Abstract
The ability of humans to discriminate enantiomeric odour pairs is substance –specific. Current literature suggests that psychophysical discrimination of odour enantiomers mainly depends on the peripheral processing at the level of the olfactory sensory neurons (OSN). To study the influence of central processing in discrimination, we investigated differences in the electrophysiological responses to psychophysically indistinguishable (+)- and (−)- rose oxide enantiomers at peripheral and central-nervous levels in humans. We recorded the electro-olfactogram (EOG) from the olfactory epithelium and the EEG-derived olfactory event-related potentials (OERP). Results from a psychophysical three alternative forced choice test indicated indistinguishability of the two odour enantiomers. In a total of 19 young participants EOG could be recorded in 74 and OERP in 95% of subjects. Significantly different EOG amplitudes and latencies were recorded in response to the 2 stimuli. However, no such differences in amplitude or latency emerged for the OERP. In conclusion, although the pair of enantiomer could be discriminated at a peripheral level this did not lead to a central-nervous/cognitive differentiation of the two stimuli.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
The ability to discriminate molecular structures from its mirror image, the so-called chiral recognition of substances, belongs to the basic principles of biological activity1. Except for the optical activity, odour enantiomers hold identical physical and chemical features. Besides differences in olfactory perception, diverse effects of enantiomers have also been well described in the context of taste perception2 and drug response3, 4. In the 90 s olfactory receptors have been identified as proteins, i.e. chiral molecules5, for which reason the interaction between enantiomers and receptors was assumed to be enantio-selective. Therefore, any differences in odour perception were understood to result from the selectivity of olfactory receptors at the level of the olfactory epithelium6, 7. However, not only distinguishable but also identically smelling or indistinguishable enantiomeric odour pairs have been described in humans and non-human primates8 highlighting an unpredictable relationship between odour structure and odour perception.
The olfactory bulb represents a central neural source which seems to be critical for odour discrimination. Information from the olfactory sensory neurons (OSN) is represented in the activity of olfactory bulb glomeruli9,10,11,12. Linster et al. demonstrated in rats the predictability of odour enantiomer discrimination from their evoked patterns of neural activation in the olfactory bulb13. A rather biological approach explaining the discriminability of only some odour enantiomers might be the occurrence of both optical isomers in nature. In analogy to the immune system there is increasing evidence for the mammalian OSNs to increase its receptor expression following repeated exposure to olfactory stimuli14,15,16. Thus, it has been hypothesized, when both optical isomers occur in a natural environment, the appropriate enantio-selective receptors will enable the differentiation of the pair of odours.
Given the lack of clarity in the neurophysiological mechanism of odour discrimination, we set out to explore the differences in peripheral and central representations of indistinguishable rose oxide enantiomers by recording electro-olfactograms (EOG) from the olfactory epithelium and central-nervous, EEG-derived olfactory-event-related potentials (OERP) from the scalp. We hypothesized that indistinguishable odour enantiomers show electrophysiological differences at the level of the OSNs (demonstrating enantio-selectivity regardless of the psychophysical discriminability) while the central neural representation exhibits similar response patterns which would be the basis for similar behavioural responses or psychophysical indistinguishability.
Results
Behavioural data
All subjects showed a normal sense of smell with an average TDI score of 38 points (SD: 3.3, ∅ scores: threshold (T): 10 (SD: 2), discrimination (D): 13 (SD: 2), identification (I): 15 (SD: 1)). The average discrimination performance for the target odour was 3.2 (SD: 1.01). Applying a one sample t-test, this result did not differ significantly from chance level (t [18]) = 0.85, p = 0.41). Regarding the intensity rating of R (+) and R (−) no significant differences emerged between the two enantiomers (∅ intensity R (+): 41 (SD: 15.4), R (−): 40 (SD: 19.9); t [18]) = −0.35, p = 0.73) as showed by the paired sample t-test. These results indicated psychophysical indistinguishability of R (+) and R (−) (Fig. 1a and b).
Electrophysiological data
EOG
EOG recordings could be obtained in 14 out of 19 (74%) participants. When comparing the electrophysiological responses to R (+) and R (−) – by using a paired sample t- test - there were significant differences in EOG amplitudes AN1 (t [13] = −2.29, p = 0.04) and AP1N1 (t [13] = 2.39, p = 0.03) with higher amplitudes obtained following R (+) stimulation (base-to-peak amplitude AN1 (in μV): R (+) 6.42 (SD: 5.3), R (−) 3.77(SD: 2.4)); peak-to-peak amplitude AP1N1 (in μV): R (+) 6.21 (SD: 4.1), R (−) 4.07 (SD: 1.9). Further, significant differences in the peak latencies TP1 (t [13] = 2.56, p = 0.024) and TN1(t [13] = 3.39, p = 0.005) were recorded with longer latencies found in response to R (+) (latencies: TP1 (in ms): R (+) 307 (SD: 208), R (−) 179 (SD: 137); TN1 (in ms): R (+) 589 (SD: 219), R (−) 415 (SD: 239)) (Figs 2A and B and 3).
OERP
OERPs in response to the enantiomers of rose oxide could be recorded in 18 out of 19 (95%) participants. Regarding amplitudes (AN1 (in μV): R (+) 2.29 (SD: 1.8), R (−) 1.94 (SD: 1.4), AP2: R (+) 4.42 (SD: 3.5), R (−) 4.08 (SD: 3.2), AN1P2: R (+) 6.71 (SD: 4.9), R (−) 6.01 (SD: 4.3)) and latencies (TN1 (in ms): R (+) 567 (SD: 62), R (−) 551 (SD: 87), TP2: R (+) 739 (SD: 47), R (−) 728 (SD: 103)) no significant differences emerged between the two enantiomers by means of paired sample t-test (all p s > 0.05) (Figs 4A and B and 5).
Discussion
The results of this study demonstrate that (1) psychophysically indistinguishable odour enantiomers show differences in peripheral response (EOG) but (2) no differences in central-nervous responses (OERP). These differences in EOG at the level of the OSN reveal distinct neuronal activation pattern of OSNs representing enantio-selectivity of the receptors for psychophysically indistinguishable odour enantiomers. Contrary to the assumptions of Laska and colleague, enantio-selectivity of olfactory receptors is given in same smelling odour enantiomers and therefore enantio-selectivity of OSNs seems not to predict the ability of humans to psychophysically discriminate molecular structures from its mirror image7, 8, 17.
Studies on the mechanism of olfactory perception and discrimination mainly focus on the peripheral interaction between molecules and receptors assuming to be the main locus for discrimination. For example, Laska et al. proposed that the combined presence of isopropenyl and methyl groups in specific positions of odour enantiomers allows their psychophysical discrimination18. Poivet et al. chose a different approach emphasizing biological function over chemical structures. They demonstrated the importance of topographical polar surface area of molecules in odour discrimination19.
Without a systematic understanding of odour detection and discrimination at the periphery it is difficult to imagine how central brain areas process the sensory input to generate perception. OSNs express one type of odorant receptors20,21,22 with those homologous for one receptor type converging onto a relatively small number of glomeruli within the OB23,24,25. The role of the OB as part of the central-nervous olfactory brain structures in odour discrimination was highlighted by Linster et al. in rats13 showing similar neural OB representation patterns for indistinguishable and distinct glomerular activation areas for distinguishable enantiomeric odour pairs. However, McBride et al. questioned these findings by demonstrating that surgical lesion of glomerular regions activated by odour enantiomers does not seem to influence discriminatory ability26. Because olfactory sensory information appears to be represented one-to-one in the activity of olfactory bulb glomeruli and we believe that - regardless of the psychophysical distinguishability - odour enantiomers show enantio-selectivity at the OSN level, we expect even higher central olfactory regions than the OB to be responsible for odour enantiomer discrimination. As shown in our study, psychophysically indistinguishable odour enantiomers of rose oxide showed differences in neural response at the peripheral level whereas no differences emerged in the activation pattern at the cortical level. We considered that differences in concentration of R (+) and R (−) (0.0275 vs 0.02) - at which odour enantiomer indistinguishability and lack of trigeminal stimulation was demonstrated - could have contributed to differences in EOG potentials27. However, in this case, we would have expected similar OERP differences at the central level in response to the same odour enantiomer concentrations which did not appear28. Hence, our findings indicate that indistinguishable odour enantiomers evoking similar cortical electrophysiological patterns in humans may result in similar behavioural responses. Consequently, a structure-based encoding seems to be present in the periphery (OSN) but the quality-based encoding shows no direct relation to structural configuration. Although we can draw no conclusion as to where the distinct information from the OSNs to R (+)- and R (−) converged or even dissolved at the central level leading to similar OERP, the following structures may be relevant to that process. The primary olfactory cortex (especially the piriform cortex) receives all direct bulbar input29, 30 and also sends feedback projections to the OB31 providing many ways for the modulation of sensory processing. Higher projections from these primary regions then converge on ‘olfactory regions’ of the orbitofrontal cortex where a ‘one-to-one’ input is lacking. Therefore, these highly processed representations seem to transcend the original sensory input and represent not only structural based codes but also include information, e.g., on previous experience.
Coding mechanisms of odour quality and odour structure in the human piriform cortex have been thoroughly investigated by Gottfried and colleagues32. While the anterior piriform cortex encodes structures of odours, the posterior region is more related to encoding of odour quality. The structure based coding appears to represent the sensory information from the OB. But a structure independent coding based on the quality of an odour, which depends on learning and experience, could explain why some odour enantiomers are perceived differently and some not. In return, the piriform cortex could therefore represent the region where distinct peripheral electrophysiological responses to R (+)- and R (−) are finally interpreted resulting in similar OERP reflecting psychophysical indistinguishability. In order to further corroborate our data, we decided to explore whether a couple of easily distinguishable odor enantiomers could evoke different OERP patterns. Limonene7 [(+)- limonene and (−)- limonene] was tested in a group of 12 healthy participants by applying the same experimental procedure (see the Supplementary Information for an overview of the methodology and the results). In line with literature, participants were able to psychophysically discriminate between the two enantiomers of limonene7 and this result was also reflected in the brain by showing significant distinct OERP patterns in response to (+)- limonene and (−)- limonene. With this finding, we underline our hypothesis on the importance of the central rather than the peripheral processing of psychophysical discriminability.
In conclusion, we demonstrated that odour enantiomers evoking different responses at the level of the olfactory epithelium but similar cortical response patterns resulted in similar behavioural responses. Hence, a structure-based encoding seems to be present in the periphery which is not necessarily translated into a central nervous electrophysiological or behavioural response.
Material and Methods
Participants
A prospective study was conducted at the Smell and Taste Clinic at the Department of Otolaryngology of the “Technische Universität” (TU) Dresden. The study was performed according to the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Ethics Committee of the Medical Faculty at the TU Dresden (application number: EK361082016). All experiments were undertaken after subjects had provided written informed consent. Only participants between the age of 18 and 40 years with a normal sense of smell ascertained by means of Sniffin’ Sticks33 were included in the study. Further, nasal endoscopy was performed in all participants to evaluate the endonasal anatomy in the area of the olfactory cleft. We finally included only those subjects with a clearly visible olfactory cleft in order to enable easy placement of the EOG electrode without causing much discomfort to the participants. The following exclusion criteria applied: pregnancy, previous head trauma leading to unconsciousness, chronic/acute rhinosinusitis, neurological diseases, any systemic disease associated with smell disorders like chronic renal failure or thyroid disorders, and impaired sense of smell. Participants were instructed to only drink water one hour prior to the experiment and not to wear any scented products on the day of testing. Finally, a total number of 19 out of 26 tested participants (14 women, 5 men) with an average age of 25 years (range: 20 to 36 years, SD: 3.5) were included in the study.
Enantiomers and trigeminality
A pair of indistinguishable odour enantiomers was used as stimulus7, 34: (+)-rose oxide (Fisher Scientific, CAS 16409-43-1) and (−)-rose oxide (Sigma-Aldrich, CAS, 16409-43-1). Starting from concentrations used in the study of Li et al.34 at which enantiomers were indistinguishable (11.1% (+)-rose oxide and 8.3% (−)-rose oxide), we further diluted the odours using 1,2- propandiol (Sigma-Aldrich, CAS 57-55-6). First, a lateralization task was employed35, 36. This was based on the repetitive presentation (n = 20) of odours to the left or right nostril the result of which depends on trigeminal sensitivity. This pilot study was performed in an independent sample of 10 participants in order to determine the trigeminal activity of the two odorants. If participants are not able to correctly localize the stimulated nostril we considered the odorant to not elicit any major trigeminal activation. The information was needed to (i) avoid behavioural discrimination based on the trigeminal component of the odour and (ii) to clearly obtain responses in response to olfactory but not to trigeminal stimulation when recording EOG.
The final concentrations used for the study were 0.0275% (+)-rose oxide (R (+)) and 0.02% (−)-rose oxide (R (−)). Based on an independent pilot study in 15 subjects, a three-alternative force choice (3-AFC) demonstrated that at these concentrations the odours were clearly perceivable, indistinguishable and rated as iso-intense (see the “behavioural testing” section for details on the 3-AFC). Odours were always presented to one nostril using a computer-controlled air-dilution olfactometer (OM6b; Burghart, Wedel, Germany) capable of delivering odorants without altering mechanical or thermal conditions inside the nasal cavity.
Behavioural testing
The ability to discriminate between the two enantiomers was assessed using 9 trials of a 3-AFC discrimination test. The participants had to discriminate the target odour (in our case R (+)) from two identical non-target odours (R (−)). The stimulus duration was set at 500 ms with an inter-stimulus interval (ISI) of 6 s and approximately 20 s between presentations of each triplet. Subsequently, participants were also asked to provide ratings of odour intensity using a computerized visual analogue scale (VAS) ranging from 0 to 100 units (0 - “stimulus not perceived” to 100 - “very intense”). If no significant difference in intensity ratings emerged and the results from the 3-AFC test (9 trials) did not differ from chance level, we considered R (−) and R (+) to be psychophysically indistinguishable.
Electrophysiological data acquisition
Olfactometer settings
Before exposing participants to the odorants, a specific breathing technique (velopharyngeal closure)37 was trained in order to avoid respiratory flow inside the nasal cavity. The site of chemical stimulation and EOG recording were chosen according to the endonasal anatomical proportions as described above. For odorous stimulation an olfactometer (OM6b; Burghart, Wedel, Germany) was employed37, 38. Stimuli were embedded in a constantly flowing air stream of controlled temperature (36.5 °C) and humidity (80% relative humidity) which was directed into the nasal cavity by means of a Teflon® tubing (8 cm length, 4/2 mm outer/inner diameter). Total flow rate was set at 6 L/min. With a sharp stimulus onset required, two thirds of the maximum stimulus concentration were reached at the olfactometer’s outlet within 20 ms37. During experiments subjects were seated comfortably in an air-conditioned room. White noise of approximately 50 dB SPL was used to mask clicking sounds associated with odour presentation. Further, participants were instructed to perform a tracking task on a video monitor during stimulation to keep them in an awake and vigilant state during the recordings39.
EOG, EEG settings
EOG was recorded by means of a tubular electrode (outer diameter 0.8 mm) filled with Ringer-agar (1%) which contained a silver-chlorided silver wire37, 40. Each electrode was freshly prepared less than 3 h before the experiment. For reference, an electrode normally used for EEG recordings (sintered Ag/AgCl electrode) was placed on each earlobe, two ground electrodes on each mastoid and a fifth electrode was placed above the right eyebrow to track vertical eye-blinks. The EOG electrode was guided medial to the middle turbinate at the level of its insertion to the back of the nose until contact had been established between tip of the electrode and the mucosa. Hence, the tip of the electrode was located at the transition area of the posterior end of the olfactory cleft and the anterior wall of the sphenoid sinus where olfactory epithelium can be expected with a high probability41. The location of the electrode was again verified endoscopically (Richard Wolf, Knittlingen, Germany; 30° visual angle; outer diameter 1.9 mm) and final adjustments were undertaken under endoscopic control. The electrode was then stabilized by means of an adjustable clip on a frame similar to lensless glasses42.
Besides the odour enantiomers, CO2 (60% v/v; air liquid, Düsseldorf, Germany) was used as stimulant at concentrations which clearly produced trigeminally mediated sensations such as stinging, burning or tickling. Thirty presentations of each odour enantiomer, 15 CO2 stimuli and 15 blank stimuli with a duration of 500ms were randomly presented to each participant. The inter stimulus interval was set at 20 (±4) s during which odourless air was delivered. Participants were instructed to control their eye blinking as much as possible. After an EOG response had been established in response to stimulation with either R (−), R (+) or CO2, the recording session started. Potentials were amplified, filtered (band pass 0.01–30 Hz), and digitized (sampling frequency 125 Hz, recording segments of 8192 ms). For recordings, pre-analysis was applied using Letswave 5 software (http://www.nocions.org/letswave5/). To establish baseline, recordings started 200 ms prior to stimulus onset. Simultaneously to EOG recordings, OERP recordings were obtained from position Cz (referenced against linked earlobes) with a Ag Ag/Cl electrode (Grass E5SH, Astro Med, West Warwick, RI, USA) according to the international 10–20 system using an 8-channel amplifier (Nihon Kohden, Tokyo, Japan). The epochs were inspected by eye for any kind of artefacts (blinks, muscle contractions etc.). Epochs containing amplitudes of 100 µV or higher were discarded. After the artefact rejection process for the EOG recordings had been completed, 3 to 5 “clean” epochs were baseline corrected, filtered with a low-pass filter (15 Hz) and averaged in the time domain. After averaging, maximum EOG peak amplitudes (P1, N1, p1n1) and peak latencies (lp1, ln1) were measured for each individual. For the EEG, clean epochs were baseline corrected, also filtered with a low-pass filter (30 Hz) and averaged in the time domain. Then, the peak amplitudes (N1, P2, n1p2) and peak latencies (ln1, lp2) of the ERP obtained from the Cz electrode were measured using Letswave 5 software.
Statistical Analysis
Using SPSS 23.0 (SPSS Inc. Chicago, Ill., USA) data were statistically analysed by means of t-tests for paired samples to investigate differences in electrophysiological responses between the two enantiomers. To evaluate whether odour enantiomer discrimination ability of the participants differed from chance level, t-tests were performed with the chancel level set at 3. The level of significance was defined as p < 0.05.
Data Availability
The datasets generated during and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
References
Holmstedt, B., Frank, H., Testa, B. Chirality and Biological Activity. (Alan R. Liss, Inc.: New York, 1990).
Kawai, M., Sekine-Hayakawa, Y., Okiyama, A. & Ninomiya, Y. Gustatory sensation of l- and d-amino acids in humans. Amino Acids 43, 2349–2358 (2012).
Caldwell, J., Berova, N. & McConnell, O. Prologue: chirality in the pharmaceutical industry. Chirality 19, 657 (2007).
Calvey, T. N. Isomerism and anaesthetic drugs. Acta Anaesthesiol. Scand. Suppl. 106, 83–90 (1995).
Buck, L. & Axel, R. A novel multigene family may encode odorant receptors: a molecular basis for odor recognition. Cell 65, 175–87 (1991).
Kraft, P. & Fráter, G. Enantioselectivity of the musk odor sensation. Chirality 13, 388–94 (2001).
Laska, M. & Teubner, P. Olfactory Discrimination Ability of Human Subjects for Ten Pairs of Enantiomers. Chem. Senses 24, 161–170 (1999).
Laska, M., Liesen, A. & Teubner, P. Enantioselectivity of odor perception in squirrel monkeys and humans. Am. J. Physiol. 277, R1098–R1103 (1999).
Johnson, B. A. & Leon, M. Modular representations of odorants in the glomerular layer of the rat olfactory bulb and the effects of stimulus concentration. J. Comp. Neurol. 422, 496–509 (2000).
Meister, M. & Bonhoeffer, T. Tuning and topography in an odor map on the rat olfactory bulb. J. Neurosci. 21, 1351–60 (2001).
Sachse, S., Rappert, A. & Galizia, C. G. The spatial representation of chemical structures in the antennal lobe of honeybees: steps towards the olfactory code. Eur. J. Neurosci. 11, 3970–82 (1999).
Buck, L. B. The molecular architecture of odor and pheromone sensing in mammals. Cell 100, 611–8 (2000).
Linster, C. et al. Perceptual Correlates of Neural Representations Evoked by Odorant Enantiomers. Eur. J. Neurosci. 21, 9837–9843 (2001).
Mainland, J. D. et al. Olfactory plasticity: one nostril knows what the other learns. Nature 419, 802 (2002).
Wang, H. W., Wysocki, C. J. & Gold, G. H. Induction of olfactory receptor sensitivity in mice. Science 260, 998–1000 (1993).
Yee, K. K. & Wysocki, C. J. Odorant exposure increases olfactory sensitivity: olfactory epithelium is implicated. Physiol. Behav. 72, 705–11 (2001).
Laska, M. & Galizia, C. G. Enantioselectivity of Odor Perception in Honeybees (Apis mellifera carnica). 115, 632–639 (2001).
Laska, M. Olfactory discrimination ability of human subjects for enantiomers with an isopropenyl group at the chiral center. Chem. Senses 29, 143–152 (2004).
Poivet, E. et al. Applying medicinal chemistry strategies to understand odorant discrimination. Nat. Commun. 7, 11157 (2016).
Malnic, B., Hirono, J., Sato, T. & Buck, L. B. Combinatorial receptor codes for odors. Cell 96, 713–23 (1999).
Serizawa, S. et al. Mutually exclusive expression of odorant receptor transgenes. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 687–93 (2000).
Touhara, K. et al. Functional identification and reconstitution of an odorant receptor in single olfactory neurons. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 96, 4040–4045 (1999).
Mombaerts, P. et al. Visualizing an olfactory sensory map. Cell 87, 675–86 (1996).
Strotmann, J. et al. Local permutations in the glomerular array of the mouse olfactory bulb. J. Neurosci. 20, 6927–38 (2000).
Vassar, R. et al. Topographic organization of sensory projections to the olfactory bulb. Cell 79, 981–91 (1994).
McBride, K. & Slotnick, B. Discrimination between the enantiomers of carvone and of terpinen-4-ol odorants in normal rats and those with lesions of the olfactory bulbs. J. Neurosci. 26, 9892–9901 (2006).
Lapid, H. et al. Odorant concentration dependence in electroolfactograms recorded from the human olfactory epithelium. J. Neurophysiol. 102, 2121–2130 (2009).
Tateyama, T., Hummel, T., Roscher, S., Post, H. & Kobal, G. Relation of olfactory event-related potentials to changes in stimulus concentration. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 108, 449–55 (1998).
de Olmos, J., Hardy, H. & Heimer, L. The afferent connections of the main and the accessory olfactory bulb formations in the rat: an experimental HRP-study. J. Comp. Neurol. 181, 213–44 (1978).
Turner, B. H., Gupta, K. C. & Mishkin, M. The locus and cytoarchitecture of the projection areas of the olfactory bulb in Macaca mulatta. J. Comp. Neurol. 177, 381–96 (1978).
Carmichael, S. T., Clugnet, M. C. & Price, J. L. Central olfactory connections in the macaque monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 346, 403–34 (1994).
Gottfried, J. A., Winston, J. S. & Dolan, R. J. Dissociable codes of odor quality and odorant structure in human piriform cortex. Neuron 49, 467–79 (2006).
Hummel, T., Sekinger, B., Wolf, S. R., Pauli, E. & Kobal, G. ‘Sniffin’ sticks’: olfactory performance assessed by the combined testing of odor identification, odor discrimination and olfactory threshold. Chem. Senses 22, 39–52 (1997).
Li, W., Howard, J. D., Parrish, T. B. & Gottfried, J. A. Aversive learning enhances perceptual and cortical discrimination of indiscriminable odor cues. Science 319, 1842–5 (2008).
Hummel, T., Futschik, T., Frasnelli, J. & Hüttenbrink, K.-B. Effects of olfactory function, age, and gender on trigeminally mediated sensations: a study based on the lateralization of chemosensory stimuli. Toxicol. Lett. 140–141, 273–80 (2003).
Wysocki, C. J., Cowart, B. J. & Radil, T. Nasal trigeminal chemosensitivity across the adult life span. Percept. Psychophys. 65, 115–22 (2003).
Kobal, G. Electrophysiologishe Untersuchungen des Menschlichen Geruchssinns. Thieme, Stuttgart (1981).
Kobal, G. & Hummel, C. Cerebral chemosensory evoked potentials elicited by chemical stimulation of the human olfactory and respiratory nasal mucosa. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. Evoked Potentials 71, 241–250 (1988).
Kobal, G., Hummel, C., Nuernberg, B. & Brune, K. Effects of pentazocine and acetylsalicylic acid on pain-rating, pain-related evoked potentials and vigilance in relationship to pharmacokinetic parameters. Agents Actions 29, 342–59 (1990).
Ottoson, D. Analysis of the electrical activity of the olfactory epithelium. Acta Physiol. Scand. Suppl. 35, 1–83 (1955).
Féron, F., Perry, C., McGrath, J. J. & Mackay-Sim, a New techniques for biopsy and culture of human olfactory epithelial neurons. Arch. Otolaryngol. Head. Neck Surg. 124, 861–866 (1998).
Knecht, M. & Hummel, T. Recording of the human electro-olfactogram. Physiol. Behav. 83, 13–19 (2004).
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by a grant from the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG HU441/18-1 to TH) and by the The Swedish Foundation for Humanities and Social Sciences (M14-0375:1 to ML). SCP was funded by the Dresden Fellowship Programme of the ‘Technische Universität’ (TU) Dresden.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
S.C.P.: Contributed to study conception and design, data acquisition, analysis and interpretation of the data, writing of the manuscript. A.C.: Contributed to study conception and design, data acquisition, analysis and interpretation of the data, critical revision of the manuscript. C.G.: Contributed to analysis and interpretation of the data, critical revision of the manuscript M.L.: Contributed to interpretation of the data and critical revision of the manuscript. T.H.: Contributed to study conception and design, interpretation of the data and critical revision of the manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing Interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Electronic supplementary material
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Poletti, S.C., Cavazzana, A., Guducu, C. et al. Indistinguishable odour enantiomers: Differences between peripheral and central-nervous electrophysiological responses. Sci Rep 7, 8978 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-09594-3
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-09594-3
Comments
By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.