Abstract
Optimal foraging theory predicts that when food is plentiful all individuals should take a small range of preferred prey types, but as competition increases less preferred prey will be included in the diet. This dietary switching may not be uniform among individuals, which produces discrete dietary clusters. We tested this hypothesis for gentoo penguins at Bird Island, South Georgia, using stable isotope analysis and biologging. Competition, in the form of the density of foraging dives, increased markedly from incubation to chick-rearing owing to increased foraging effort. Birds responded behaviourally by exploiting a greater portion of the available foraging radius and increasing dive depths. Dietary niche width doubled and two discrete dietary clusters appeared; one comprising birds that consumed mostly krill and another that ate a greater proportion of demersal fish. There were no differences in morphology between the dietary classes, but birds in the fish class had a tendency to dive deeper, which suggests a behavioural basis for specialization. Our findings are consistent with the hypothesis that intra-specific competition expands the population’s dietary niche width and drives divergence in diets among individuals.
Introduction
Optimal foraging theory predicts that, when resources are abundant, animals should select a small number of preferred prey types that provide the highest nutritional return for time and energy expended1,2. As the availability of these prey declines, alternative sub-optimal prey should be incorporated into the diet such that individual and population niche widths expand2. This dietary niche expansion is expected to occur equally across individuals, such that both population and individual niche widths become broader1. However, evidence is emerging that individuals may not exhibit uniform dietary responses to reductions in the availability of preferred prey, such that they diverge into discrete dietary clusters3. The decision to join a cluster may be driven by morphology4, social dominance5 or behaviour6 which result in the rankings in the optimality of prey types differing among individuals7,8.
Intraspecific competition has the potential to reduce the abundance or accessibility of preferred prey types and hence produce increased dietary variation, both among individuals and across the population7. Increases in population niche width and individual specialization in response to elevated intraspecific competition have been convincingly demonstrated in experiments that manipulated the densities of lacustrine fish in enclosures3,9,10. In wild populations, studying the interplay between competition and diets depends upon correlative studies where per-capita availability of food varies through space11 or time6,12. Potential confounding arises where increased diversity of prey types gives rise to higher predator densities, such that variability in niche width and individual specialization might arise from an “ecological opportunity” to exploit different prey types7 rather than competition. Information on spatial or temporal variation in prey availability is required to disentangle these processes which can be challenging to obtain, particularly in the marine environment.
An alternative approach is to examine the diets of colonial central place foraging birds, which all share an equal opportunity to feed over the same spatial area, such that variability among individuals must arise from different foraging strategies rather than differences in spatio-temporal availability of resources13. The occurrence of large numbers of predators foraging within a maximum radius from the central place often results in high levels of intra-specific competition in the form of interference or prey depletion14. Evidence for this comes from animals breeding at larger colonies tending to have greater foraging ranges15,16, higher energy expenditure17 and lower adult condition or chick growth rates18 compared to smaller ones. Abrupt increases in the intensity of intra-specific competition often occurs at hatching when parents are obliged to increase foraging effort to meet the demands of the growing chick19, which comprises a natural experiment to test the effects of competition upon dietary niche widths.
Caution is required when attributing changes in diet at hatching to increased competition as other aspects of foraging ecology of seabirds may change simultaneously. The majority of seabird species reduce their foraging range in order to provide their chick with the frequent meals required to sustain growth and survival, which alters levels of competition and affects the accessibility of offshore vs. inshore prey types20. Diets may also switch at hatching owing to parents provisioning chicks with different prey to that upon which they feed themselves: possible mechanisms include parents ingesting small prey whilst carrying larger items to chicks in their bills21, or parents alternating long self-feeding trips with short chick-feeding trips22. Studies seeking to isolate the effects of competition upon diet therefore need to be directed at species that have consistently short foraging ranges throughout the breeding season and feed their chicks by regurgitation.
Gentoo penguins Pygoscelis papua represent an ideal model for the study of the effects of competition upon dietary niche breadth and intra-population diet variability. Like all penguins they feed chicks by regurgitation but, unusually, they have consistently short, inshore foraging ranges throughout the year23. Compared to congeners with greater foraging ranges, gentoo penguins have small colony sizes24 and are more susceptible to breeding failure during years of low prey availability25, which are indicative of high levels of density-dependence that arises from intra-specific competition for food. They are dietary generalists at the population level, feeding on a mixture of crustaceans, fish and occasionally cephalopods and typically have broader dietary niches than other penguin species breeding at the same location23. Prey preference shows striking spatiotemporal variability at regional, local, annual and seasonal scales in response to the availability of different prey species23,26. Despite their high trophic plasticity, the diets of individuals sampled at the same site and time are often dominated by different prey types27 and these individual preferences may show consistency within and across breeding seasons28,29,30, suggesting gentoo penguins form generalist populations of specialized individuals. A study of gentoo penguins on the Kerguelen Islands found that trip range and dive depths increase with body mass of individuals, which suggests a morphological basis for such specialization30.
This paper examines the changes in diet and foraging behaviour in a colony of gentoo penguins at Bird Island, South Georgia, across the incubation and chick-rearing stages. Changes in diet in terms of the proportion of crustaceans vs. fish are inferred from nitrogen stable isotope ratios, while foraging behaviour is described by tracking birds with GPS and time-depth recorders (TDR). We use this information to examine support for the following hypotheses: (H1) at-sea density and hence intra-specific competition following hatching will increase abruptly owing to birds spending more time at sea without increasing foraging ranges; (H2) birds will reduce this competition by diving deeper to expand their spatial niche width in the vertical dimension; (H3) the population dietary niche width will broaden and discrete dietary clusters of individuals will be formed and (H4) these clusters will be related to differences in individual morphology.
Results
Sample sizes
The study resulted in 53 gentoo penguins being equipped with TDRs; one was initialised incorrectly and two more did not go to sea before recapture, resulting in 50 TDR datasets (25 in each stage) which recorded a total of 217 foraging trips and 23,092 dives, all of which were used to assess variation in time budgets and dive depths. Blood could not be obtained from two birds despite attempts on both flippers. Blood congealed before centrifuging in 16 cases and did not separate into fractions, resulting in a sample of 32 birds (16 in each stage) for which plasma was available. GPS tags were deployed on 30 of the TDR-equipped birds and 21 (9 and 12 during incubation and guard, respectively) were recovered along with dive data (producing a total of 84 trips during which 10,901 dives were made). Three failures were caused by the problems mentioned above for the TDRs, and an additional six by malfunctions or flooding of the TrackTags.
Time budgets
There were no significant effects of bird weight (LR = 1.29, df = 1, p = 0.27) or being equipped with a GPS logger (LR = 1.09, df = 1, p = 0.22) on trip durations. The duration of foraging trips was significantly longer during chick-rearing than incubation (LR = 12.19, df = 1, p < 0.0001). Trips during incubation lasted for 291 ± 0.91 (SE) minutes and those during chick rearing were 25 ± 1.08 minutes longer. The random individual effect accounted for 33.5% of the variation remaining once the fixed effects were explained. Based on relationships between travel time and distance metrics (Fig. 1), the average distance travelled during a trip was 16.40 ± 0.01 km during incubation and increased by 3.01 ± 0.03 km during chick-rearing, while the average maximum distance reached from the colony during a trip was 5.40 ± 0.01 km during incubation and increased by 0.82 ± 0.02 km during chick rearing. The greatest distance reached from the colony across all birds and trips, based on observed GPS locations, was 26.7 km during incubation and 37.8 km during chick-rearing.
The relationship between (a) the square root of the total distance travelled during a trip (km) and (b) the square root of the maximum distance reached from the colony (km) in relation to the trip duration (mins). Distance travelled (D) increased with trip duration (t) according to the equation: D = 1.52 + 0.009 √t, while maximum distance from the colony (M) increased with trip duration according to the equation: M = 0.77 + 0.005 √t. The relationships are highly significant (distance travelled: F = 544.9, df = 1, P < 0.0001; maximum distance: F = 342.1, df = 1, P < 0.0001).
There was no significant effect of bird weight (LR = 0.01, df = 1, p = 0.97) or being equipped with a GPS logger (LR = 0.13, df = 1, p = 0.67) on the proportion of time at sea, but stage of the breeding season had a highly significant effect (LR = 35.97, df = 1, p < 0.0001). The proportion of time spent at sea was 0.162 ± 0.02 during incubation but more than doubled to 0.360 ± 0.03 during chick-rearing. These values result in daily energy expenditures of 405 kJ day kg−1 during incubation, and 483 kJ day kg−1 during chick-rearing: a 19% increase. The rate of diving whilst at sea differed significantly among stages of the season (LR = 84.82, df = 1, p < 0.0001), being higher during incubation (15.39 ± 1.01 dives per hour) than during chick-rearing (13.62 ± 1.01).
Spatial variation in foraging activity
There were no significant effects of bird weight (LR = 1.44, df = 1, p = 0.28) or being equipped with a GPS unit (LR = 1.45, df = 1, p = 0.26) upon dive depths. The effect of the stage of the breeding season on the mean and variation in dive depth was highly significant (LR = 3296.14, df = 2, p < 0.0001). Dives were on average 36.4 m ± 2.13 deep during incubation and 17.7 m ± 3.09 deeper and 1.8 times more variable during chick rearing. The autocorrelation coefficient for successive dives within trips was 0.47. The random individual effect accounted for 10.0% of the variation while that of trip accounted for 17.6%. The deepest dive recorded was 173 m.
The spatial distribution and density of foraging dives both increased from incubation to chick rearing. During incubation, dives occupied an area of 137 km2 and were concentrated on inshore waters to the immediate south and west of Bird Island and in two prongs to the north and south of the Willis Islands (Fig. 2a). During chick rearing, these areas were again used most intensively, but the total area used increased five-fold to 702 km2 and extended across a wider portion of the available radius around the island (Fig. 2b). Despite the expansion in the foraging area, the average foraging intensity during chick-rearing within the area used during incubation still increased from 60.9 to 76.6 dives per 0.005° grid square (Fig. 2c). The exceptions were those cells over the shallows around the south and west coasts of Bird Island and the reefs to the northwest, which were used less during chick-rearing compared to incubation (Fig. 2c).
Number foraging dives per day within 0.005 decimal degrees grid cells by gentoo penguins from the Landing Beach and Square Pond colonies during (a) incubation, (b) chick-rearing and (c) the difference between the two (chick-rearing minus incubation densities). Maps were created by the authors using R version 3.4.2 (R Core Team. R: A language and Environment for Statistical Computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. https://www.R-project.org (2017)).
Diet
The stable isotope ratios for gentoo penguin plasma during incubation and chick-rearing and of different prey types are shown in Table 1 and Fig. 3. Clear differences were evident in the δ15N ratios of fish and krill, while δ13C of krill was very variable and overlapped that of fish. Painted noties Lepidonotothen larseni had somewhat higher δ13C ratios than mackerel icefish Champsocephalus gunnari although there was some overlap between them. Gentoo penguins had similar δ15N ratios to the fish species while that of δ13C was variable, tending to be higher during chick-rearing than during incubation (Fig. 1).
Stable isotope ratios (‰) of gentoo penguins at Bird Island, South Georgia, and their prey. Symbol fills represent gentoo incubation (gray), gentoo chick-rearing diet Class 1 (white), gentoo chick-rearing diet Class 2 (red), Antarctic krill (pink), mackerel icefish (blue) and painted notie (yellow). The bidirectional error bars of the prey values represent 1 SD.
The effect of stage on the mean and variability of δ15N was highly significant (LR = 18.9, df = 1, p = 0.0001). The model estimated that δ15N was 9.41‰ ± 0.13 during incubation and was 0.78‰ ± 0.21 higher and 2.08 times more variable during chick rearing. This represents an increase in the trophic level at which birds were feeding and a doubling of the dietary niche width. The Gaussian finite mixing model found support for two distinct classes in the δ15N data; Class 1 comprising 26 samples with a mean of 9.54‰ ± 0.49 and Class 2 comprising six samples with a mean of 11.31‰ ± 0.49. All of the six samples in Class 2 were collected during the chick rearing stage and so comprised 38% of birds sampled in this period (Fig. 3).
The stable isotope mixing model showed that the proportion of crustaceans in the diet was higher during incubation (0.93; 95% credible intervals = 0.87, (1) than during chick-rearing (0.75; 0.63, 0.87). The proportion of crustaceans in the diet based on stomach content analysis during chick rearing in February 2010 was 0.62, which is at the mixing model’s 95% lower credible interval. When samples were grouped according to the classifications by the Gaussian finite mixing model the proportion of crustaceans in the diet of Class 1 birds was higher (0.92; 0.86, 0.99) than that of Class 2 birds (0.53; 0.37, 0.69).
Relationships between diet class, behaviour and morphology
Diet class had a significant effect upon dive depths (LR = 6.822, df = 1, p = 0.006), with members of the fish class on average diving 10.68 m ± 4.04 deeper than those of the crustacean class. Variance components analysis showed that diet class explained 34% of the variability in dive depths among individuals but a mere 3% of the combined random and residual variation. There were no differences in trip durations (LR = 0.14, df = 1, p = 0.71), body mass (LR = 3.19, df = 1, p = 0.08) or bill dimensions (LR = 0.57, df = 1, p = 0.4) between the two diet classes.
Discussion
Tracking of animals with devices has proliferated in the past two decades, and studies need to test the effects of equipping birds with tags for both ethical reasons and to assess bias in the data collected. The effects of devices on behaviour typically increase with weight and drag31, so the experimental deployment of minute TDRs or accelerometers offers a powerful means of testing the effects of larger tracking devices on behaviour whilst animals are unobservable at sea32,33. Using this approach, we found no support for the hypothesis that GPS devices affected any of the behavioural variables studied, which provides confidence that the parameters presented here are representative of those for unequipped birds in the population.
The stable isotope mixing model estimated a somewhat higher proportion of crustaceans in the diet than did analysis of stomach contents, although these were sampled later in the chick rearing season than the blood samples such that the differences might be due to temporal changes in diet rather than methodology. Other studies of penguin diets have found that stable isotopes provide comparable estimates of diet composition to stomach contents34,35, so we surmise that the diet composition inferred from our isotope mixing model is broadly accurate.
Parent gentoo penguins have to increase their foraging effort to meet the food demands of their chicks after hatching, and achieve this by increasing the frequency of foraging trips whilst only increasing trip durations to a modest degree19. Many seabird species extend foraging ranges to compensate for high conspecific foraging densities, but gentoo penguins are obligate inshore foragers23 and so their at-sea densities are prone to increase markedly whenever foraging effort increases. In our study, birds attempted to reduce at-sea densities during chick rearing by foraging over a greater proportion of their available foraging radius, but densities, and hence intra-specific competition, increased across almost all cells despite this. The only cells in which density declined were those used most intensively during incubation, suggesting these might have been avoided during chick-rearing due to previous depletion of prey.
Adult gentoo penguins eat approximately 1.1 kg of prey per day and chicks require 60 kg over the whole fledging period36, so over the course of the 137 day breeding season the 1,933 pairs and 1,604 chicks present on the whole of Bird Island in 2009 would have consumed 680 metric tonnes of prey from an area of approximately 1,000 km2. Depletion is therefore plausible in the absence of high rates of prey flux. Interference competition is unlikely to arise as krill avoid predation by diel vertical movement37,38, and gentoo penguins diving abilities exceed the water depth across most of their foraging range around Bird Island. This contrasts with studies of species with more offshore distributions where interference was believed to be the key driver of intra-specific competition16,39. In concert, these findings are consistent with an increase in at-sea density, intra-specific competition and possibly prey depletion occurring after hatching, which provides support for Hypothesis 1.
The weak compensation for increased foraging density by increasing the foraging range would create a strong selective pressure for niche expansion along other axes. The mean dive depth and variability of dive depths increased during hatching compared to incubation, which indicates that birds were feeding across a wider range of the water column, thus providing support for Hypothesis 2. Similarly, thick-billed murres Uria lomvia dived deeper during chick rearing than during incubation40 and as prey was depleted through the course of the chick-rearing period39. Deeper diving has the potential to either reduce foraging densities in the vertical axis or allow access to alternative deeper-dwelling prey species41,42, either of which would reduce intra-specific competition. However, deeper dives are less efficient owing to time spent commuting from the surface and in post-dive recovery, so the benefits of performing them in terms of prey intake have to be sufficient to outweigh these costs43.
The dietary niche width of gentoo penguins doubled during chick-rearing compared to incubation due to a switch to fish. Long-term monitoring shows gentoo penguins switch increasingly to mackerel icefish as krill availability declines44, and that breeding success is higher in years when diet is krill-dominated27. This indicates that krill is the preferred prey and fish are only taken as an alternative when its availability declines. Optimal foraging theory suggests that animals should feed on preferred prey items until intra-specific competition reduces their availability, after which they should switch to less preferred prey types1,2. The finding that fish only became important in the diet after hatching when intra-specific competition increased is consistent with this theory. The wider population niche was due to some individuals switching to fish, rather than all birds adopting mixed diets, which produced two distinct dietary clusters comprising krill and fish specialists and provides support for Hypothesis 3. The individual clusters indicates that dietary niche did not merely broaden in order to meet specific nutritional requirements of chicks, otherwise all individuals would be expected to have switched diets in a similar manner post-hatching. Intra-specific competition has also been found to produce dietary clusters of individuals in sticklebacks Gasterosteus aculeatus3 and food limitation results in greater dietary diversification among individual sea otters Enhydra lutris11.
Two alternative explanations for the observed changes in diets need to be explored before drawing conclusions regarding the effects of competition. The first is that of “ecological opportunity” in which different prey species become accessible across different stages of the season7. Changes in trip durations among breeding stages can affect diets via altered accessibility of inshore vs. offshore prey fields in some seabird species20, but this does not apply to gentoo penguins as they forage inshore throughout the year. It is also possible that a decline in the abundance of krill, or increase in that of fish, occurred within the gentoo penguin’s foraging radius and happened to coincide with hatching. This might occur through advection, but flux of krill in currents is low in the inshore areas to the west of South Georgia45, while fish of the species and age classes taken by gentoo penguins are resident in shallow shelf waters over reefs and kelp beds46,47. Wider-scale declines in krill stocks might also explain the observed patterns, but observations of predator diet and demography27 are consistent with a recruitment event occurring during 2009 which would have resulted in krill stocks around South Georgia increasing rather than declining.
The second possible explanation is that inter-specific competition might reduce krill stocks and cause dietary divergence7, since gentoo penguins share Bird Island with large colonies of macaroni penguins Eudyptes chrysolophus and Antarctic fur seals Arctocephalus gazella that consume substantial amounts of krill48. However, gentoo penguins have unique foraging niches among Antarctic predators which isolate them from high levels of inter-specific competition49,50,51. We conclude that the weight of evidence points to intra-specific competition being the most likely explanation for the observed patterns of dietary switching and divergence by gentoo penguins.
We were unable to quantify individual specialization in the diet data sensu stricto as we had no repeated measures of blood samples from individual birds. Nonetheless, we found discrete dietary clusters in the stable isotope ratios that represent specialization over at least the seven-day period during which stable isotope ratios in blood plasma are integrated. Previous studies have found evidence for high variability in isotope ratios among, and consistency within, individual gentoo penguins28,30. Modest levels of between-individual variability were also evident in trip durations and dive depths: values were similar to those found on the Kerguelen Islands, although unlike that study we found no relationship between individual variation and body mass30. Individual specialization has also been documented in the diets or foraging behaviour of a wide range of air-breathing marine vertebrates52,53,54.
The weight and bill dimensions of birds did not differ among dietary classes suggesting that there was no morphological basis for dietary divergence, which leads us to reject Hypothesis 4. Likewise, there was no association between diet class membership and morphology in thick-billed murres55. Dive depths for gentoo penguins in our study averaged 10 m deeper for members of the fish diet class than for those in the crustacean class which points to a behavioural basis of specialization. Associations between foraging behaviour and diet have been documented in other seabird species56,57. A previous study of gentoo penguins at Bird Island also found that birds feeding on fish dived deeper than those feeding on crustaceans, and concluded this was a consequence of krill being pelagic and the fish species being benthic41. However, this is an oversimplification, since variability in bathymetry and vertical diel movements of both krill and fish can result in substantial overlap in the depths at which they occur37,46. This is likely to explain why specialization in fish or krill only explained a small proportion of the overall variation in gentoo penguin dive depths.
In conclusion, it is widely acknowledged that the restricted foraging radii of gentoo penguins leads to them having small colony sizes and a breeding success that is highly sensitive to variations in food supply27,58. Our study reveals the behavioural mechanisms underpinning these observations: high foraging effort during chick-rearing depletes prey in their restricted foraging range and birds attempt to compensate for this by increasing the time spent at sea, expanding the portion of the foraging radius they utilise, making deeper dives and diversifying diets. These compensatory mechanisms will be overwhelmed where large colony sizes or environmentally-induced reductions in prey availability produce particularly high levels of intra-specific competition for food, resulting in breeding failure and population regulation.
Methods
Study site and fieldwork methods
The study was conducted between Dec 2009 and Feb 2010 at Landing Beach, Bird Island, South Georgia (−54.01°S; −38.05°W). This beach hosts a colony of 120 pairs of gentoo penguins and is a sub-colony of the larger one at Square Pond (270 pairs and 500 m away). Hatching peaked in the first week of January 2010. To place the study season in context, the gentoo penguin numbers on Bird Island were low compared to the previous and subsequent year while breeding success was moderate (BAS, unpublished data). The low numbers are likely to be due to low Antarctic krill (Euphausia superba; hereafter krill) stocks in 2008/09 which caused complete breeding failure and elevated adult overwinter mortality59. Acoustic stock assessments to the NW of South Georgia showed krill densities were moderate in 2009, but estimates are unavailable for 200860. These patterns are consistent with moderate krill recruitment into South Georgia waters the spring of 2009.
All methods were carried out in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations, as described in the permits from the Government of South Georgia and the South Sandwich Islands and the BAS Animal Ethics Panel. Birds leaving their nests were captured using a net before being weighed to the nearest 0.1 kg using spring balance (Pesola, Schindellegi, Switzerland) and having their bill length and depth measured to the nearest 0.1 mm using dial callipers. The nest’s contents were recorded to classify the birds as being at the incubation or chick rearing stage at the start of the deployment. All birds were equipped with G5 standard time-depth recorders (TDR; CEFAS Technology Ltd, Lowestoft, UK), which were attached to the feathers of the lower back using two-part epoxy resin and waterproof tape (Tesa, Hamburg, Germany). These devices weigh 2.7 g in air (1.3 g in water) and have a diameter of 8 mm and length of 31 mm. The average mass of equipped birds was 6.3 kg (SD = 0.75) so the additional load of the logger was trivial. We initialised the loggers to start recording both pressure and temperature every second, starting at 04:00 in the morning so they were logging as birds departed the colony to forage at dawn.
To study horizontal movements of birds we additionally equipped a sub-sample of birds with rapid-acquisition GPS loggers (Fastloc 1; Sirtrack Havelock, New Zealand and TrackTag; Navsys, Edinburgh, UK, the latter enclosed in custom housings manufactured by Bangor University, UK). These weighed 120 g (1.9% of bird weight) with dimensions of 11 cm L, 5 cm W and 2 cm D, and featured saltwater switches and sub-second fix acquisition times to enable positions to be estimated during short periods at the surface between dives. We initialised the loggers to collect positions every 3 minutes and synchronised their clocks and start times with those of the TDR loggers.
Equipped birds were released and then recaptured between four and eleven days later, during which time they completed at least two foraging trips. Upon recapture, the devices were removed and the birds were weighed again. A ~1 ml blood sample was then taken from the brachial vein using a hypodermic needle and syringe, which was transferred to an Eppendorf tube and taken immediately to the laboratory (~5 min walk) for centrifuging and decanting into plasma and cell fractions. These samples were then frozen at −20 °C. Birds were sampled only once over the course of the season owing to ethical considerations of subjecting individuals to these relatively intrusive procedures twice in one season.
Stable isotope analysis
Plasma and cell samples were freeze-dried and subsequently homogenised using an electronic mixer mill (Retsch, MM200). Samples of ~1 mg were weighed into tin capsules and analysed for δ13C and δ15N ratios. All isotope analyses were carried out at the Scottish Universities Environmental Research Centre, East Kilbride, UK. Analysis was done by continuous-flow isotope ratio mass spectrometer (CF-IRMS) using a Costech Elemental Analyser (EA) and Thermo Finnigan Delta Plus XP Mass Spectrometer. Laboratory standards were analysed for every 10 unknown samples, correcting for any instrument drift. Stable isotope ratios were expressed in δ notation as parts per thousand (‰) deviation from the international standards Vienna PeeDee Belemnite (carbon) and air (atmospheric nitrogen), according to the following equation δ X = [(R sample/R standard) − 1] × 1000, where X is 15N or 13C and R is the corresponding ratio 15N/14N or 13C/12C. Measurement precision of both δ 15N and δ 13C was <0.69‰ and 0.50‰ respectively.
Logger data analysis
Dive and time-budget statistics were extracted from the TDR data using the R package diveMove61. The depth data were zero offset corrected to define the sea-surface, dives were identified using a 2 m depth threshold and the maximum depth was extracted for each. The start and ends of trips were identified by inspection of dive and temperature records. This information was used to derive trip durations and the proportion of time spent at sea for each individual.
We used linear mixed effects models, implemented in the R package nlme62, to analyse dive depths, trip durations (square root transformed) and proportion of time at sea (arcsine transformed). Explanatory fixed effects tested were stage of the season (incubation or chick-rearing) and bird mass (as a proxy for both allometric and sex effects, since gentoo penguins exhibit sexual size dimorphism63). We included a fixed factor of whether the bird was equipped with a GPS logger to test for device effects. Random effects were trip nested within individual for dive depth, individual for trip duration and none for proportion of time spent at sea, reflecting the different hierarchical sampling of each variable. In the case of dive depth, we also fitted a first-order autocorrelation term within trips to account for the fact that sequential dives tended to reach similar depths64. We used a model selection process based on backward stepwise deletion and ANOVA tests. Dive rate per hour was modelled in relation to stage of the season as the number of dives per trip using Poisson errors with the natural log of the trip duration (in hours) as an offset65. We converted the proportion of time at sea into energy expenditure using equations in Bevan et al.19 that were derived from gentoo penguins at Bird Island during the summers of 1991/92 and 1992/93.
The locations of dives along the GPS tracks were modelled using the R package CRAWL66. This is a random correlated walk model fitted in a state-space framework, allowing interpolation of the path followed between GPS fixes, with uncertainty. We produced 100 possible locations for each dive made by each individual penguin. These were gridded for all birds and trips (at a cell size of 0.005 decimal degrees) to produce a maps of dive densities by sampled birds during incubation and chick rearing. The estimated dive density per cell per day was calculated as the product of the proportion of dives made within a cell, the number of breeding birds in the Landing Beach and Square Pond colonies (pairs multiplied by two), the number of hours they spent at sea per day (the proportion of time at sea multiplied by 24) and the dive rate per hour.
Stable isotope data analysis
δ15N is a proxy for the trophic level at which animals feed67. Gentoo penguins at South Georgia mostly feed on Antarctic krill or one- and two-year old mackerel icefish and painted notie27 which themselves feed upon krill47. The fish are therefore a trophic level higher than crustaceans so δ 15N provides information on consumption of these broad dietary components35. δ15N in penguin blood cells and plasma have different turnover rates of approximately 32 and seven days, respectively67. The slow turnover rate in cells would have resulted in chick-stage samples collected within a month of hatching containing isotope ratios from incubation20. We therefore focussed on analysis of the plasma samples only.
General least squares (gls) models fitted in the R package nlme were used to test the fixed effects of stage on average δ15N ratios and their variance. We used a finite Gaussian mixtures model to examine evidence for discrete dietary clusters in the δ15N data, implemented in the R package mclust68. This approach has been used previously to identify dietary classes from isotope ratio data in penguins20. Relationships between diet class (as identified by the mclust analysis) and behavioural traits were investigated by including class as a fixed factor in the previously identified best-fit models of dive depths and trip durations. Variance components analysis was used to investigate the proportion of the individual variance in behaviour that was explained by diet class13. Relationships between diet class and the body mass and bill dimensions (bill length × bill depth) were investigated using gls models with class as an explanatory factor.
The diet composition during each stage, dietary cluster and individual was inferred from δ15N using Bayesian mixing models implemented in the R package SIAR69. We used a two-source mixing model to estimate the proportion of the diet comprising crustaceans35. Prey items were collected from gastric lavage of 38 adult gentoo penguins on Bird Island during Jan and Feb 2010 were sorted into the lowest possible taxonomic order and weighed. The contents expressed as percentage wet mass of crustacean in each stomach sample to allow comparisons with diet composition estimated by the stable isotope mixing model. Sample specimens were collected from stomach-flushed samples collected in July-Sep 2009 (one per bird) were delipidated, freeze dried and milled, and used to estimate the mean and SD of δ15N for krill and fish. We used a trophic enrichment factor of 2.7 in the mixing model to adjust for increases in δ 15N of prey following ingestion and assimilation by penguins70.
References
Stephens, D. W. & Krebs, J. R. Foraging theory. (Princeton University Press, 1986).
Pianka, E. R. In Theoretical ecology (ed May, R. M.) 114–141 (Blackwell, 1976).
Araújo, M. S. et al. Network analysis reveals contrasting effects of intraspecific competition on individual vs. population diets. Ecology 89, 1981–1993, https://doi.org/10.1890/07-0630.1 (2008).
Svanbäck, R. & Eklöv, P. Morphology dependent foraging efficiency in perch: a trade‐off for ecological specialization? Oikos 102, 273–284, https://doi.org/10.1034/j.1600-0706.2003.12657.x (2003).
Sol, D. et al. Ecological mechanisms of a resource polymorphism in Zenaida Doves of Barbados. Ecology 86, 2397–2407, https://doi.org/10.1890/04-1136 (2005).
Kobler, A., Klefoth, T., Mehner, T. & Arlinghaus, R. Coexistence of behavioural types in an aquatic top predator: a response to resource limitation? Oecologia 161, 837–847, https://doi.org/10.1007/s00442-009-1415-9 (2009).
Araújo, M. S., Bolnick, D. I. & Layman, C. A. The ecological causes of individual specialisation. Ecol. Lett. 14, 948–958, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1461-0248.2011.01662.x (2012).
Bolnick, D. I. et al. The ecology of individuals: Incidence and implications of individual specialization. Am. Nat. 161, 1–28, https://doi.org/10.1086/343878 (2003).
Svanbäck, R. & Bolnick, D. I. Intraspecific competition drives increased resource use diversity within a natural population. Proc. Roy. Soc. B 274, 839–844, https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2006.0198 (2007).
Huss, M., Byström, P. & Persson, L. Resource heterogeneity, diet shifts and intra-cohort competition: effects on size divergence in YOY fish. Oecologia 158, 249–257, https://doi.org/10.1007/s00442-008-1140-9 (2008).
Tinker, M. T., Bentall, G. & Estes, J. A. Food limitation leads to behavioral diversification and dietary specialization in sea otters. PNAS 105, 560–565, https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0709263105 (2008).
Svanbäck, R., Rydberg, C., Leonardsson, K. & Englund, G. Diet specialization in a fluctuating population of Saduria entomon: a consequence of resource or forager densities? Oikos 120, 848–854, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0706.2010.18945.x (2011).
Ratcliffe, N. et al. The roles of sex, mass and individual specialisation in partitioning foraging-depth niches of a pursuit-diving predator. Plos One 8, e79107, https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0079107 (2013).
Ashmole, N. P. The regulation of numbers of tropical oceanic birds. Ibis 103b, 458–473, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1474-919X.1963.tb06766.x (1963).
Ainley, D. G. et al. Geographic structure of Adélie Penguin populations: overlap in colony-specific foraging areas. Ecol. Monogr. 74, 159–178, https://doi.org/10.1890/02-4073 (2004).
Lewis, S., Sherratt, T. N., Hamer, K. C. & Wanless, S. Evidence of intra-specific competition for food in a pelagic seabird. Nature 412, 816–819, https://doi.org/10.1038/35090566 (2001).
Ballance, L. T., Ainley, D. G., Ballard, G. & Barton, K. An energetic correlate between colony size and foraging effort in seabirds, an example of the Adélie penguin Pygoscelis adeliae. J. Avian Biol. 40, 279–288, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-048X.2008.04538.x (2009).
Gaston, A. J. & Hipfner, J. M. Adult Brunnich’s Guillemots Uria lomvia balance body condition and investment in chick growth. Ibis 148, 106–113 (2006).
Bevan, R. M., Butler, P. J., Woakes, A. J. & Boyd, I. L. The energetics of Gentoo Penguins, Pygoscelis papua, during the breeding season. Funct. Ecol. 16, 175–190, https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-2435.2002.00622.x (2002).
Horswill, C. et al. Drivers of intrapopulation variation in resource use in a generalist predator, the macaroni penguin. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 548, 233–247, https://doi.org/10.3354/meps11626 (2016).
Wilson, L. J., Daunt, F. & Wanless, S. Self-feeding and chick provisioning diet differ in the Common Guillemot Uria aalge. Ardea 92, 197–207 (2004).
Weimerskirch, H. et al. Alternate long and short foraging trips in pelagic seabird parents. Anim. Behav. 47, 472–476 (1994).
Ratcliffe, N. & Trathan, P. A review of the diet and at-sea distribution of penguins breeding within the CAMLR Convention Area. CCAMLR Sci. 19, 75–114 (2012).
Woehler, E. J. The distribution and abundance of Antarctic and Subantarctic Penguins. (SCAR, Cambridge, UK, 1993).
Croxall, J. P., Reid, K. & Prince, P. A. Diet, provisioning and productivity responses of marine predators to differences in availability of Antarctic krill. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 177, 115–131, https://doi.org/10.3354/meps177115ER (1999).
Handley, J. M., Connan, M., Baylis, A. M. M., Brickle, P. & Pistorius, P. Jack of all prey, master of some: Influence of habitat on the feeding ecology of a diving marine predator. Mar. Biol. 164, 82, https://doi.org/10.1007/s00227-017-3113-1 (2017).
Waluda, C. M., Hill, S. L., Peat, H. J. & Trathan, P. N. Long term variability in the diet and reproductive performance of penguins at Bird Island, South Georgia. Mar. Biol. 164, 39, https://doi.org/10.1007/s00227-016-3067-8 (2017).
Polito, M. J. et al. Contrasting specialist and generalist patterns facilitate foraging niche partitioning in sympatric populations of Pygoscelis penguins. Mar.Ecol. Prog. Ser. 519, 221–237, https://doi.org/10.3354/meps11095ER (2015).
Jablonski, B. The diet of penguins on King George Island, South Shetland Islands. Acta Zool. Cracov 29, 117–186 (1985).
Camprasse, E. C. M., Cherel, Y., Bustamante, P., Arnould, J. P. Y. & Bost, C. A. Intra- and inter-individual variation in the foraging ecology of a generalist subantarctic seabird, the gentoo penguin. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser., https://doi.org/10.3354/meps12151 (2017).
Ropert-Coudert, Y., Knott, N., Chiaradia, A. & Kato, A. How do different data logger sizes and attachment positions affect the diving behaviour of little penguins? Deep-Sea Res. II 54, 415–423, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dsr2.2006.11.018 (2007).
Chivers, L. S., Hatch, S. A. & Elliott, K. H. Accelerometry reveals an impact of short-term tagging on seabird activity budgets. Condor 118, 159–168, https://doi.org/10.1650/CONDOR-15-66.1 (2016).
Ludynia, K. et al. Evaluating the impact of handling and logger attachment on foraging parameters and physiology in southern rockhopper penguins. PLoS ONE 7, e50429, https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0050429 (2012).
Tierney, M., Southwell, C., Emmerson, L. M. & Hindell, M. A. Evaluating and using stable-isotope analysis to infer diet composition and foraging ecology of Adélie penguins Pygoscelis adeliae. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 355, 297–307, https://doi.org/10.3354/meps07235 (2008).
Polito, M. J. et al. Integrating stomach content and stable isotope analyses to quantify the diets of Pygoscelid penguins. PLoS ONE 6, e26642, https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0026642 (2011).
Robertson, G., Green, B. & Newgrain, K. Estimated feeding rates and energy-requirements of gentoo penguins, Pygoscelis papua, at Macquarie Island. Polar Biol. 9, 89–93, https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00442034 (1988).
Wilson, R. P. et al. Diel dive depth of penguins in relation to diel vertical migration of prey: whose dinner by candlelight? Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 94, 101–104, https://doi.org/10.2307/24832699 (1993).
Schmidt, K. et al. Seabed foraging by Antarctic krill: Implications for stock assessment, bentho‐pelagic coupling, and the vertical transfer of iron. Limnol. Oceanogr. 56, 1411–1428, https://doi.org/10.4319/lo.2011.56.4.1411 (2011).
Elliott, K. H. et al. Central-place foraging in an Arctic seabird provides evidence for Storer-Ashmole’s halo. Auk 126, 613–625, https://doi.org/10.1525/auk.2009.08245 (2009).
Benvenuti, S., Dall’Antonia, L. & Falk, K. Diving behaviour differs between incubating and brooding Brunnich’s guillemots, Uria lomvia. Polar Biol. 25, 474–478, https://doi.org/10.1007/s00300-002-0372-0 (2002).
Croxall, J. P., Davis, R. W. & O’ Connell, M. J. Diving patterns in relation to diet of gentoo and macaroni penguins at South Georgia. Condor 90, 157–167, https://doi.org/10.2307/1368444 (1988).
Woo, K. J., Elliott, K. H., Davidson, M., Gaston, A. J. & Davoren, G. K. Individual specialization in diet by a generalist marine predator reflects specialization in foraging behaviour. J. Anim. Ecol. 77, 1082–1091, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2656.2008.01429.x (2008).
Zimmer, I. et al. Dive efficiency versus depth in foraging emperor penguins. Aquat. Biol. 8, 269–277, https://doi.org/10.3354/ab00213 (2010).
Hill, S. L., Reid, K. & North, A. W. Recruitment of mackerel icefish (Champsocephalus gunnari) at South Georgia indicated by predator diets and its relationship with sea surface temperature. Can. J. Fish. Aquat. Sci. 62, 2530–2537, https://doi.org/10.1139/f05-157 (2005).
Young, E. F., Thorpe, S. E., Banglawala, N. & Murphy, E. J. Variability in transport pathways on and around the South Georgia shelf, Southern Ocean: Implications for recruitment and retention. J. Geophys. Res. 119, 241–252, https://doi.org/10.1002/2013JC009348 (2014).
Frolkina, G. A., Konstantinova, M. P. & Trunov, I. A. Composition and characteristics of ichthyofauna in pelagic waters of South Georgia (Subarea 48.3). CCAMLR Sci. 5, 125–164 (1998).
Frolkina, Z. A. Distribution of mackerel icefish (Champsocephalus gunnari)(Channichthyidae) around South Georgia at various stages of its life cycle. CCAMLR Sci. 9, 49–69 (2002).
Boyd, I. L. Estimating food consumption of marine predators: Antarctic fur seals and macaroni penguins. J. Appl. Ecol. 39, 103–119, https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-2664.2002.00697.x (2002).
Mori, Y. & Boyd, I. L. Segregation of foraging between two sympatric penguin species: does rate maximisation make the difference? Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 275, 241–249, https://doi.org/10.3354/meps275241ER (2004).
Wilson, R. P. Resource partitioning and niche hyper-volume overlap in free-living Pygoscelid penguins. Funct. Ecol. 24, 646–657, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2435.2009.01654.x (2010).
Kokubun, N., Takahashi, A., Mori, Y., Watanabe, S. & Shin, H.-C. Comparison of diving behavior and foraging habitat use between chinstrap and gentoo penguins breeding in the South Shetland Islands, Antarctica. Mar. Biol 157, 811–825, https://doi.org/10.1007/s00227-009-1364-1 (2010).
Ceia, F. R. & Ramos, J. A. Individual specialization in the foraging and feeding strategies of seabirds: a review. Mar. Biol. 162, 1923–1938 (2015).
Tinker, M. T., Mangel, M. & Estes, J. A. Learning to be different: acquired skills, social learning, frequency dependence, and environmental variation can cause behaviourally mediated foraging specializations. Evol. Ecol. Res. 11, 841–869 (2009).
Weise, M. J., Harvey, J. T. & Costa, D. P. The role of body size in individual-based foraging strategies of a top marine predator. Ecology 91, 1004–1015, https://doi.org/10.1890/08-1554.1 (2010).
Provencher, J. F., Elliott, K. H., Gaston, A. J. & Braune, B. M. Networks of prey specialization in an Arctic monomorphic seabird. J. Avian Biol. 44, 551–560, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-048X.2013.05717.x (2013).
Elliott, K. H. et al. Seabird foraging behaviour indicates prey type. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 354, 289–303, https://doi.org/10.3354/meps07221 (2008).
Deagle, B. E., Gales, N. J. & Hindell, M. A. Variability in foraging behaviour of chick-rearing macaroni penguins Eudyptes chrysolophus and its relation to diet. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 359, 295–309, https://doi.org/10.3354/meps07307 (2008).
Croxall, J. P. & Rothery, P. In Penguins: Ecology and Management (eds Daan, P., Norman, I. & Reilly, P.) 26–38 (Surrey Beatty and Sons, 1995).
Xavier, J. C. et al. Sexual and individual foraging segregation in gentoo penguins Pygoscelis papua from the Southern Ocean during an abnormal winter. Plos one 12, e0174850, https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174850 (2017).
Fielding, S. et al. Interannual variability in Antarctic krill (Euphausia superba) density at South Georgia, Southern Ocean: 1997–2013. ICES J. Mar. Sci 71, 2578–2588, https://doi.org/10.1093/icesjms/fsu104 (2014).
Luque, S. P. Diving Behaviour Analysis in R. R News 7, 8–14 (2007).
Pinheiro, J., Bates, D., DebRoy, S., Sarkar, D. & Team, R. C. nlme: Linear and Nonlinear Mixed Effects Models. R package version 3.1–131.1, https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=nlme (2018).
Agnew, D. J. & Kerry, K. R. In The Penguins: Ecology and Management (eds Daan, P., Norman, I. & Reilly, P.) Ch. 15, 299–318 (Surrey Beatty and Sons, 1995).
Zuur, A. F., Ieno, E. N., Walker, N. J., Saveliev, A. A. & Smith, G. M. Mixed effects models and extensions in R. (Springer, 2009).
Crawley, M. J. Statistical Computing. An introduction to data analysis using S-Plus. (Wiley, 2002).
Johnson, D. S., London, J. M., Lea, M.-A. & Durban, J. W. Continuous-time correlated random walk model for animal telemetry data. Ecology 89, 1208–1215, https://doi.org/10.1890/07-1032.1 (2008).
Cherel, Y., Hobson, K. A., Guinet, C. & Vanpe, C. Stable isotopes document seasonal changes in trophic niches and winter foraging individual specialization in diving predators from the Southern Ocean. J. Anim. Ecol. 76, 826–836, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2656.2007.01238.x (2007).
Fraley, C., Raftery, A. E., Murphy, B. & Scrucca, L. mclust Version 4 for R: Normal Mixture Modeling for Model-Based Clustering, Classification, and Density Estimation. (Department of Statistics, University of Washington, 2012).
Parnell, A. C., Inger, R., Bearhop, S. & Jackson, A. L. Source partitioning using stable isotopes: coping with too much variation. Plos one 5, e9672, https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0009672 (2010).
Cherel, Y., Hobson, K. A. & Hassani, S. Isotopic discrimination between food and blood and feathers of captive penguins: Implications for dietary studies in the wild. Physiol. Biochem Zool. 78, 106–115, https://doi.org/10.1086/425202 (2005).
Acknowledgements
This study is part of the British Antarctic Survey Polar Science for Planet Earth Programme, funded by the Natural Environment Research Council. Stable isotope analysis was conducted under a grant awarded by the Scottish Universities Environmental Research Centre node of the NERC Life Sciences Mass Spectrometry Facility in East Kilbride. We thank Charles Bishop and his technicians at Bangor University for manufacturing the housings for the Navsys GPS loggers. Jose Xavier kindly provided the nitrogen isotope ratio means and SDs for prey species that were used in the mixing models and Table 1. Derren Fox and Samantha Gwyn-Williams assisted with the fieldwork. Devin Johnson and Sebastian Luque provided valuable advice on use of the CRAWL and diveMove packages, respectively. Phil Trathan provided helpful advice during the planning stages while Richard Phillips, Iain Staniland and Rocio Moreno provided insightful comments on an earlier draft.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
N.R. conceived the study, conducted the fieldwork, performed statistical analysis of the data and drafted the manuscript. S.A. assisted with the fieldwork and G.S., R.M.G. prepared and analysed the blood samples. All authors contributed to the writing of the paper.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing Interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Ratcliffe, N., Adlard, S., Stowasser, G. et al. Dietary divergence is associated with increased intra-specific competition in a marine predator. Sci Rep 8, 6827 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-25318-7
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-25318-7
This article is cited by
-
Pellet analysis evidences flexibility in the diet of Brown Skua (Stercorarius antarcticus) during breeding at Esperanza/Hope Bay (Antarctic Peninsula)
Polar Biology (2022)
-
Stable Isotope Values in South American Fur Seal Pup Whiskers as Proxies of Year-round Maternal Foraging Ecology
Marine Biology (2020)
Comments
By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.