Abstract
Sexual selection favours traits that increase reproductive success via increased competitive ability, attractiveness, or both. Male rhesus macaque (Macaca mulatta) morphological traits are likely to reflect the effects of multiple sexual selection pressures. Here, we use a quantitative genetic approach to investigate the production and maintenance of variation in male rhesus macaque morphometric traits which may be subject to sexual selection. We collected measurements of body size, canine length, and fat, from 125 male and 21 female free-ranging rhesus macaques on Cayo Santiago. We also collected testis volumes from males. We used a genetic pedigree to calculate trait heritability, to investigate potential trait trade-offs, and to estimate selection gradients. We found that variation in most male morphometric traits was heritable, but found no evidence of trait trade-offs nor that traits predicted reproductive success. Our results suggest that male rhesus macaque morphometric traits are either not under selection, or are under mechanisms of sexual selection that we could not test (e.g. balancing selection). In species subject to complex interacting mechanisms of selection, measures of body size, weaponry, and testis volume may not increase reproductive success via easily-testable mechanisms such as linear directional selection.
Introduction
Sexual selection favours traits that increase reproductive success because they confer advantages in mating competition (intrasexual selection), mate choice (intersexual selection), or both1,2. Mating competition can take several forms, including direct contest competition (physical fights) and indirect competition2, whereby males do not compete through physical fights, but rather through mechanisms such as sperm competition (in which ejaculates compete within the female reproductive tract)3 and endurance rivalry (whereby males compete in endurance, by investing in mating effort over long periods of time)2. Where direct male-male mating competition (intrasexual competition) for access to fertile females is high, sexual selection promotes the evolution of traits such as large body size and weaponry (horns, antlers, or large canine teeth)4,5. Sperm competition selects for large testis volumes relative to body size and high levels of sperm production3,6,7. Where males compete through endurance rivalry, they invest in maintaining body condition2. Strong intersexual selection typically leads to the evolution of ornaments, like the bright coloration exhibited by many bird species2. These mechanisms do not act in isolation, however. An increasing number of studies have shown that intra- and intersexual selection may act on the same traits in a reinforcing or opposing manner (see Hunt et al.8). Furthermore, there is evidence for trade-offs between traits related to pre-copulatory selection (e.g., ornaments and weaponry) and post-copulatory selection (testis volume and ejuaculates)9,10,11,12.
The evolution of exaggerated male traits such as ornaments, large body size, weaponry, and large testis volumes, has been linked to sexual selection pressures across many species. Ornaments have been shown to impact male reproductive success (e.g., tail length and color in European barn swallow Hirundo rustica13, coloration in house finches Carpodacus mexicanus14, coloration in guppies Poecilia reticulata15). Weaponry and body size predict reproductive success in some (e.g., field crickets Gryllus bimaculatus16, American rubyspots Haeterina americana17, minnows Phoxinus phoxinus18, kangaroos Macropus giganteus19, red deer Cervus elaphus20, Soay sheep Ovis aries21, mandrills Mandrillus sphinx22), but not all (e.g., Atlantic cod Gadus morhua23, bighorn sheep Ovis canadensis24, and sifakas Propithecus verrauxi25) taxa, and there is some evidence that testis size influences offspring production as well (e.g., Soay sheep21, yellow-pine chipmunks Taimias amoneus26). Some male traits are influenced by both male-male competition and female choice (e.g., body size in carrion flies Prochylzia xanthostma27, body size in pond dragonflies Libellua luctosa28). Additionally, male morphology may be affected by evolutionary trade-offs between investment in traits associated with pre- and post-copulatory selection (e.g., weaponry and testes across cetacean species29, body mass and genital size across species of pinnipeds30, ornaments and testis volumes across primate species12, ornaments and sperm swimming speed in guppies31, hyoid volume and testis size in howler monkeys Alouatta spp.32).
Primates are a good taxonomic group in which to investigate how sexual selection pressures shape phenotypic variation. Sexual dimorphism in body and canine size33,34,35 and relative testis volume6 vary widely across species, suggesting variation in investment in both direct male-male competition and in sperm competition across the Order. The function of color ornaments also varies across species – some primarily function as “badges of status”36,37,38,39 and seem to have evolved under male-male competition, while others appear to be selected through both male-male competition and female choice (e.g., mandrills40, rhesus macaques41,42,43,44). However, the degree to which male-male competiton and female choice have influenced the evolution of primate morphological traits generally, or indeed whether primate morphological traits are evolving under any mechanism of sexual selection, remains largely unknown.
Rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) are a useful primate species in which to examine how sexual selection mechanisms influence male morphology because they appear to experience multiple sexual selection pressures. Rhesus macaques live in multi-male multi-female social groups and have a polygynandrous mating system45. Males acquire dominance through queueing rather than contest, such that the most dominant males are not always the strongest or highest quality in the group46. Relatedly, dominance rank is not usually a strong predictor of reproductive success among male rhesus macaques and reproductive skew is low in males as well47,48. Furthermore, there are multiple routes to reproductive success in rhesus males: some males engage in consortships while others employ alternative reproductive tactics, like sneaky matings, and both very passive and very aggressive males sire offspring49,50,51,52.
Rhesus macaque morphology most likely reflects the effect of a unique suite of sexual selection pressures. Rhesus macaques are moderately dimorphic in body mass (males are ~44% larger than females) and more strongly dimorphic in canine length (male canines are ~207% longer than female canines)53, reflecting investment in direct male-male competition. Male rhesus macaques also have large testis volumes for their body size, indicating a role for sperm competition, and they accumulate body fat prior to the mating season, enabling them to undertake costly mating strategies like consortships, as a form of endurance rivalry6,54,55. In additional to body fat, several other male traits show seasonal variation: males exhibit the largest testis volumes, highest androgen concentrations, and deepest facial coloration during the mating season56,57,58. Finally, male red facial coloration is attractive to females and influences reproductive success in high-ranking males41,42,43. To date however, whether most of these traits (with the exception of facial coloration41), are genetically inherited or influence reproductive success, and hence if they might be evolving under sexual selection – is unknown.
Here, we explored whether morphometric traits in male rhesus macaques are evolving under sexual selection. We quantified trait heritability (1), as variation in a trait must be heritable in order for the trait to respond to selection. We also investigated potential evolutionary and developmental trade-offs (2) between investment in different traits. We investigated the impact of seasonality (2a) on trait values, correlations between traits (2b) and correlations between dominance rank and trait values (2c). We predicted that: 2a) males captured later in the trapping season would be of greater body mass, higher in body fat, and exhibitlarger testis volumes, reflecting increasing investment in these traits leading up to the mating season; 2b) testis volume and measures of weaponry (body size and canine size) would be inversely correlated across males; and 2c) that dominance rank would not be correlated with trait values. Finally, we quantified selection on traits (3) by measuring relationships between trait values and reproductive success.
Results
Trait heritability
In our models using data from both parents, all measurements except testis volume were heritable (h2 ≥ 0.159, Table 1a). DIC values for models including the animal (heritability) random effect were lower than DIC values for all models excluding the animal term, indicating that the addition of the heritability term produced a better fitting model (Table 1a,b), even though the testis volume heritability estimate did not meet our threshold to be considered heritable. Testis volume heritability estimates were extremely low (h2 < 0.1), and about half of the variance in testis volume could be explained by the date the animals were measured (see more details below). Date measured was not a significant contributor to variance in any of the other traits. Confidence intervals for the heritability and maternal effects were wide, so we interpret the HDPI values with caution, given the very low lower limits of these intervals. None of our sex-linked heritability models converged.
Evolutionary and developmental trade-offs
Seasonality
Date measured influenced three of our traits of interest: body mass, testis volume, and crown-rump length (Table 2). Males measured later in the trapping period had higher body masses, longer crown-rump lengths, and larger testis volumes (Fig. 1). Date measured did not influence measurements of canine length or abdominal skinfold thickness. These results contrast with those from our heritability models, where we treated date measured as a random, rather than a fixed effect. In the heritability models, date measured only contributed to a large proportion of the variation in testis volume.
Correlations between traits
In our dataset, body mass was significantly correlated with crown-rump length, testis volume and abdominal skinfold thickness (Table 3, Supplementary Fig. S1). Testis volume (either absolute or relative) was not correlated with either abdominal skinfold thickness or canine length (Table 3). Because relationships involving testis volume did not change after controlling for body mass, we used absolute testis volume in all subsequent analyses. Age was a significant term in all of the models, and was always negative.
Correlations between dominance rank and trait values
There was no relationship between current ordinal dominance rank (low, medium, high) and any morphometric trait (ANOVA, p > 0.05, Supplementary Table S1).
Selection on traits
We found no definitive evidence for directional, stabilizing, disruptive, or correlational selection on any of our focal morphometric traits (Tables 4–6a,b). Neither age nor date measured influenced relationships between trait values and reproductive success. Dominance rank (as assumed from dispersal rate) had a minor effect in several of our models, but the term was not statistically significant (0.05 < p < 0.10).
Discussion
We used a quantitative genetic approach to investigate the production and maintenance of variation in male rhesus macaque morphometric traits putatively associated with intrasexual competition. Our results suggest that male morphometric traits are heritable, but that variation in these traits does not predict reproductive success. We also found that male morphometrics were not influenced by dominance rank, and we found no evidence for trade-offs in investment between morphometric traits.
For several traits, either additive genetic variance (heritability) or maternal ID contributed to a moderate proportion of the phenotypic variation in the trait. Canine length, body mass, and abdominal skinfold thickness all had moderate heritability values (h2 > 0.3), though crown-rump length values were lower (h2 < 0.2). We found very low additive genetic variance in testis volume. It is possible that intraindividual seasonal increases in testis volume have confounded our results. However, low additive genetic variance may also be the result of strong selection on testis volume, as strong selection may erode additive genetic variation60. Additionally, all of our estimates for heritability and maternal effects have wide confidence intervals, ranging from very low (0.02) to quite high (0.75) in some cases. Future studies with larger samples might help to resolve these values with greater confidence. Overall, our results suggest that variation in most male morphometric traits has a genetic basis, and as such, that these traits can evolve under selection.
We found no evidence for trade-offs between investment in different morphometric traits. Body mass was positively correlated with crown-rump length, testis volume, and abdominal skinfold thickness. These correlations likely reflect allometric relationships, not investment in multiple reproductive strategies, and are consistent with previous work on male morphometric traits in rhesus macaques61. We found no correlation between canine length and testis volume, and we also found that jointly, these traits did not explain variation in reproductive success, suggesting that inter-individual variation in male morphology does not reflect relatively higher investment in either direct or sperm competition. These findings do not support our prediction that testis volume would be inversely correlated with variation in body size (mass and length) and canine size. This finding is consistent with inter-specific analyses that have shown that species with lower levels of direct male-male competition do not exhibit strong trade-offs between investment in weaponry and investment in testis volume10 but contrasts with intra-specific studies of other taxa, which have clearly demonstrated trade-offs between traits that are involved in pre- and post-copulatory competition31,32,62. This finding may also reflect different patterns of investment in canines and testes: once canines are formed, no additional energetic investment is required – in contrast, testes need to be maintained throughout adult life12. We would need many data points collected across the lifespan of individual males in order to test this idea. Furthermore, our prediction that current dominance rank was not correlated with morphometrics was supported, providing further evidence that competitive ability is not important for dominance acquisition in this species. Lastly, these analyses revealed an effect of age on variation in morphology. Age was always a negative term in the models, suggesting that older animals are smaller than younger ones. We cannot determine whether this reflects the aging process, cohort effects, or selective mortality of particular male phenotypes with our data. Additional work is necessary to address whether smaller males are more likely to survive to older ages.
Our prediction that body mass, testis volume, and fat mass would be higher in males captured later during the capture-release period was partially supported. We found that males captured later had higher testis volumes than those captured earlier, which confirms prior work demonstrating that rhesus males undergo dramatic increases in testis volume prior to the mating season, indicating strong investment in sperm competition56. We also found that males trapped later in the trapping season had higher body masses and longer body lengths, but not higher fat mass. This finding may either reflect seasonal increases in body size or an effect of body size on a male’s ability to be captured. In order to test for seasonal increases in body size, we would need to collect data on body size in the months leading up to the mating season. Since it is not possible to trap animals on Cayo Santiago during this period, one possible way of doing this would be to collect multiple body length measurements per male (e.g., one measurement per week in the three months leading up to the mating season) using photogrammetric methods (e.g., Breuer et al.63; Wright et al.64). Alternatively, this result may reflect the fact that larger males happen to be trapped later than smaller ones because they are harder to capture.
We did not find evidence of selection on any of our morphometric traits. Our results echo those from Atlantic cod23, bighorn sheep24, and sifakas25 but contrast with those from mandrills22, Soay sheep21, red deer20, kangaroos19, minnows18, and field crickets16, among others. Our findings provide additional evidence that even in sexually dimorphic species, larger or more highly-weaponized males do not always enjoy the highest reproductive success. Rather, among rhesus macaques, female preference (such as that based on facial coloration41,43) and male behavioural strategies51,52 are likely stronger predictors of male reproductive success.
Our results could be interpreted in multiple different ways. One interpretation is that the measured male morphometric traits are not under selection. Alternatively, the results are also largely consistent with previous evidence of flat fitness landscapes and multiple routes to male reproductive success in male rhesus macaques51,52. Under these scenarios, we are unlikely to find clear linear or quadratic relationships between specific traits and reproductive success, because there are multiple routes to equal levels of success. We found weak, but not statistically-significant, evidence that male dominance rank influences reproductive success independent of variation in male morphometric traits, consistent with previous analyses of reproductive skew47. Because rhesus males do not contest dominance, this result provides further evidence that direct male-male competition is not a strong selection pressure in this species. In general, our results confirm prior research indicating that variation in morphological traits associated with competition is not a strong predictor of reproductive success in this species54,61. Additional studies are necessary to determine whether male reproductive strategies or aggression levels are correlated with morphology.
Our findings illustrate that in species that are evolving under multiple sexual selection pressures, such as rhesus macaques, male traits like large body size, enhanced weaponry, and large testis volumes may not increase reproductive success through linear, quadratic, or correlational selection. Our results highlight the importance of understanding how interactions between sexual selection pressures, and between behavior and morphology, function to influence male reproductive success.
Methods
Field site and subjects
Cayo Santiago is a 15.2 hectare island located off the southeast coast of Puerto Rico. The Caribbean Primate Research Center (CPRC) manages the island and the population of free-ranging rhesus macaques that live there65. At the time this study was conducted, the island was inhabited by ~1,500 rhesus macaques divided into seven naturally-formed social groups, all of which descend from a founding population of 409 animals brought to the island from India in 193865. Even though no outside animals have been introduced into the colony, the population is not inbred66. The CPRC monitors the population daily and maintains long-term (>75 years) behavioural and demographic databases including data on social group membership for all animals, plus a genetic parentage database for animals born after 198565,67,68. Each year, before the onset of the mating season, a subset of the animals ranging on the island are captured for collection of blood samples and morphometric data, and then released. During the capture-release period, all one-year-old animals are captured, sampled for blood, assigned a unique ID, and tattoed, enabling researchers to easily identify individual animals.
Morphometric data collection
We collected morphometric data from male and female rhesus macaques (n = 146) captured during the annual capture-release period (October 15, 2015 to December 15, 2015). Our sample is composed of all adult males (ages 6 and above) ranging on the island who were able to be captured (n = 125), but we also collected data on females closely related to the males we sampled (n = 21) for use in our heritability analyses. We collected a set of measurements on focal traits that could act as proxies for different types of male-male competition: crown-rump length (direct contest competition), body mass (direct contest competition and endurance rivalry), canine length (direct contest competition), testis dimensions (sperm competition), and upper abdominal skinfold thickness (endurance rivalry). We chose these traits based on prior studies of sexual selection in rhesus macaque males54,61. Animals were only captured and measured once during the capture-release period. All measurements were collected by one trained observer. Body weight was measured using a hanging scale and all other measurements were collected using either a tape measure or digital calipers (accurate to 0.01 mm). Weight (lbs) was converted to mass (kg) for all analyses. Testis volume was calculated from three dimensions: height (h), width (w), and depth (d) and modeled as an ellipsoid: \(V=\frac{4}{3}\pi hwd\) 69. We calculated relative testis volume by dividing testis volume by body mass. We excluded one measurement of an extremely worn or broken canines, one crown-rump length value that was three standard deviations above the mean and likely an error, and measurements affected by pathological conditions (n = 12) from our analyses.
Genetic parentage information
The CPRC maintains a pedigree database containing information on behavioural dams (available for all animals) as well as genetic parentage assignments for dams and sires (available for animals born after 1985). Genetic parentage assignments are made based on a panel of microsatellites48. We used the R package MasterBayes to prune the full Cayo Santiago pedigree so that it only included phenotyped animals and those individuals that provided connections between them70. Our pruned pedigree spanned ten generations and included 902 animals, with 885 maternities and 567 paternities (pedigree statistics were generated using the pedantics R package71).
We used average number of offspring produced per year as a proxy for male reproductive success. We then calculated relative annual reproductive success (each animal’s reproductive success divided by the average value across our entire sample) and used this measure in our selection gradient models72. We could not use lifetime reproductive success because the majority of our study animals had not yet reached reproductive senescence (>17 years of age48).
Dominance rank
We quantified dominance rank two different ways: first, over the course of one year and second, as an average measure over the animal’s life to date (up to when they were captured for morphometric measurements). We used current dominance rank (available for a subset of 55 adult males) to determine whether current rank and male morphology were correlated. We determined the dominance rank of all subjects using pairwise win-loss information from agonistic encounters that were recorded during focal animal samples or during ad libitum observations collected as part of an on-going, unrelated, study. We calculated dominance rank amongst males living within the same social group group. In order to account for variable group sizes, we then calculated dominance rank as the percentage of male groupmates that a subject outranked. We then classed males as either high, mid or low ranking based on this scale, with high ranking animals being those that outranked between 80–100% of males in their group, and low-ranking animals being those that outranked fewer than 49% of males in their group. We chose to bin the ranks this way because the behavioral data used to calculate these ranks are fairly coarse, so these categories are likely to be more accurate than continuous ranks, which may contain errors in exact rank order. Furthermore, the correlation between average dispersal rate and continuous dominance ranks, while strong, is not perfect, so binning the ranks makes the measures more comparable. Finally, this method has been used in previous studies on this population73,74 so conducting our analyses this way makes our study more comparable with prior work.
In our selection gradient analyses (n = 108), we chose to use an average measure of dominance rank – average annual dispersal rate – because our measure of reproductive success was also averaged over the animal’s life (up until they were captured)41 and we did not have the behavioral data necessary to calculate dominance ranks for many of the males in our sample. Dispersal rate is a good proxy for dominance rank because rhesus males acquire dominance through queuing instead of contest, such that a male’s dominance rank can be predicted by group tenure length46. In our dataset, dominance rank and tenure length were strongly correlated – males with longer tenure lengths were higher-ranked (Pearson’s product moment correlation = −0.579, n = 81, p < 0.001).
Statistical analyses
All statistical analyses were run in R version 3.5.275. We considered p-values to be significant if alpha levels were below 0.05. P-values for generalized linear models (GLMs) were calculated based on a Student t distribution, p-values for ANOVA models were calculated using the F distribution.
Trait heritability
We used animal models to estimate narrow-sense heritability values (h2) for our morphometric traits. Animal models are univariate generalized mixed models that combine phenotypic and pedigree data to parse out the contributions of additive genetic and environmental factors to variation in a trait76,77. We implemented our models in the R package MCMCglmm78,79. We ran models on a combined sample of both males and females for body mass, crown-rump length, canine length, and upper abdominal skinfold thickness. We included six-year old males in our canine heritability analyses, as canine formation and eruption is generally complete by age six80,81, while all other analyses were run on males age seven and above because body growth is generally not complete until age seven. We ran models on mean-scaled measurements. In the pooled sex models, we controlled for age and sex (fixed effects), plus maternal ID, animal ID, and date measured (random effects). We also ran models for testis volume; these included maternal ID, animal ID, and date measured (random effects) and age (fixed effect). Lastly, we ran models for males using only paternal pedigree data to test for sex-linked inheritance, using the same model structure as listed above. We included maternal ID to account for non-genetic differences in maternal care and date measured to account for any changes in morphology over the course of the capture-release period. Animal ID is used to calculate the additive genetic variance.
We ran models on each trait for 2,550,000 iterations with a burn-in period of 50,000 iterations and a thinning interval of 1,000. Although we ran our analyses using a range of prior types and structures, we report values from models with inverse Wishart priors (V = 1, nu = 0.2). Priors with lower values of nu (e.g., nu = 0.002) did not mix well – the chains were autocorrelated – and confidence intervals for random effect terms were very wide. In order to verify that models met assumptions regarding autocorrelation and convergence, we inspected plots of the MCMC chain, ran Heidelberg stationarity tests, and ensured that autocorrelation between estimates was less than 0.182,83. We calculated narrow-sense heritability values (h2) by dividing the proportion of variation due to additive genetic variance (VA; the posterior distribution of the animal effect) by the total phenotypic variance (VP; the summed posterior distribution of the maternal effect, date effect, and residual variance). We also ran a set of models without the animal ID term. We then compared DIC values from models with animal ID and those without – models with the lower DIC value were considered to be the best fit models.
Evolutionary and developmental trade-offs
First, we explored how seasonality may influence our morphometric traits of interest, as this has direct implications for our ability to detect trade-offs between traits. We investigated whether the date an animal was measured was related to variation in morphometrics using generalized linear models (GLMs). We set the trait as the response variable and age and date measured as fixed effects.
We then investigated whether males exhibited trade-off between traits associated with different mechanisms of competition using GLMs. We examined relationships between body mass and testis volume, body mass and abdominal skinfold thickness, abdominal skinfold thickness and testis volume (both relative and absolute), canine length and testis volume (both relative and absolute), and canine length and body mass, controlling for age and date measured.
We also explored whether current dominance rank (categorical: low, medium, high) was related to variation in male morphology (crown-rump length, body mass, canine length, testis volume, relative testis volume, and abdominal skinfold thickness) using ANOVA tests. We controlled for age, date measured and social group in our analyses.
Selection on traits
We assessed whether male trait variation predicted variation in reproductive success (measured as average annual offspring production) using selection gradient models84,85. We calculated linear selection gradients to estimate directional selection on single traits, quadratic selection gradients to estimate disruptive or stabilizing selection on single traits, and correlational selection gradients to determine if trait values in combination with average annual dominance rank influenced reproductive success. We ran linear selection gradients using mean-standardized trait values, quadratic selection gradients on squared mean-standardized trait values86, and correlational selection gradients using mean-standardized values of one trait multiplied by mean-standardized values of another trait87. We ran correlational models for trait values and average annual dominance rank, and for pairs of morphometric traits. We used the cube-root of body mass and testis volume in these correlational gradients so that both of our morphometric variables of interest were on the same scale. We controlled for age, dominance rank, and date measured (fixed effects) in our models. We square-root transformed age and average annual dominance rank so that models met assumptions (normally-distributed residuals), but did not transform reproductive success values84. We included all males age seven or above in our selection analyses (n = 108), regardless of whether or not they had produced an offspring.
Ethical statement
This work was conducted in accordance with the Animal Behavior Society guidelines. All protocols were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of the University of Puerto Rico (protocol A150116) and the University Animal Welfare Committee of New York University (protocol 14-1439).
Data availability
The datasets generated during and/or analyzed during the current study are available at https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.11343971.
References
Darwin, C. The descent of man and selection in relation to sex. (John Murray, 1871).
Andersson, M. B. Sexual selection. (Princeton University Press, 1994).
Simmons, L. W. & Fitzpatrick, J. L. Sperm wars and the evolution of male fertility. Reproduction 144, 519–534 (2012).
Plavcan, J. M. Sexual dimorphism in primate evolution. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 116, 25–53 (2001).
Emlen, D. J. The evolution of animal weapons. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 39, 387–413 (2008).
Harcourt, A. H., Harvey, P. H., Larson, S. G. & Short, R. V. Testis weight, body weight and breeding system in primates. Nature 293, 55–57 (1981).
Møller, A. P. Ejaculate quality, testes size and sperm production in mammals. Funct. Ecol. 91–96 (1989).
Hunt, J., Breuker, C. J., Sadowski, J. A. & Moore, A. J. Male–male competition, female mate choice and their interaction: determining total sexual selection. J. Evol. Biol. 22, 13–26 (2009).
Parker, G. A., Lessells, C. M. & Simmons, L. W. Sperm competition games: a general model for precopulatory male-male competition: male premating competition and sperm allocation. Evolution 67, 95–109 (2013).
Lüpold, S., Tomkins, J. L., Simmons, L. W. & Fitzpatrick, J. L. Female monopolization mediates the relationship between pre- and postcopulatory sexual traits. Nat. Commun. 5, 3184 (2014).
Simmons, L. W., Lüpold, S. & Fitzpatrick, J. L. Evolutionary trade-off between secondary sexual traits and ejaculates. Trends Ecol. Evol. 32, 964–976 (2017).
Lüpold, S., Simmons, L. W. & Grueter, C. C. Sexual ornaments but not weapons trade off against testes size in primates. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 286, 20182542 (2019).
Costanzo, A. et al. Lifetime reproductive success, selection on lifespan, and multiple sexual ornaments in male European barn swallows. Evolution 71, 2457–2468 (2017).
McGraw, K. J., Stoehr, A. M., Nolan, P. M. & Hill, G. E. Plumage redness predicts breeding onset and reproductive success in the House Finch: a validation of Darwin’s theory. J. Avian Biol. 32, 90–94 (2001).
Gordon, S. P. et al. Selection analysis on the rapid evolution of a secondary sexual trait. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 282, 20151244 (2015).
Simmons, L. W. Male size, mating potential and lifetime reproductive success in the field cricket, Gryllus bimaculatus (De Geer). Anim. Behav. 36, 372–379 (1988).
Serrano-Meneses, M. A., Córdoba-Aguilar, A., Méndez, V., Layen, S. J. & Székely, T. Sexual size dimorphism in the American rubyspot: male body size predicts male competition and mating success. Anim. Behav. 73, 987–997 (2007).
Jacob, A., Evanno, G., Renai, E., Sermier, R. & Wedekind, C. Male body size and breeding tubercles are both linked to intrasexual dominance and reproductive success in the minnow. Anim. Behav. 77, 823–829 (2009).
Miller, E. J., Eldridge, M. D. B., Cooper, D. W. & Herbert, C. A. Dominance, body size and internal relatedness influence male reproductive success in eastern grey kangaroos (Macropus giganteus). Reprod. Fertil. Dev. 22, 539 (2010).
Kruuk, L. E. et al. Antler size in red deer: heritability and selection but no evolution. Evolution 56, 1683–1695 (2002).
Preston, B. T., Stevenson, I. R., Pemberton, J. M., Coltman, D. W. & Wilson, K. Overt and covert competition in a promiscuous mammal: the importance of weaponry and testes size to male reproductive success. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 270, 633–640 (2003).
Leigh, S. R., Setchell, J. M., Charpentier, M., Knapp, L. A. & Wickings, E. J. Canine tooth size and fitness in male mandrills (Mandrillus sphinx). J. Hum. Evol. 55, 75–85 (2008).
Rakitin, A., Ferguson, M. & Trippel, E. Male reproductive success and body size in Atlantic cod Gadus morhua L. Mar. Biol. 138, 1077–1085 (2001).
Poissant, J., Wilson, A. J., Festa-Bianchet, M., Hogg, J. T. & Coltman, D. W. Quantitative genetics and sex-specific selection on sexually dimorphic traits in bighorn sheep. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 275, 623–628 (2008).
Lawler, R. R., Richard, A. F. & Riley, M. A. Intrasexual selection in Verreaux’s sifaka (Propithecus verreauxi verreauxi). J. Hum. Evol. 48, 259–277 (2005).
Schulte-Hostedde, A. I. & Millar, J. S. Intraspecific variation of testis size and sperm length in the yellow-pine chipmunk (Tamias amoenus): implications for sperm competition and reproductive success. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 55, 272–277 (2004).
Bonduriansky, R. & Rowe, L. Interactions among mechanisms of sexual selection on male body size and head shape in a sexually dimorphic fly. Evolution 57, 2046–2053 (2003).
Moore, A. J. The evolution of sexual dimorphism by sexual selection: the separate effects of intrasexual selection and intersexual selection. Evolution 44, 315–331 (1990).
Dines, J. P. et al. A trade-off between precopulatory and postcopulatory trait investment in male cetaceans. Evolution 69, 1560–1572 (2015).
Fitzpatrick, J. L., Almbro, M., Gonzalez-Voyer, A., Kolm, N. & Simmons, L. W. Male contest competition and the coevolution of weaponry and testes in pinnipeds: reproductive trait evolution in pinnepids. Evolution 66, 3595–3604 (2012).
Evans, J. P. Quantitative genetic evidence that males trade attractiveness for ejaculate quality in guppies. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 277, 3195–3201 (2010).
Dunn, J. C. et al. Evolutionary trade-off between vocal tract and testes dimensions in howler monkeys. Curr. Biol. 25, 2839–2844 (2015).
Plavcan, J. M. & van Schaik, C. P. Intrasexual competition and canine dimorphism in anthropoid primates. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 87, 461–477 (1992).
Plavcan, J. M. & van Schaik, C. P. Intrasexual competition and body weight dimorphism in anthropoid primates. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 103, 37–68 (1997).
Plavcan, J. M., van Schaik, C. P. & Kappeler, P. M. Competition, coalitions and canine size in primates. J. Hum. Evol. 28, 245–276 (1995).
Setchell, J. M. & Wickings, E. J. Dominance, status signals and coloration in male mandrills (Mandrillus sphinx). Ethology 111, 25–50 (2005).
Marty, J. S., Higham, J. P., Gadsby, E. L. & Ross, C. Dominance, coloration, and social and sexual behavior in male drills Mandrillus leucophaeus. Int. J. Primatol. 30, 807–823 (2009).
Bergman, T. J., Ho, L. & Beehner, J. C. Chest color and social status in male geladas (Theropithecus gelada). Int. J. Primatol. 30, 791–806 (2009).
Grueter, C. C. et al. Sexually selected lip colour indicates male group-holding status in the mating season in a multi-level primate society. R. Soc. Open Sci. 2, 150490 (2015).
Setchell, J. M. Sexual selection and the differences between the sexes in Mandrills (Mandrillus sphinx). Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 159, S105–29 (2016).
Dubuc, C. et al. Sexually selected skin colour is heritable and related to fecundity in a non-human primate. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 281, 20141602 (2014).
Dubuc, C. et al. Who cares? Experimental attention biases provide new insights into a mammalian sexual signal. Behav. Ecol. 27, 68–74 (2016).
Dubuc, C., Allen, W. L., Maestripieri, D. & Higham, J. P. Is male rhesus macaque red color ornamentation attractive to females? Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 68, 1215–1224 (2014).
Petersdorf, M., Dubuc, C., Georgiev, A. V., Winters, S. & Higham, J. P. Is male rhesus macaque facial coloration under intrasexual selection? Behav. Ecol. 28, 1472–1481 (2017).
Melnick, D. J., Pearl, M. C. & Richard, A. F. Male migration and inbreeding avoidance in wild rhesus monkeys. Am. J. Primatol. 7, 229–243 (1984).
Manson, J. H. Do female rhesus macaques choose novel males? Am. J. Primatol. 37, 285–296 (1995).
Dubuc, C., Muniz, L., Heistermann, M., Engelhardt, A. & Widdig, A. Testing the priority-of-access model in a seasonally breeding primate species. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 65, 1615–1627 (2011).
Dubuc, C., Ruiz-Lambides, A. & Widdig, A. Variance in male lifetime reproductive success and estimation of the degree of polygyny in a primate. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 25, 878–889 (2014).
Berard, J. D., Nurnberg, P., Epplen, J. T. & Schmidtke, J. Alternative reproductive tactics and reproductive success in male rhesus macaques. Behaviour 129, 177–201 (1994).
Dubuc, C., Muniz, L., Heistermann, M., Widdig, A. & Engelhardt, A. Do males time their mate-guarding effort with the fertile phase in order to secure fertilisation in Cayo Santiago rhesus macaques? Horm. Behav. 61, 696–705 (2012).
Brent, L. J. et al. Genetic origins of social networks in rhesus macaques. Sci. Rep. 3, 1042 (2013).
Higham, J. P. & Maestripieri, D. The costs of reproductive success in male rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) on Cayo Santiago. Int. J. Primatol. 35, 661–676 (2014).
Plavcan, J. M. Sexual selection, measures of sexual selection, and sexual dimorphism in primates. In Sexual Selection in Primates: New and Comparative Perspectives (eds Kappeler, P. & Van Schaik, C.) 230–252 (Cambridge University Press, 2004).
Bercovitch, F. B. Reproductive strategies of rhesus macaques. Primates 38, 247–263 (1997).
Higham, J. P., Heistermann, M. & Maestripieri, D. The energetics of male–male endurance rivalry in free-ranging rhesus macaques, Macaca mulatta. Anim. Behav. 81, 1001–1007 (2011).
Sade, D. S. Seasonal cycle in size of testes of free-ranging Macaca mulatta. Folia Primatol. 2, 171–180 (1964).
Baulu, J. Seasonal sex skin coloration and hormonal fluctuations in free-ranging and captive monkeys. Horm. Behav. 7, 481–494 (1976).
Higham, J. P., Heistermann, M. & Maestripieri, D. The endocrinology of male rhesus macaque social and reproductive status: a test of the challenge and social stress hypotheses. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 67, 19–30 (2013).
Brent, L. J. N. et al. Personality traits in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) are heritable but do not predict reproductive output. Int. J. Primatol. 35, 188–209 (2014).
Fisher, R. A. The genetical theory of natural selection. (Clarendon Press, 1930).
Bercovitch, F. B. & Nürnberg, P. Socioendocrine and morphological correlates of paternity in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). J. Reprod. Fertil. 107, 59–68 (1996).
Somjee, U., Miller, C. W., Tatarnic, N. J. & Simmons, L. W. Experimental manipulation reveals a trade-off between weapons and testes. J. Evol. Biol. 31, 57–65 (2018).
Breuer, T., Robbins, A. M., Boesch, C. & Robbins, M. M. Phenotypic correlates of male reproductive success in western gorillas. J. Hum. Evol. 62, 466–472 (2012).
Wright, E. et al. Male body size, dominance rank and strategic use of aggression in a group-living mammal. Anim. Behav. 151, 87–102 (2019).
Kessler, M. J. & Rawlins, R. G. A 75-year pictorial history of the Cayo Santiago rhesus monkey colony. Am. J. Primatol. 78, 6–43 (2016).
Widdig, A. et al. Low incidence of inbreeding in a long-lived primate population isolated for 75 years. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 71, 18 (2017).
Rawlins, R. G. & Kessler, M. J. The Cayo Santiago macaques: history, behavior, and biology. (SUNY Press, 1986).
Widdig, A. et al. Genetic studies on the Cayo Santiago rhesus macaques: A review of 40 years of research. Am. J. Primatol. 78, 44–62 (2016).
Harcourt, A. H., Purvis, A. & Liles, L. Sperm competition: mating system, not breeding season, affects testes size of primates. Funct. Ecol. 9, 468–476 (1995).
Hadfield, J. D., Richardson, D. S. & Burke, T. Towards unbiased parentage assignment: combining genetic, behavioural and spatial data in a Bayesian framework. Mol. Ecol. 15, 3715–3730 (2006).
Morrissey, M. B. & Wilson, A. J. Pedantics: an r package for pedigree‐based genetic simulation and pedigree manipulation, characterization and viewing. Mol. Ecol. Resour. 10, 711–719 (2010).
Arnold, S. J. & Wade, M. J. On the measurement of natural and sexual selection: applications. Evolution 38, 720–734 (1984).
Mandalaywala, T. M., Higham, J. P., Heistermann, M., Parker, K. J. & Maestripieri, D. Physiological and behavioural responses to weaning conflict in free-ranging primate infants. Anim. Behav. 97, 241–247 (2014).
Hoffman, C. L., Higham, J. P., Mas-Rivera, A., Ayala, J. E. & Maestripieri, D. Terminal investment and senescence in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) on Cayo Santiago. Behav Ecol 21, 972–978 (2010).
R Development Core Team. R: a language and environment for statistical computing. (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, 2018).
Wilson, A. J. et al. An ecologist’s guide to the animal model. J. Anim. Ecol. 79, 13–26 (2010).
Kruuk, L. E. Estimating genetic parameters in natural populations using the ‘animal model’. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 359, 873–890 (2004).
Hadfield, J. D. MCMC methods for multi-response generalized linear mixed models: the MCMCglmm R package. J. Stat. Softw. 33, 1–22 (2010).
Hadfield, J. D. & Nakagawa, S. General quantitative genetic methods for comparative biology: phylogenies, taxonomies and multi-trait models for continuous and categorical characters. J. Evol. Biol. 23, 494–508 (2010).
Trotter, M., Hixon, B. B. & MacDonald, B. J. Development and size of the teeth of Macaca mulatta. Am. J. Anat. 150, 109–127 (1977).
Wang, Q. Dental maturity and the ontogeny of sex-based differences in the dentofacial complex of rhesus macaques from Cayo Santiago. In Bones, Genetics, and Behavior of Rhesus Macaques 177–194 (Springer, 2012).
De Villemereuil, P. Tutorial: estimation of a biological trait heritability using the animal model: How to use the MCMCglmm R package (2012).
Hadfield, J. MCMCglmm Course Notes (2016).
Lande, R. & Arnold, S. J. The measurement of selection on correlated characters. Evolution 37, 1210–1226 (1983).
Morrissey, M. B. & Sakrejda, K. Unification of regression-based methods for the analysis of natural selection. Evolution 67, 2094–2100 (2013).
Stinchcombe, J. R., Agrawal, A. F., Hohenlohe, P. A., Arnold, S. J. & Blows, M. W. Estimating nonlinear selection gradients using quadratic regression coefficients: double or nothing? Evolution 62, 2435–2440 (2008).
Brodie, E. D. III, Moore, A. J. & Janzen, F. J. Visualizing and quantifying natural selection. Trends Ecol. Evol. 10, 313–318 (1995).
Benjamini, Y. & Hochberg, Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. B Methodol. 57, 289–300 (1995).
Acknowledgements
We thank Victoria Johnson for assistance with data collection, and the staff of the Caribbean Primate Research Center for trapping the animals and for their help with access to the study population. The content of this publication is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the University of Puerto Rico or the Office of Research Infrastructure Programs of The National Institutes of Health. This work was supported by a MacCracken Fellowship from New York University (NYU) to C.M.K., The National Institute of Mental Health (grant numbers R01-MH089484 and R01-MH096875), and a Leakey Foundation grant to C.D. The Cayo Santiago field station is supported by the University of Puerto Rico and the Office of Research Infrastructure Programs of The National Institutes of Health (grant number 2 P40 OD012217).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
C.M.K. performed analyses and drafted the manuscript, C.D. designed research and revised the manuscript, L.J.N.B. contributed data and revised the manuscript, and J.P.H. designed research and revised the manuscript. All authors approved the final version of the manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Kimock, C.M., Dubuc, C., Brent, L.J.N. et al. Male morphological traits are heritable but do not predict reproductive success in a sexually-dimorphic primate. Sci Rep 9, 19794 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-52633-4
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-52633-4
This article is cited by
-
Same-sex sociosexual behaviour is widespread and heritable in male rhesus macaques
Nature Ecology & Evolution (2023)
-
Heritability and developmental plasticity of growth in an oviparous lizard
Heredity (2023)
-
Immune cell composition varies by age, sex and exposure to social adversity in free-ranging Rhesus Macaques
GeroScience (2023)
-
Endocranial volume is variable and heritable, but not related to fitness, in a free-ranging primate
Scientific Reports (2021)
Comments
By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.