (Fig. 4).
The vascular smooth muscle relaxes in response to the ADRβ subunit (Gs) and contracts in response to the ADRα subunit (Gi). This mechanism substantially affects the blood vessel responses during sympathetic excitement. In this state, the ADRα subunit (Gi) is expressed mainly in the blood vessels that regulate blood flow to the heart and to the skeletal muscles required for fighting41,42,43,44. The blood vessels of the heart, lungs, and skeletal muscles mainly express the ADRβ subunit (Gs)12 and expand in response to sympathetic excitation45,46,47,48,49,50,51,52,53,54. In this way, they maintain blood flow to the organs required for fighting. Therefore, relative differences in NE level and specific receptor mutations were observed in different environments, and they favoured aggression in Shamo and a calm nature in Shaver Brown living communally. These mutations occurred in captive populations and were deemed necessary for domestication55,56.
Changes in the NE receptor genes and hormone sensitivity occur as the NE level increases57. Excess NE increases the aggressiveness of Shamo, whereas low NE levels result in lethargy but enable Shamo to endure prolonged fighting stress.
Although dopamine is the precursor of NE, we observe no significant differences in the brains of the different breeds in terms of the dopamine levels. However, in our previous study, we found that the NST of DRD4 in Shamo (0.072) was significantly higher than that of the other genes in domesticated chicken populations7. Therefore, the fighting behaviour of Shamo is more closely related to the norepinephrine level than the dopamine level, and mutations in the ADR receptor have the strongest influence on fighting behaviour. Thus, the accumulation of several ADR polymorphisms and elevation in the brain NE levels may have conserved the aggressiveness in Shamo.
NE also reduces pain perception58,59,60,61,62. ADRα2A, closely associated with pain response, was mutated in Shamo bred for cockfights63,64. The Mid NE alleviates pain via the ADRα2 receptor gene58,63,64. In combats, Shamo birds either fight or fly away (‘fight-or-flight’). They must be able to take immediate action in response to changes in the NE level55,65. If Shamo birds were insensitive to pain during fighting, they could either continue fighting or escape. Several receptors may be involved in stress relief. Isolation of mutant alleles in Shamo by aCGH revealed the γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA-a) receptor gene associated with the central nervous system (CNS)1. The GABA-a receptor mitigates anxiety and relaxes skeletal muscles66,67,68. Artificial selection of Shamo for cockfight enhanced its fighting behaviour by reducing stress related to anxiety and fear.
The CNS responds to stimuli from receptors and induces feelings or sensations. This response may be used to assess the effects of neurotransmitters on the brain. The ‘fight-or-flight’ response is a conserved behaviour in vertebrates experiencing fear, stress, or intense physical activity. Thus, we can investigate the effects of the neurotransmitters triggering the ‘fight-or-flight’ response in the brain69.
On the basis of the typical brain monoamine levels, we propose that aggressiveness may be related to the ADR genes based on the observed relative differences in brain NE level. Aggressiveness is determined by several genes and neurotransmitters70,71,72,73,74,75,76. Identification of these factors may help elucidate their modes of action.
In future research, we will investigate the gene levels of receptors for catecholamines and other substances in various parts of the brains of Shamo and other chickens. We will also measure neurotransmitter levels after cockfights and analyse the exomes of ornamental chickens, Shamo, and other chickens bred for different purposes. We will explore the relationships between chicken domestication and neurotransmitters and the effect of artificial selection on the genes implicated in this process. Here, we identified the adrenergic genes in Shaver Brown crucial for group living and those related to the combative trait in Shamo. Therefore, we will conduct ex vivo assays of the localised NE levels in the Mid of live animals and/or animal models subjected to various stressors.
Stress contributes to numerous human diseases and influences brain neurotransmitter levels. In the future, we will examine the effects of neurotransmitters that govern the ‘fight-or-flight’ response. Elucidation of the neurochemical mechanisms involved in this process may help improve defences against the deleterious effects of stress.
Conclusions
Our results showed that the Mid level of NE was significantly higher in Shamo bred for fighting than in Shaver Brown bred for egg laying. The fighting instinct might be correlated with the type and distribution of ADRβ2 alleles and their responses to the NE level. Gene polymorphisms can change with the NE level. Therefore, we believe that the strong fighting behaviour of Shamo was probably due to the combination of naturally inherited genes with mutant genes derived from artificial selection.
Materials and Methods
Ethics statements
All animal experiments were conducted in strict compliance with the ethical guidelines of Tokai University, Japan. Approval was also obtained from the Animal Investigation Committee of Tokai University, Japan (Approval Nos. 141024 and 152010).
Chickens and brain samples
Shamo chicks were randomly selected from various areas and breeders. These birds would have otherwise been used in cockfights. The Shaver Brown chicks were randomly selected from several farms. Shaver Brown was selected as the docile breed. It has a superior egg production trait. Furthermore, it was previously described as a strong candidate for artificial selection. The brain samples were randomly collected from 6 female Shamo birds and 5 domesticated Shaver Brown chickens. The latter were bred as egg layers and served as the controls77 (Fig. 5). Shamo females were used because they are invaluable in the genetic improvement of chicken lines2. Shamo birds were individually maintained in separate cages. Shaver Brown chickens were maintained in groups outside. Samples were obtained from 24-wk-old chickens, because at this age, the females start ovulating and the males are trained for cockfights. The chickens were decapitated and the backs of their skull were opened with pliers. Then, the brains were rapidly excised and divided into Cx, St, and Mid according to the Chicken Brain Atlas (http://avianbrain.org/nomen/Chicken_Atlas.html)78,79,80,81. These tissues were selected because they are easily distinguished from each other and respond to multiple neurotransmitters. The brain samples were frozen on dry ice, weighed, and stored at −80 °C. Images of Shamo brains are shown in Fig. 6.
Striatum (St), cerebral cortex (Cx), and midbrain (Mid) of Shamo. Shamo skulls were more difficult to remove than Shaver Brown skulls as the former have been strengthened for cockfighting by selective breeding. Thus, the removal of their skulls required considerable force and resulted in the skulls shattering into numerous fragments. Moreover, the Shamo brains had a higher tension and gloss than the Shaver Brown chicken brains.
The skull of Shamo birds was more difficult to remove than that of Shaver Brown chickens as the former birds have been strengthened for cockfighting by selective breeding. Thus, the removal of their skull required considerable force and resulted in their skulls shattering into numerous fragments. Moreover, the brain of Shamo birds had a higher tension and gloss than the brain of Shaver Brown birds (Fig. 6).
Analysis of monoamines and their metabolites in the brain tissues
The brain tissues were homogenised in 0.2 M perchloric acid (HClO4) containing 100 µM EDTA-Na and 100 ng isoproterenol as an internal standard. The homogenates were centrifuged at 20 000 × g and 4 °C for 15 min. The pH of the supernatant was adjusted to 3.0 with 1 M sodium acetate. The samples were passed through a 0.45-µm filter (UFC40HV; EMD Millipore, Billerica, MA, USA). The filtrate (10 µL) was injected into a high-performance liquid chromatography-electrochemical detection (HPLC-ECD) system (Eicom, Kyoto, Japan) consisting of a 150 mm × 3 mm octadecylsilane column (EICOMPAK SC-5ODS; Eicom, Kyoto, Japan), a pump (EP-300; Eicom, Kyoto, Japan), a column oven (ATC-300; Eicom, Kyoto, Japan), and an electrochemical detector (ECD-300; Eicom, Kyoto, Japan). The mobile phase consisted of aceto–citric acid buffer (0.1 M; pH 3.5), methanol, sodium-1-octane sulfonate (0.46 M), and disodium ethylenediaminetetraacetic acid (0.015 mM) [830:170:1.9:1]. The flow rate was 0.5 mL min−l. The levels of DA, Epi, NE, 5-HT, DOPAC, 5-HIAA, HVA, MHPG, 3-MT, and NM were calculated using PowerChrom v. 2.6.11 (eDAQ Inc., Colorado Springs, CO, USA).
DNA extraction, polymerase chain reaction (PCR) amplification, and sequencing of the adrenaline genes (ADRα1A, ADRα1B, ADRα1D, ADRα2A, ADRα2B, ADRα2C, ADRβ1, ADRβ2, and ADRβ3)
Blood was drawn from each chicken and the samples were suspended in 400 μL of TNES-8M urea. Twenty microlitres of proteinase K (20 mg mL−l) and 20 μL of 1 M dithiothreitol (DTT) were added to the samples, which were then incubated for 1–5 h at 60 °C and mixed with 500 μL of phenol/chloroform/isoamyl alcohol solution (25:24:1) for 3 min. This step was repeated twice. After precipitation with 2–2.5 volumes of ethanol, the pellets were rinsed in 70% v/v cold ethanol and dried. The samples were dissolved in TE buffer [10 mM Tris-HCl (pH 8.0) and 1 mM EDTA]. The PCR was performed to amplify ADRα1A (Ex1 and Ex2), ADRα1B (Ex1 and Ex2), ADRα1D (Ex1 and Ex2), ADRα2A, ADRα2B, ADRα2C, ADRβ1, ADRβ2, and ADRβ3 (Ex1 and Ex2). The PCR primers are listed in Table S6. The PCR enzymes used were KOD-Plus and KOD FX neo (TOYOBO, Osaka, Japan). The PCR was performed under the following conditions: denaturation for 20 s at 98 °C, 30 cycles for 5 s at 98 °C, annealing at 64 °C for 30 s, 68 °C for 1 min, and a final extension at 68 °C for 7 min. The PCR products were purified using the MinElute PCR purification kit (QIAGEN, Duesseldorf, Germany) and ExoSAP-IT (USB Corp., Cleveland, OH, USA) and sequenced using BigDye Terminator v. 3.1 (Thermo Fisher Scientific, Waltham, MA, USA) and the ABI Prism 3730xl DNA sequencer (ABI, Foster City, CA, USA).
Sequence assembly and alignment
The contigs of each ADR gene were assembled from the ABI DNA sequences using ATGC GENETYX v. 13 (GENETYX Corp., Tokyo, Japan). The sequences were then determined and registered in the International Nucleotide Sequence Database Collaboration (INSDC). The accession numbers of the ADR DNA sequences (LC483765-LC483818) were obtained from the DDBJ/EMBL/GenBank. All assembled sequences were aligned using CLUSTALW-MEGA v. 782,83. These analyses confirmed the mutation sites in the ADR genes of each chicken breed. The ADR receptor gene sequences were mapped for both breeds and their mutation sites were determined. The sequence of red junglefowl, the wild ancestor of chickens, was used as the reference (Table S4).
Statistical analysis
Data are presented as mean ± SD. Statistical analyses were conducted using Prism v. 6.0c (GraphPad Software, San Diego, CA, USA). Comparisons of the mean monoamine and metabolite levels were made using the Mann–Whitney U test. The results with a P value of <0.05 were considered statistically significant.
Molecular phylogeny analysis of complete ADRβ2
A phylogenetic tree (Fig. 1) was constructed using the UPGMA method83. The UPGMA algorithms were incorporated into CLUSTALW-MEGA v. 7 using distances corrected for multiple hits based on Kimura’s two-parameter model83. Sites representing gaps in any of the aligned sequences were excluded from the analysis. For phylogenetic tree construction, we used the bootstrap analysis of 1000 replications to assess statistical confidence in the branching order of the trees. Complete ADRβ2 sequences for red junglefowl (XM_015293684.2), wild turkey (XM_010719047.2), Guinea fowl (XM_021410489.1), Japanese quail (XM_015876329.1), and Shamo (LC483807 - LC483809) were obtained from the DDBJ/EMBL/GenBank database. These birds were selected because of their close evolutionary relationships with chickens.
Prediction of transmembrane helices in proteins
The transmembrane helices in the encoded proteins were analysed for each mutant allele using TMHMM server v. 2.0 (http://www.cbs.dtu.dk/services/TMHMM/).
For this analysis, ADRα2A; XP_004942333.2, ADRα1D; XP_004936331.1, and ADRβ2; XP_015149170.1 were used as the reference from the NCBI.
Data availability
All sequence data are available from the DDBJ, EBI, and NCBI databases.
References
Komiyama, T., Lin, M. & Ogura, A. aCGH Analysis to estimate genetic variations among domesticated chickens. BioMed. Res. Int. 2016, 1–8, https://doi.org/10.1155/2016/1794329 (2016).
Komiyama, T., Ikeo, K., Tateno, Y. & Gojobori, T. Japanese domesticated chickens have been derived from Shamo traditional fighting cocks. Mol. Phylogenet. Evol. 33, 16–21, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ympev.2004.04.019 (2004).
Komiyama, T., Ikeo, K. & Gojobori, T. The evolutionary origin of long-crowing chicken: its evolutionary relationship with fighting cocks disclosed by the mtDNA sequence analysis. Gene 333, 91–99, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gene.2004.02.035 (2004).
Finsterbusch, C. Cock fighting all over the world 99–116 (Nimrod Press Ltd., 1980).
Dundes, A. The cockfight. England 9–16 (The University of Wisconsin Press, 1994).
Komiyama, T., Ikeo, K. & Gojobori, T. Where is the origin of the Japanese gamecocks? Gene 317, 195–202, https://doi.org/10.1016/s0378-1119(03)00703-0 (2003).
Komiyama, T. et al. Dopamine receptor genes and evolutionary differentiation in the domestication of fighting cocks and long-crowing chickens. Plos One 9, e101778, https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0101778 (2014).
Alvarenga, M. E., Richards, J. C., Lambert, G. & Esler, M. D. Psychophysiological mechanisms in panic disorder: a correlative analysis of noradrenaline spillover, neuronal noradrenaline reuptake, power spectral analysis of heart rate variability, and psychological variables. Psychosom. Med. 68, 8–16, https://doi.org/10.1097/01.psy.0000195872.00987.db (2006).
Carhart-Harris, R. L. et al. Psilocybin with psychological support for treatment-resistant depression: an open-label feasibility study. Lancet Psychiat. 3, 619–627, https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(16)30065-7 (2016).
Pinto-Sanchez, M. I. et al. Probiotic Bifidobacterium longum NCC3001 reduces depression scores and alters brain activity: A pilot study in patients with irritable bowel syndrome. Gastroenterology 153, 448–459, https://doi.org/10.1053/j.gastro.2017.05.003 (2017).
Komiyama, T. et al. Neurally mediated syncope diagnosis based on adenylate cyclase activity in Japanese patients. Plos One 14, e0214733, https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0214733 (2019).
Komiyama, T. et al. Relationship between human evolution and neurally mediated syncope disclosed by the polymorphic sites of the adrenergic receptor gene α2B-AR. Plos One 10, e0120788, https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0120788 (2015).
Eisenhofer, G. & Finberg, J. P. Different metabolism of norepinephrine and epinephrine by catechol-O-methyltransferase and monoamine oxidase in rats. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 268, 1242–1251 (1994).
Haavik, J., Blau, N. & Thöny, B. Mutations in human monoamine-related neurotransmitter pathway genes. Hum. Mutat. 29, 891–902, https://doi.org/10.1002/humu.20700 (2008).
Sjoerdsma, A. Relationships between alterations in amine metabolism and blood pressure. Circ. Res. 9, 734–745, https://doi.org/10.1161/01.RES.9.3.734 (1961).
Eisenhofer, G., Kopin, I. J. & Goldstein, D. S. Catecholamine metabolism: a contemporary view with implications for physiology and medicine. Pharmacol. Rev. 56, 331–349, https://doi.org/10.1124/pr.56.3.1 (2004).
Stevens, L. & Lewis, S. Genetics and evolution of the domestic fowl (University Press, 1991).
Helm-Bychowski, K. M. & Wilson, A. C. Rates of nuclear DNA evolution in pheasant-like birds: evidence from restriction maps. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 83, 688–692, https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.83.3.688 (1986).
von Schantz, T. et al. Female choice selects for a viability-based male trait in pheasants. Nature 337, 166–169, https://doi.org/10.1038/337166a0 (1989).
Göransson, G., von Schantz, T., Fröberg, I., Helgee, A. & Wittzell, H. Male characteristics, viability and harem size in the pheasant, Phasianus colchicus. Anim. Behav. 40, 89–104, https://doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80668-2 (1990).
Hoodless, A. N., Kurtenbach, K., Nuttall, P. A. & Randolph, S. E. The impact of ticks on pheasant territoriality. Oikos 96, 245–250, https://doi.org/10.1034/j.1600-0706.2002.960206.x (2002).
Todd, R. M. et al. Neurogenetic variations in norepinephrine availability enhance perceptual vividness. J. Neurosci. 35, 6506–6516, https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4489-14.2015 (2015).
White, T. L. & Depue, R. A. Differential association of traits of fear and anxiety with norepinephrine-and dark-induced pupil reactivity. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 77, 863–877, https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.77.4.863 (1999).
Graeff, F. G. Neuroanatomy and neurotransmitter regulation of defensive behaviors and related emotions in mammals. Braz. J. Med. Biol. Res. 27, 811–829 (1994).
Schweimer, J., Fendt, M. & Schnitzler, H. U. Effects of clonidine injections into the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis on fear and anxiety behavior in rats. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 507, 117–124 (2005).
Ferrazzo, S. et al. Increased anxiety-like behavior following circuit-specific catecholamine denervation in mice. Neurobiol. Dis. 125, 55–66 (2019).
Delfs, J., Zhu, Y., Druhan, J. & Aston-Jones, G. Noradrenaline in the ventral forebrain is critical for opiate withdrawal-induced aversion. Nature 403, 430–434, https://doi.org/10.1038/35000212 (2000).
Mellor, D. Animal emotions, behaviour and the promotion of positive welfare states. (Taylor & Francis, 2012).
Carter, J. R., Kupiers, N. T. & Ray, C. A. Neurovascular responses to mental stress. J. Physiol 564, 321–327, https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.2004.079665 (2005).
Hugdahl, K. Psychophysiology: the mind-body perspective. (Harvard University Press, 1995).
Lumsden, A. & Keynes, R. Segmental patterns of neuronal development in the chick hindbrain. Nature 337, 424–428, https://doi.org/10.1038/337424a0 (1989).
Nottebohm, F. Brain pathways for vocal learning in birds. Prog. Psychobiol. Physiol. 86–125 (1980).
De Lanerolle, N. & Andrew, R. Midbrain structures controlling vocalization in the domestic chick. Brain Behav. Evol. 10, 354–376, https://doi.org/10.1159/000124324 (1974).
Benzo, C. Nervous system in Avian physiology (ed. Sturkie, P. D.), 1–36 (Springer, 1986).
Muskens, L. J. J. On tracts and centers involved in the upward and downward associated movements of the eyes after experiments in birds. J. Comp. Neurol. 50, 289–331 (1930).
Zoladz, P. R. et al. ADRA2B deletion variant influences time-dependent effects of pre-learning stress on long-term memory. Neurobiol. Learn Mem. 140, 71–81, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2017.02.014 (2017).
Mammarella, N. et al. The modulating role of ADRA2B in emotional working memory: Attending the negative but remembering the positive. Neurobiol. Learn Mem. 130, 129–134, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2016.02.009 (2016).
Gibbs, A. A., Naudts, K. H., Azevedo, R. T. & David, A. S. Deletion variant of α2b-adrenergic receptor gene moderates the effect of COMT val158met polymorphism on episodic memory performance. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 20, 272–275, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2009.12.007 (2010).
Wirz, L., Wacker, J., Felten, A., Reuter, M. & Schwabe, L. A deletion variant of the α2b-adrenoceptor modulates the stress-induced shift from “cognitive” to “habit” memory. J. Neurosci. 37, 2149–2160, https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3507-16.2017 (2017).
Sawai, H. et al. The origin and genetic variation of domestic chickens with special reference to junglefowls Gallus g. gallus and G. varius. Plos One 5, e10639, https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0010639 (2010).
Robertson, D., Biaggioni, I., Burnstock, G., Low, P. A. & Paton, J. F. Primer on the autonomic nervous system. (Academic Press, 2012).
Graham, R. M., Perez, D. M., Hwa, J. & Piascik, M. T. α1-adrenergic receptor subtypes molecular structure, function, and signaling. Circ. Res. 78, 737–749, https://doi.org/10.1161/01.RES.78.5.737 (1996).
Cockcroft, J. R. et al. P. β2-adrenoceptor polymorphism determines vascular reactivity in humans. Hypertension 36, 371–375 (2000).
Xie, H. G. et al. Human β2-adrenergic receptor polymorphisms: No association with essential hypertension in black or white Americans. Clin. Pharmacol. Ther. 67, 670–675, https://doi.org/10.1067/mcp.2000.106293 (2000).
Davare, M. A. et al. A β 2 adrenergic receptor signaling complex assembled with the Ca 2+ channel Cav1.2. Science 293, 98–101, https://doi.org/10.1126/science.293.5527.98 (2001).
Baillie, G. S. et al. β-Arrestin-mediated PDE4 cAMP phosphodiesterase recruitment regulates β-adrenoceptor switching from Gs to Gi. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 100, 940–945, https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.262787199 (2003).
Meinkoth, J. L. et al. Signal transduction through the cAMP-dependent protein kinase. Mol. Cell Biochem. 127–128, 179–186, https://doi.org/10.1007/bf01076769 (1993).
Gray, P. C., Scott, J. D. & Catterall, W. A. Regulation of ion channels by cAMP-dependent protein kinase and A-kinase anchoring proteins. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 8, 330–334, https://doi.org/10.1016/S0959-4388(98)80057-3 (1998).
Horinouchi, T. et al. Adenylate cyclase/cAMP/protein kinase A signaling pathway inhibits endothelin type A receptor-operated Ca2+ entry mediated via transient receptor potential canonical 6 channels. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 340, 143–151, https://doi.org/10.1124/jpet (2012).
Ekokoski, E. Extracellular ATP as a regulator of intracellular signaling in Thyroid FRTL-5 cells: Regulation by a G(i)/G(o) protein, Ca2+, and mitogen-activated protein kinase. J. Cell Physiol. 183, 155–162, 10.1002/(SICI)1097-4652(200005)183:2<155::AID-JCP2>3.0.CO;2-R (2000).
Willoughby, D. & Cooper, D. M. Organization and Ca2+ regulation of adenylyl cyclases in cAMP microdomains. Physiol. Rev. 87, 965–1010, https://doi.org/10.1152/physrev.00049.2006 (2007).
Lincoln, T. M., Cornwell, T. L. & Taylor, A. E. cGMP-dependent protein kinase mediates the reduction of Ca2+ by cAMP in vascular smooth muscle cells. Am. J. Physiol. 258, C399–C407, https://doi.org/10.1152/ajpcell.1990.258.3.C399 (1990).
Ishikawa, T., Hume, J. R. & Keef, K. D. Regulation of Ca2+ channels by cAMP and cGMP in vascular smooth muscle cells. Circ. Res. 73, 1128–1137, https://doi.org/10.1161/01.RES.73.6.1128 (1993).
Keef, K. D., Hume, J. R. & Zhong, J. Regulation of cardiac and smooth muscle Ca2+ channels (CaV1. 2a, b) by protein kinases. Am. J. Physiol. Cell Physiol. 281, C1743–C56, https://doi.org/10.1152/ajpcell.2001.281.6.C1743 (2001).
Ericsson, M., Fallahsharoudi, A., Bergquist, J., Kushnir, M. M. & Jensen, P. Domestication effects on behavioral and hormonal responses to acute stress in chickens. Physiol. Behav. 133, 161–169, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.05.024 (2014).
Moe, R. O. et al. Anticipatory and foraging behaviors in response to palatable food reward in chickens: effects of dopamine D2 receptor blockade and domestication. Physiol. Behav. 133, 170–177 (2014).
Płaźnik, A. & Kostowski, W. Modification of behavioral response to intra-hippocampal injections of noradrenaline and adrenoceptor agonists by chronic treatment with desipramine and citalopram: functional aspects of adaptive receptor changes. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 117, 245–252 (1985).
Strobel, C., Hunt, S., Sullivan, R., Sun, J. & Sah, P. Emotional regulation of pain: the role of noradrenaline in the amygdala. Sci. China Life Sci. 57, 384–390, https://doi.org/10.1007/s11427-014-4638-x (2014).
Kohli, U. et al. Effects of variation in the human α2A-and α2C-adrenoceptor genes on cognitive tasks and pain perception. Eur. J. Pain 14, 154–159, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejpain.2009.04.003 (2010).
Bruehl, S., Chung, O. Y., Diedrich, L., Diedrich, A. & Robertson, D. The relationship between resting blood pressure and acute pain sensitivity: effects of chronic pain and alpha-2 adrenergic blockade. J. Behav. Med. 31, 71–80 (2008).
Elfwing, M., Fallahshahroudi, A., Lindgren, I., Jensen, P. & Altimiras, J. The strong selective sweep candidate gene ADRA2C does not explain domestication related changes in the stress response of chickens. Plos One 9, e103218 (2014).
Millan, M. J. Descending control of pain. Prog. Neurobiol. 66, 355–474, https://doi.org/10.1016/s0301-0082(02)00009-6 (2002).
Alba-Delgado, C. et al. The function of alpha-2-adrenoceptors in the rat locus coeruleus is preserved in the chronic constriction injury model of neuropathic pain. Psychopharmacology 221, 53–65, https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-011-2542-7 (2012).
Pertovaara, A. Noradrenergic pain modulation. Prog. Neurobiol. 80, 53–83, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pneurobio.2006.08.001 (2006).
Løtvedt, P., Fallahshahroudi, A., Bektic, L., Altimiras, J. & Jensen, P. Chicken domestication changes expression of stress-related genes in brain, pituitary and adrenals. Neurobiol. Stress 7, 113–121 (2017).
Dumont, É. C. & Williams, J. T. Noradrenaline triggers GABAA inhibition of bed nucleus of the stria terminalis neurons projecting to the ventral tegmental area. J. Neurosci. 24, 8198–8204, https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0425-04.2004 (2004).
Harvey, R. A., Clark, M., Finkel, R., Rey, J. & Whalen, K. Lippincott’s illustrated reviews: Pharmacology. (Philadelphia, 2012).
McCarthy, M., Pfaff, D. & Schwartz-Giblin, S. Midbrain central gray GABA A receptor activation enhances, and blockade reduces, sexual behavior in the female rat. Exp. Brain Res. 86, 108–116, https://doi.org/10.1007/bf00231045 (1991).
Fuller, M. D., Emrick, M. A., Sadilek, M., Scheuer, T. & Catterall, W. A. Molecular mechanism of calcium channel regulation in the fight-or-flight response. Sci. Signal. 3, ra70, https://doi.org/10.1126/scisignal.2001152 (2010).
Lutz, V. et al. A genome-wide association study in a large F2-cross of laying hens reveals novel genomic regions associated with feather pecking and aggressive pecking behavior. Genet. Sel. Evol. 49, 18 (2017).
Li, Z. et al. Genome-wide association study of aggressive behavior in chicken. Sci. Rep 6, 30981 (2016).
Buitenhuis, B., Hedegaard, J., Janss, L. & Sørensen, P. Differentially expressed genes for aggressive pecking behavior in laying hens. BMC Genom 10, 544 (2009).
Tsuda, A. et al. Expression of aggression attenuates stress-induced increases in rat brain noradrenaline turnover. Brain Res. 474, 174–180 (1988).
Matsumoto, K., Ojima, K. & Watanabe, H. Noradrenergic denervation attenuates desipramine enhancement of aggressive behavior in isolated mice. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 50, 481–484 (1995).
Haller, J. et al. Behavioral responses to social stress in noradrenaline transporter knockout mice: Effects on social behavior and depression. Brain Res. Bull. 58, 279–284 (2002).
Sulser, F. Regulation and function of noradrenaline receptor systems in brain: psychopharmacological aspects. Neuropharmacology 23, 255–261 (1984).
Profit, G. Comparative performance of Nera Black and Shaver Brown hens fed self-compounded and commercial layers’ diets. Asian J. Sci. Technol 6, 940–946 (2015).
Kops, M. S. et al. Selection for low mortality in laying hens affects catecholamine levels in the arcopallium, a brain area involved in fear and motor regulation. Behav. Brain Res. 257, 54–61, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2013.09.035 (2013).
Kubikova, L., Wada, K. & Jarvis, E. D. Dopamine receptors in a songbird brain. J. Comp. Neurol. 518, 741–769, https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.22255 (2010).
Thode, C., Bock, J., Braun, K. & Darlison, M. The chicken immediate-early gene ZENK is expressed in the medio-rostral neostriatum/hyperstriatum ventrale, a brain region involved in acoustic imprinting, and is up-regulated after exposure to an auditory stimulus. Neuroscience 130, 611–617, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.10.015 (2005).
Kostál, L. et al. Influence of food restriction on dopamine receptor densities, catecholamine concentrations and dopamine turnover in chicken brain. Neuroscience 94, 323–328, https://doi.org/10.1016/s0306-4522(99)00255-9 (1999).
Thompson, J. D., Higgins, D. G. & Gibson, T. J. CLUSTAL W: improving the sensitivity of progressive multiple sequence alignment through sequence weighting, position-specific gap penalties and weight matrix choice. Nucleic Acids Res 22, 4673–4680, https://doi.org/10.1093/nar/22.22.4673 (1994).
Kumar, S., Stecher, G. & Tamura, K. MEGA7: Molecular Evolutionary Genetics Analysis Version 7.0 for bigger datasets. Mol. Biol. Evol. 33, 1870–1874, https://doi.org/10.1093/molbev/msw054 (2016).
Acknowledgements
We thank Dr. Kazuhiro Kobayashi for providing the brain samples of Shamo and Shaver Brown chickens. This study was partially supported by Tokai University School of Medicine, Project Research and Tokai University School of Medicine Research Aid.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
Conceptualisation and Methodology, T.K. and M.Y.; Data Analysis, T.K. and M.Y.; Gene Sequencing, K.Y.; Sequence assembly and alignment, T.K.; Validation, T.K. and H.K.; Original Draft Preparation, T.K.; Review and Editing, T.K. All authors read and approved the final draft of the manuscript for publication.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Komiyama, T., Yoshikawa, M., Yokoyama, K. et al. Analysis of the source of aggressiveness in gamecocks. Sci Rep 10, 7005 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-63961-1
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-63961-1
Comments
By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.