Introduction

During the course of evolution, the echolocation systems of bats have adapted to deliver information necessary to successfully perform species-specific tasks. These tasks depend on the foraging habitat, foraging strategy, and the type of prey bats feed on1,2,3. Bats of the “narrow space flutter detecting foragers” guild, comprising Rhinolophidae, Hipposideridae, and the phylogenetically distant Pteronotus parnellii of the family Mormoopidae, actively hunt for insects in narrow spaces where the prey echo either overlaps with or is masked by background echoes1,2,3,4,5. Flutter detecting foragers have evolved a highly specialized echolocation system to find fluttering insect prey between background targets. They emit signals which consist of a long constant frequency (CF) component followed by a shorter frequency modulated (FM) part (Fig. 1b). Hipposiderids emit shorter signals than rhinolophids and P. parnellii (reviewed in4,6). Signals are emitted with a high duty cycle; therefore, these bats are also called high duty cycle bats7,8. The CF-FM signals have the highest amplitude in the second harmonic. The frequency of the CF component of the second harmonic (CF2) is species-specific, but varies slightly between individuals [e.g. refs9,10,11,12]. In stationary bats, the CF2-frequency is kept almost constant and is referred to as the resting frequency (Frest) (reviewed in4)13.

Figure 1
figure 1

Sonogram and oscillogram (512 FFT, blackman) of an echolocation sequence in flight (a) with representative signals (bd). The bat (HA 2) started to fly at the 1st arrow, the 2nd arrow indicates the beginning of the terminal approach, and the 3rd arrow the time of landing. Sonograms, oscillograms, and averaged power spectra of representative signals of a resting signal (b), an echolocation signal during the orientation flight (c), and the terminal approach (d). The CF and FM component of the signal are marked in (b). (c) and (d) are taken from the echolocation sequence shown in (a) and marked with asterisks. The oscillogram in (d) was amplified by a factor of two.

When searching for prey, rhinolophids emit one long signal per wing beat, whereas P. parnellii often emits groups of two and hipposiderids emit groups with more signals of shorter duration4,9,14,15,16.

If a long CF-FM signal encounters a flying insect, the movement of the insect’s wings induces frequency and amplitude modulations in the CF component of the returning echoes dependent on the rhythm of the wing beat. Every time the wing is perpendicular to the impinging sound wave, an amplitude glint is produced which is up to 20 dB above the amplitude of the echo from the insect body. Simultaneously, the moving wings also produce a spectral glint, which encodes the direction of the wing movement. The generated glint pattern contains flutter information about the prey species in terms of the wing beat frequency, wing structure, wing length and size, and aspect angle17,18,19,20,21,22,23, and allows the discrimination of modulated prey echoes from unmodulated background echoes.

When flying, flutter detecting foragers lower the emission frequency to compensate for Doppler shifts (DS) of echoes from stationary targets ahead which are generated by the bat’s own movement. Doppler shift compensation (DSC) ensures that the returning echo frequency (Fecho) is adjusted for the auditory fovea, an area on the basilar membrane of the cochlea with a highly expanded frequency representation centred around the reference frequency (Fref)24,25,26,27,28,29. Fref is measured in flying bats as the average Fecho30 and is always a few hundred Hertz above Frest. The afferent projections from the enlarged area on the basilar membrane lead to foveal areas in the brain with an overrepresentation of sharply tuned neurons with best frequencies around Fref (P. parnellii31, R. ferrumequinum32, H. speoris33, H. armiger34). These neurons are very sensitive to amplitude and frequency modulations contained in the echoes from fluttering insects. Long CF-FM signals emitted at a high duty cycle, DSC, and an auditory fovea are adaptations for the detection and evaluation of echoes containing specific flutter information within unmodulated background echoes (reviewed in4)35,36,37,38,39.

In stationary rhinolophids and P. parnellii, the CF2-frequency is kept almost constant and varies only by approximately 0.1–0.2% or 200 Hz around Frest within short time periods. In hipposiderids variations of up to 0.75% or 1.17 kHz have been reported4,40, suggesting that the CF2-frequency of hipposiderids may be less stable. Over periods of minutes, days, and months, distinct changes in Frest have been observed in rhinolophids, hipposiderids, and P. parnellii41,42,43,44,45,46. In P. parnellii, Frest declined by up to 120–300 Hz in experiments where bats flew freely or were swung on a pendulum, and up to 366 Hz over a 50-day period41,42,43. In rhinolophids, Frest decreased gradually by 220 Hz within a three-month period, with variations of up to 2 kHz45. In a hipposiderid bat, maximal variations of Frest of up to 4.8 kHz and 3.0 kHz on average were reported in an experiment lasting four years44.

DSC was studied in the laboratory in bats flying to a landing bar or when sitting on a pendulum, and in playback experiments. DSC has been described by the difference between Fref and Frest, referred to as the offset4,30, and the precision with which Fecho is kept at Fref4. Rhinolophids and P. parnellii maintained Fecho with a high precision of only 0.1–0.2% around Fref, while in hipposiderids a much higher variation of 0.4–0.7% around Fref has been reported4. In flight, Rhinolophus ferrumequinum, Rhinolophus euryale, and P. parnellii accurately maintain Fref at approximately 150–200 Hz above Frest9,13,14,16,47,48,49,50,51. The hipposiderid, Asellia tridens, also maintains Fref 150–200 Hz above Frest15, whereas for some species of the genus Hipposideros an offset of up to 300 to 600 Hz was reported4. DSC was also shown in Hipposideros terasensis with no further information regarding the Fref or offset52. In a passive situation, e.g. when sitting on a pendulum or in playback experiments, rhinolophids and P. parnellii fully compensated for positive DS40,41,49,50,51,53,54,55 up to 8 kHz as seen in R. ferrumequinum18,35. However, Hipposideros speoris and Hipposideros bicolor compensated for DS only partly when sitting on a pendulum56, and Schuller40 found that H. speoris and H. bicolor did not even react to playback. These results led to the assumption that the DSC system of hipposiderids is less accurate than that of rhinolophids and P. parnellii4,6,57. This assumption is corroborated by behavioural audiograms and by neurophysiological data indicating that the auditory fovea seems to be less sophisticated, as it exposes less sharply tuned neurons, which results in a lower frequency selectivity34,40,58, (for details see4).

There are, however, some disadvantages in the design of studies of DSC that make it difficult to evaluate the accuracy of DSC. The echolocation behaviour of stationary bats does not necessarily reflect the behaviour of flying bats. If bats do not react to DS, it does not necessarily mean that they are not able to compensate for DS. The only approach that ensures reliable data on the precision of DSC are studies in flying bats. Further, in hipposiderids4,40,44, but also in P. parnellii41,42,43 and in R. ferrumequinum tragatus46, Frest is not stable. To get reliable values of the offset, it is mandatory to measure Frest directly before Fref is determined. None of the studies in hipposiderids took the variations of Frest into account. Furthermore, meaningful information concerning the precision of the DSC is missing. Some authors have used the ability to keep Fecho constant within a small frequency band just above Frest18 as a measure for precision, while others have used the offset between Fref and Frest as a quality measurement for DSC41,55,56. However, the quality of a feedback control system is indicated by the precision by which the controlled parameter is kept constant over the control range of the system4. In the DSC system, the precision is indicated by the accuracy by which the Fecho of each signal is kept at Fref, independent of the flight speed. Thus far, this has only been measured in R. ferrumequinum flying in a wind tunnel, where the precision of DSC was similar at all ground speeds13. However, none of the studies in other flutter detecting foragers have addressed this question.

For a better understanding of the precision of the DSC in a hipposiderid bat, we trained Hipposideros armiger to fly to a landing grid. Prior to each flight, we measured Frest before the bat took off. We determined the frequency recorded at the microphone at the landing grid during flight, measured the flight speed from 3D video recordings, and calculated according to the measured speed the encountered DS and perceived Fecho. For each flight, we precisely determined the offset between Frest and the averaged Fecho or Fref to understand their coupling, and adjusted for variations in Frest and Fref between flights. We also determined the precision of the DSC system by measuring whether the adjustment of Fecho to Fref is independent of the flight speed.

Results

Echolocation behaviour in resting bats

Both H. armiger individuals (HA 1 and HA 2) exhibited similar echolocation behaviour before take-off (Table 1). They continuously emitted resting signals, generally arranged in groups, and the main energy of the multi-harmonic signals was concentrated at the second harmonic (Fig. 1b). With a mean duration of 9.7 ms the calls of HA 1 were 0.7 ms shorter than the calls of HA 2 (t (38) = 2.13, p = 0.040) (Table 1). For HA 1, the FM component was longer in duration and higher in bandwidth (t (38, 38) ≥ 5.34, p < 0.0001) than HA 2. Hence, 88% (HA 1) and 91% (HA 2) of the total signal duration was determined by the CF component. The pulse interval and duty cycle were highly variable before the bats started to fly (Table 1, Fig. 2a,d). Both bats had an average pulse interval of around 70 ms (t (38) = 0.51, p = 0.61). The duty cycle was 16% in HA 1 and 18% in HA 2 (t (38) = 3.35, p = 0.0019). Frest was not stable (see below).

Table 1 Mean ± SD of the signal parameters of resting signals of H. armiger before take-off [bat 1 (HA 1) and bat 2 (HA 2), n = 400 per bat].
Figure 2
figure 2

Signal parameters (ae) and flight speed (f) of a representative flight of HA 2 (same sequence as shown in Fig. 1). Signal parameters (ae) include the last 20 resting signals before take-off. The bat took off at 0 s and landed at 1.67 s. The beginning of the terminal approach is marked with an arrow. The duration of the total signal (SD) and the duration of the FM component (FM D) are shown in (b). Emission frequency (Femitted) and echo frequency (Fecho) during flight are calculated for targets ahead (e) by using the flight speed (f). Before take-off the emission frequency corresponds to the resting frequency (Frest) (e). The averaged Fecho corresponds to the Fref.

Echolocation behaviour in flying bats

H. armiger flew stereotyped flight paths straight to the landing grid [Supplementary Fig. S1]. Before landing on the grid, they turned upside down. After take-off, the bats emitted search or orientation signals, which were often arranged in groups (Fig. 1a,c). About 1 s before landing the bats switched to the initial approach, indicated by an increase of pulses per group and a reduction of the pulse interval and signal duration. At 1.6 ± 0.3 m (HA 1) and 1.7 ± 03 m (HA 2) or 640 ± 90 ms (HA 1) and 660 ± 70 ms (HA 2) before landing on the grid, the bats started the terminal approach and emitted a long terminal group with 41 ± 4 (HA 1) and 44 ± 4 (HA 2) short signals on average (Fig. 1a,d). The start of the terminal approach and the number of signals did not differ between the bats (t (18, 18) ≤ 1.59, p ≥ 0.1296). In flight, the bats emitted multi-harmonic signals with the main amplitude in the second harmonic, as they did before take-off. However, the other harmonics were more strongly suppressed than in resting signals (Fig. 1b,c). Harmonic suppression was even more distinct in the terminal group (Fig. 1d). The CF component was maintained up to the last signal of the terminal group, and sometimes the FM component had a higher peak amplitude than the CF component (Fig. 1a,d). In search or orientation signals, the averaged pulse interval and signal duration differed between bats. HA 1 had a mean pulse interval of 38 ± 7 ms and HA 2 of 48 ± 12 ms; the mean call duration of 9.6 ± 0.8 ms was also shorter in HA 1 than in HA 2 who emitted signals with a duration of 10.5 ± 1 ms (t (18, 18) ≥ 3.95, p ≤ 0.0009). During approach, both parameters decreased continuously the closer the bat was to the landing grid. In the terminal group, the pulse interval declined to a minimum of 12.1 ms (HA 1) and 11.4 ms (HA 2), and signal duration to a minimum of 4.3 ms (HA 1) and 3.2 ms (HA 2) (Fig. 2a,b). When the bats started to fly, the duty cycle of 26 ± 4% in HA 1 was slightly higher than the duty cycle of 23 ± 4% in HA 2 (t (18) = 7.31, p < 0.0001) and increased during approach up to 41% in HA 1 and 48% in HA 2 (Fig. 2d). HA 1 reduced the duration of the FM component from 1.4 ± 0.2 ms during search flight to 1.3 ± 0.2 ms in the terminal approach (t (18) = 3.8, p = 0.0013), whereas HA 2 did not change the duration of the FM component significantly [FM duration, 1.25 ± 0.1 ms during search flight, 1.23 ± 0.1 ms in the terminal approach (t (18) = 0.94, p = 0.3588)]. In search flight, the bandwidth of the FM component in HA 1 (7.5 kHz) was much higher than in HA 2 (5.2 kHz) (Z = −3.7, n = 10, p = 0.0002). Both bats exhibited an increase in bandwidth from search to terminal approach, in HA 1, who had the higher bandwidth in search flight, by 290 Hz on average and in HA 2 by 940 Hz (t (18) = 5.37, p < 0.0001) (Fig. 2b). Overall, the sweep rate of the FM was increased in the terminal FM, either by a reduction in duration and increase of bandwidth (HA 1), or an increase in bandwidth alone (HA 2, Fig. 2c).

Doppler shift compensation

In flight, H. armiger lowered the frequency of the CF component of the CF-FM signals, such that the frequency of the CF2 component of the echoes from ahead (Fecho) was kept almost constant around Fref, which was determined as the average of all calculated echo frequencies of the corresponding flight (Fig. 2e). The lowest emission frequency was measured when flight speed reached a maximum of 4.7 m/s (Fig. 2e,f). In single flights, standard deviations ranging from 80 Hz to 170 Hz were measured. The average standard deviation for all flights of both bats was 110 Hz (Fig. 3a), corresponding to a deviation of only 0.17% from Fref. The averaged standard deviation of 140 Hz around Frest was slightly higher, corresponding to 0.21%. Frest and Fref were not stable; they differed from flight to flight and from day to day (Figs 3a and 4a). However, Fref and Frest varied in a systematic way during flights. The correlation of Frest with Fref was highly significant (HA 1: Spearman r = 0.92, p < 0.0001; HA 2: Spearman r = 0.84, p < 0.0001) and the regression lines were almost parallel to the bisector, indicating that Frest and Fref were tightly coupled (Fig. 5). In nine out of ten sessions, Fref and Frest decreased with the number of flights performed by the bats but started at approximately the same initial level on the next day. Fref declined by 130 Hz on average between the first and last flight within a session (Fig. 3a). The highest difference between the first and last flight within one session was 250 Hz (HA 1, 02/03, Fig. 3a). Furthermore, in HA 2 we observed a decline of Fref by 1 kHz over the course of just nine days (27/02 and 08/03, Fig. 3a).

Figure 3
figure 3

Reference frequency (Fref) and corresponding resting frequency (Frest). Mean ± SD of the CF2- or resting frequencies before the bats take-off (black bars) and Fref (white bars) for 20 flights per bat (a). The x-axis shows the number of the flight within one session. The distribution of the offset between Frest and Fref is shown in (b).

Figure 4
figure 4

Echo frequency plotted against flight speed for HA 1 and HA 2. Means of the echo frequencies (Fecho) calculated for 0.5 m/s classes of ten flights (coloured) (a). Deviation of Fecho from the reference frequency of the corresponding flight calculated for each call of ten flights (grey dots). Black dots indicate means (±SD) calculated for 0.5 m/s classes (b).

Figure 5
figure 5

Correlation between the means of resting frequency and reference frequency. For each bat, 20 flights with linear regression lines are shown. The grey line indicates the angle bisector.

Since Fref and Frest were not stable, we calculated the offset between Frest and Fref for every single flight. The offset between Frest emitted just before the bat took off and the calculated Fref during flight measured between 5 and 140 Hz, except for 3 out of 20 flights in HA 2 in which Frest was higher than the calculated Fref. The mean difference between Frest and Fref was 90 Hz in HA 1 and 70 Hz in HA 2 (excluding the three negative values), which corresponds to an offset of approximately 0.12% (Fig. 3b).

The precision with which Fecho is kept at Fref, independent of the encountered DS and thus independent of flight speed, indicates the quality of the DSC feedback control system. The correlation between the deviation of Fecho from Fref and the flight speed, as a measure of precision, was significant in both bats; however, Fecho was not predicted by flight speed as indicated by the coefficient of determination, which was smaller than 0.1 (HA 1: r² (680) = 0.080, p < 0.0001; HA 2: r² (670) = 0.056, p < 0.0001). Variation was higher at low flight speeds, indicated by higher standard deviations (Fig. 4b).

Discussion

Rhinolophids, hipposiderids, and the mormoopid bat, P. parnellii, use the flutter detecting echolocation strategy to find prey. The echolocation systems of flutter detecting foragers are described by Frest and Fref. Frest is determined by the average frequency of the CF component of the CF-FM signals in stationary bats, and Fref is measured in bats performing DSC as the average frequency of the CF component in the echoes returning from ahead. In rhinolophids and P. parnellii, Frest and Fref are almost constant within short time periods; offsets between Frest and Fref of only 150–200 Hz9,14,30,48,50,51 have been measured. However, it has been reported that in hipposiderids Frest was less stable40, Fecho was regulated with less precision around Fref, (reviewed in4), and offsets of 300 to 600 Hz were relatively high. This led to the assumption that the DSC feedback system of hipposiderids may be less precise than in rhinolophids and P. parnellii40,56.

The variability of Frest and Fref in hipposiderids poses the problem that the values given for Frest, Fref, and the offset between them strongly depend on the instant in time when they are measured. To overcome this problem, we adjusted for frequency variations by measuring Fref for every flight and Frest just prior to take-off, and used these values to calculate the offset. Both Frest and Fref were rather stable when measured for every single flight, with deviations similar to those observed in rhinolophids and P. parnellii. However, Frest and Fref varied up to 230 Hz and 250 Hz, respectively, within a session and between daily sessions. In one bat, a drop of 1 kHz over 10 days was observed.

Although we found a high variability in Frest and Fref, there was always a tight coupling between the two frequencies. Frest was slightly below Fref, and both Fref and Frest changed in the same manner. The tight coupling accounts for a rather small mean offset of 80 Hz measured in single flights, independent of the changes in absolute frequency, such that either frequency can be used to predict the other. The offset of 80 Hz measured in H. armiger is within the range of 150–200 Hz reported for rhinolophids and P. parnellii9,14,40,48,50,51. This suggests a similar coupling mechanism for all DSC bats. The variations of Fref and Frest in hipposiderids were not considered in previous studies and most likely account for the high deviations of Fref, Frest, and offset reported in this family. Only Gustafson and Schnitzler15 measured the offset in a similar way in A. tridens, which explains why their results were in the same range of values as found in this study.

Stationary flutter detecting foragers emit signals with an Frest always slightly below Fref. This has the advantage that echoes from objects moving towards the bat will generate positive DS and are thus higher in frequency than the emitted signals. These echo frequencies will fall into the range of Fref where the auditory system is most sensitive. Frest being below Fref may have the function of an alarm system for prey or predators moving towards the bat. The tight coupling of Frest to Fref ensures that this system also functions with a variable Fref.

The auditory fovea of flutter detecting foragers consists of an enlarged area on the basilar membrane with an overrepresentation of frequencies around Fref24,29 that projects into higher auditory centres in the brain (e.g. in hipposiderids:33,34,59) with sharply tuned neurons specialized for the evaluation of flutter information. DSC bats lower the emission frequency to keep the echo frequency constant at Fref. High precision DSC ensures an optimal flutter evaluation. A variable Fref implies that the frequency that leads to maximal activation of the auditory fovea changes somewhat for reasons that are yet unknown. Liu et al.46 came to a similar conclusion when explaining the variation of Frest in R. ferrumequinum tragatus. They housed pairs of bats and recorded their echolocation and social calls. While social calls did not change in frequency, they found day-to-day changes in Frest of up to 900 Hz. They concluded that changes in Frest mirror changes in the tuning of the auditory fovea. We assume that the resonance properties of the basilar membrane in the cochlea change, influencing the maxima of the travelling wave and with it the position at which a specific frequency is represented. This is supported by studies in P. parnellii where body temperature and/or flight activity influenced the cochlear resonance frequency42,60, and also Frest and Fref43. Frest and Fref were positively correlated with body temperature, exhibiting changes of 93 Hz/°C and 90 Hz/°C, respectively. Huffman and Henson43 suggested that the cochlear resonance frequency is affected by changes in body temperature, inducing shifts in Frest and Fref. We assume that in our flight experiments the foveal resonance properties in the cochlea of H. armiger changed, possibly due to variations in body temperature, and that these changes are mirrored by the shift of Frest and Fref. DSC bats thus adjust Fecho or Fref such that the foveal resonance area on the cochlea and their projections into the auditory brain are maximally activated4.

In feedback control systems the best measure to assess quality is the precision with which the parameter under control (i.e. Fecho) is kept at the reference parameter (i.e. Fref)4. In a first approximation the precision can be estimated by the deviation of Fecho from Fref. In rhinolophids and P. parnellii this deviation was measured as the width of the frequency range of Fecho around Fref and reached values of 0.1–0.2%. In hipposiderids the estimated values were much higher at 0.4–0.7%4, except for A. tridens in which the deviation was distinctly smaller15. In H. armiger, we measured an averaged standard deviation of 110 Hz, corresponding to a deviation of only 0.17% from Fref. These values are similar to the variability measured in rhinolophids and P. parnellii.

The use of the averaged standard deviation as an indicator of precision is only appropriate if bats are able to maintain Fecho at Fref independent of the flight speed. We controlled for this assumption by calculating the deviation of Fecho from Fref for every single echo and related it to the flight speed (Fig. 4). Although the correlation between the deviation of Fecho from Fref and flight speed was significant, biased by the large sample size, a coefficient of determination of 0.080 or lower indicates a poor model fit with less than 10% of the Fecho variance explained by the influence of the flight velocity. This implies that the Fecho is rather independent from flight speed. The higher variation at low flight speeds, indicated by the higher standard deviations, is due to the accelerated flight during take-off and landing. Fast changes reduce the precision of the slow feedback control system61. In flying R. ferrumequinum, the performance of DSC at different flight speeds was measured in a wind tunnel13. Independent of the ground speed, the bats adjusted Fecho in such a way that the offset to Frest stayed the same and was similar to the offset measured in flying bats under normal conditions. To describe the precision of DSC systems, we suggest testing whether a species compensates for DS independent of flight speed and, if yes, to determine the average standard deviation of Fecho relative to that of Fref.

In playback experiments with stationary bats that simulate different flight speeds by presenting echoes with positive DS up to 8 kHz, R. ferrumequinum fully compensated for DS and kept Fecho at Fref with standard deviations of 30 to 40 Hz30,35. Comparable playback data for P. parnellii are missing; however, similar DSC performance in flight suggests a similar precision14,51. In contrast, hipposiderids failed to react in playback experiments40. This does not necessarily prove that the feedback control system of hipposiderids is less precise, as these experiments probably were not sufficiently naturalistic for the bats. In conclusion, our data from H. armiger and data from A. tridens15 suggest that most likely all hipposiderids compensate for DS with the same precision as rhinolophids and P. parnellii.

When flying towards the landing grid, H. armiger already emitted groups of signals during orientation or search flight. The beginning of the approach phase is therefore not as obvious as in other bats. However, a reduction of signal duration and pulse interval, and an increase in duty cycle and number of signals per group indicated that the approach behaviour started at about 1 s before landing. The terminal group had the highest number of signals, 43 on average. The echolocation behaviour of H. armiger in our landing task was similar to the behaviour of other hipposiderids in a comparable behavioural situation15,52. There are, however, distinct differences to the approach behaviour of rhinolophids. In orientation or search flight rhinolophids often emit one long signal per wing beat, whereas hipposiderids emit several shorter signals per wing beat. In the initial approach, the grouping of signals was less regular than in rhinolophids and the terminal group comprised a much higher number of signals than the terminal group of other flutter detecting foragers9,14,15,16. In general, hipposiderids operate with the shortest signals of all flutter detecting foragers. Nevertheless, due to the high repetition rate, they have a high duty cycle.

Conclusion

In H. armiger, the precision of DSC, measured as the deviation of Fecho from Fref during flight, was similar to that in horseshoe bats and P. parnellii, and the precision of DSC was independent of the flight speed. Fref and Frest varied from flight to flight, but Fref and Frest were tightly coupled. The offset between the two frequencies was similar to the offset measured in rhinolophids and P. parnellii. The variation of Fref, and with it, Frest, may indicate that the resonance frequency of the foveal area in the cochlea varies and that the bats adjust Fecho or Fref such that the foveal resonance area on the cochlea and its projections into the auditory brain are maximally activated to ensure an optimal evaluation of flutter information. This implies that the DSC system of hipposiderids is of similar precision to that of horseshoe bats and P. parnellii. The frequency variations may explain why in former studies it has been assumed that DSC in hipposiderid bats is incomplete.

There are, however, differences in the flutter evaluation systems between families that might affect the quality of flutter information extracted from echoes4. Rhinolophids and P. parnellii emit long signals and increase their duty cycle by producing longer signals as a reaction to flutter information. The echo of a single signal comprises the glints of several wing beats of an insect. Signals of hipposiderids are much shorter and the duty cycle is increased by emitting more signals; thus flutter information is encoded over several echoes (reviewed in4,6). Additionally, the auditory fovea of rhinolophids and P. parnellii is largely expanded around Fref24,28 whereas the auditory fovea of hipposiderids is less developed (40, 58). Further, the neurons in foveal areas of the auditory pathway are more sharply tuned in rhinolophids and P. parnellii than in hipposiderids, indicated by higher Q10dB values. Thus, hipposiderids may be less able to decode detailed flutter information compared with rhinolophids or P. parnellii. The precise DSC, however, enables them to keep Fecho exactly at Fref to distinguish moving prey echoes from non-moving background echoes.

Materials and Methods

Animals

We conducted the experiments with two adult Great Leaf-nosed Bats [Hipposideros armiger (Hodgson, 1835)], captured in the Ba Be National Park in northern Vietnam (Permission No. 129/STTNSV from May 13th, 2009, granted to the Vietnamese Institute of Ecology and Biological Resources, Hanoi). They were housed in two aviaries (3.2 × 1.25 × 2 m and 2.4 × 1.2 × 2 m) but had access to a room (6.0 × 3.6 × 3 m) where they could freely fly together. Abiotic conditions were kept constant (12:12 hours light/dark cycle, 26.6 ± 2 °C and 60 ± 5% humidity) and bats had free access to water and mealworms (Tenebrio molitor). Food was supplemented with vitamins (Nutrical®), fatty acids (Efaderm®), and minerals (Korvimin®). Additionally, they were hand-fed with large insects, including crickets (Gryllus spp. and Acheta domestica), grasshoppers (Schistocerca gregaria), and beetles (Zophobas morio, Pachnoda marginata).

Ethical statement

The animal facility of the Institute for Neurobiology at the University of Tübingen in which the bats were kept is approved by the Regierungspräsidium Tübingen (AZ: 35/9185.46/Uni Tü) according to §11 of the German Animal Welfare Law. We and the animal welfare officers of the Faculty of Science and of the University of Tübingen agreed that our experiments did not require animal experimentation approval, as it could be excluded that the bats were exposed to procedures that cause pain, distress, suffering, or harm according to Directive 2010/63/EU and §7 of the German Animal Welfare Law. Bats were accustomed to being handled and familiar with the flight room. They always had access to water and food and could fly without restriction in the flight room. Using large insects favoured by the bats as positive reinforcement, they learned to fly from a starting bar to their preferred landing site in the room. For similar experiments, the approval authority agreed that a permit is not necessary (by letter from the Regierungspräsidium Tübingen, March 29th, 2012).

Experimental setup

All experiments were carried out in complete darkness in a 6.0 × 3.6 × 3 m flight room. The walls and the ground were covered with sound absorbent foam to reduce echoes. Bats were trained by positive reinforcement with favoured insects to fly freely from a starting bar (1.2 m above ground) to a landing grid positioned at the preferred landing site of each bat. The grid of bat 1 (HA 1) was positioned 2.7 m above ground at a distance of 3.5 m from the starting bar. Bat 2 (HA 2) landed on a grid fixed to the wall, 1.4 m above the ground, at a distance of 4.3 m from the starting bar. The ultrasonic microphone was positioned 3 cm above each landing grid (Supplementary Fig. S1). All other recording devices were behind the starting bar and did not affect the bats’ behaviour. Except for the walls of the room, there were no objects along the flight paths.

Data recordings and analysis

The flight behaviour of the bats was recorded with two IR cameras (Sanyo IR CCD, Japan) at a rate of 50 Hz. Each half frame was illuminated for 1 ms by two infrared strobe flashes. The videos were stored on Panasonic DVC mini-tapes using two camcorders (Sony, DCR-TR V50E, Japan). After digitizing, the recordings were analysed (SIMI Motion Reality Motion Systems, 7.5.293) to reconstruct the 3D flight path (mean reconstruction error of 3.2 cm) and to calculate the flight speed.

Sound recordings were made with PC-Tape (Animal Physiology, University of Tübingen). The echolocation signals were picked up through a custom-made ultrasonic microphone (nearly flat frequency response, at 100 kHz 4 dB less sensitive than at 20 kHz), digitized (480 kHz, 16 bit), and stored as wav-files. Sound recordings were synchronized with the video recordings. For analysis, the recordings were displayed as colour sonograms (FFT 512, Blackman window, dynamic range 90 dB) in a window of 512 × 512 pixels with a frequency range of 35–95 kHz and a duration of 50 ms (custom-written software Selena, Animal Physiology, University of Tübingen). Due to autopadding and interpolation in time, we achieved a resolution of Δf = 110 Hz and Δt = 0.1 ms. Sound parameters were calculated using a MATLAB routine (Matlab®, 7.7.0.402, 2009b, written by Peter Stilz). The beginning and end of an echolocation signal was defined at −30 dB below best amplitude, and the beginning of the terminal FM component at 660 Hz below the CF2-frequency corresponding to approximately 1% of the CF2-frequency.

To determine the precise CF2-frequency recorded at the microphone (FM) to calculate the DSC, we displayed the signals between 63 and 69 kHz using an FFT with 8192 points (zero padding), resulting in a frequency resolution of 11.5 Hz. The CF2-frequency was measured at the peak amplitude of the CF component.

To calculate the emitted CF2- or signal frequency (FS) and the Fecho from the frequency recorded by the microphone, we used the equations published by Schnitzler13. The signal frequency is given by equation (1)

$${{\rm{F}}}_{{\rm{S}}}={{\rm{F}}}_{{\rm{M}}}\times ({\rm{c}}-{{\rm{v}}}_{{\rm{B}}})/{\rm{c}}$$
(1)

where FM is the recorded frequency at the microphone, vB the speed of the bat, and c the velocity of sound (343 m/s).

In situations where the bat was not flying directly towards the microphone, e.g. immediately after take-off, the Doppler shift at the microphone was smaller than the calculated Doppler shift from ahead according to the cosine of the angle between the flight and microphone direction. Therefore, the calculated signal frequency (FS) was slightly too high. At low flight speeds and the small angles after take-off these deviations were very small, e.g. at an angle of 30° and a flight speed of 1 m/s the deviation was only 28 Hz. The deviations reduced to 0 Hz when the bats changed flight direction and flew directly towards the recording microphone. Therefore, these deviations were not considered.

To determine the frequency of the echo (Fecho) which the bat receives from stationary targets directly ahead, two Doppler shifts have to be added13. If vB is small in comparison to c, equation (2) gives a good approximation for Fecho

$${{\rm{F}}}_{{\rm{echo}}}={{\rm{F}}}_{{\rm{S}}}+{{\rm{F}}}_{{\rm{S}}}\times {{\rm{2v}}}_{{\rm{B}}}/{\rm{c}}.$$
(2)

In total, we analysed the flight and echolocation behaviour of 20 flights for each bat recorded over a time course of 16 days. We first determined the resting frequency (Frest) by measuring the averaged CF2-frequency of the last 20 echolocation signals while the bat was still hanging at the starting bar before take-off. For every single flight, we determined the reference frequency (Fref) as the mean of all calculated Fecho. Further, we calculated the standard deviation with which Fecho varied around Fref. Since data were not normally distributed (Shapiro Wilk test, p < 0.05), we evaluated the Spearman correlation between the mean of Frest and the Fref for all 20 flights per bat.

An indicator of the quality of the DSC feedback control system is the precision with which Fecho is kept at Fref independent of the flight speed. Since Fecho varied between flights, we subtracted the calculated Fecho of every single signal from the Fref (which is the mean of the calculated echo frequencies of the whole sequence) in 10 flights per bat, plotted it against flight speed, and tested whether the deviation depends on flight speed with a Pearson correlation. Further, we calculated the means of this deviation from Fref within flight speed classes of 0.5 m/s. In 10 of the 20 flights per bat, we analysed the echolocation behaviour and measured pulse interval, signal duration, duty cycle, bandwidth, and duration of the FM component. We visually checked for a normal distribution using histograms and normal quantile plots. We tested whether the echolocation behaviour differed between individuals using a t-test or Wilcoxon rank sum test.

Data availability

The datasets generated and analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.