Introduction

Organisms across taxa exhibit daily rhythms in behavior and physiology generated by endogenous ~24 h oscillations (circadian rhythms) that facilitate synchronization to the day-night cycle1. Circadian oscillations are ubiquitous in cells throughout the brain and body and their coordinated daily activity is essential for normal health1,2,3. Disruption to the 24-hour light cycle desynchronizes rhythmic systems throughout the brain and periphery, dysregulating physiological networks and inducing suboptimal physiological functioning until systems realign. When these disruptions are acute (e.g., in the case of a single ‘jet lag’), adults can achieve internal realignment with limited negative impact to health4. However, when circadian disruptions are chronic, as in shift work or frequent travel across time zones, long term physiological and neurological health deteriorate2, 5. In modern settings, work and school schedules, artificial light, and portable electronic devices have made chronic circadian disruptions a near-universal part of the landscape.

In adult mammals, chronic circadian disruption has diverse negative impacts on physical and mental health6, 7, including a shortened lifespan8. Because developing organisms are sensitive to environmental perturbations9,10,11,12, we speculated that developmental chronic circadian disruption (DCCD) would negatively impact normal development, with consequences persisting into adulthood. Furthermore, because different systems develop at different ontogenetic stages, we hypothesized that the timing of DCCD would differentially impact distinct physiological and behavioral traits. In previous studies, exposing pregnant mice and rats to constant light produced lasting increases in anxiety-like behavior13,14,15, with mixed results in learning tasks14,15,16 and associated hippocampal functions15, 16. Likewise, maternal circadian disruption alters offspring metabolic functioning in adulthood13, 17,18,19, including the regulation of glucocorticoids17 and insulin18.

Whether these outcomes are a consequence of circadian disruptions in utero or from alterations in maternal care by circadian-disrupted mothers has not been disambiguated. Thus, the first goal of this investigation was to determine the contributions of circadian disruptions at different developmental life stages to adult deficits. In addressing this question, we elected to employ manipulations that would mimic the type of disruptions – phase shifts – experienced by pregnant women under rotating shift schedules. We therefore implemented a repeated phase-shift design rather than exposure to constant light or only a single “jet lag”. To dissociate the impact of circadian disruption in utero from changes in maternal care resulting from circadian disruption, DCCD mice were cross-fostered to control mothers at birth. To isolate the impact of DCCD to the postnatal period, control animals were cross-fostered to DCCD mothers.

The second goal was to investigate putative mechanisms for the observed effects. Previous studies have proposed that perturbed maternal melatonin signaling is a mechanism by which circadian disruption negatively impacts development15, 17, 20,21,22,23,24,25. For instance, hippocampal clock gene rhythms are disrupted after gestation in constant light, and timed infusions of melatonin delivered to the dam prevent these deficits in adult offspring15. In the present study, we used BALB/c mice that have markedly attenuated melatonin production26, 27 to eliminate any effects of circadian disruption resulting from dysregulated melatonin.

Because chronic circadian disruption can activate the stress axis28, we examined whether or not adverse effects of DCCD are associated with well-established markers of early life stress (ELS). ELS, often transmitted to offspring through dams’ poor maternal care, has lasting developmental effects that vary depending on the life stage in which the stress is experienced29. ELS results in hippocampal hypermethylation at the promoter regions of genes related to the stress axis and brain development, including glucocorticoid receptor (NR3c1) region 17 29,30,31 and brain derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) region IV32,33,34. When disruptions occur in utero, this epigenotype is rescued by cross-fostering pups to an unstressed dam29. Along with hypermethylation of stress-related genes, maternal stress and ELS are associated with decreases in birth weight35, 36 and litter size37, and increases in maternal cannibalism of pups38, 39. If DCCD impacts offspring by acting as a maternal stressor, then disrupted dams should show reduced maternal care and DCCD offspring should exhibit methylation patterns seen in offspring born to stressed dams.

Methods

Animals

All procedures were approved by the UC Berkeley Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUCs) and are in accordance with NIH policies on the care and use of animals. Female BALB/c mice were mated between 2–3 months of age, with males 2–4 months of age. Mice were housed under an LD 12:12 photocycle with ad libitum access to water and laboratory chow. Light onset and offset occurred at 0600 h and 1800 h, respectively. Light intensity during the photo- and scotophases were ~400 lux white light and <1 lux red light, respectively. Humidity and temperature were held constant at 40% and 21 °C, respectively. Mice were group-housed until two weeks before first pairings; thereafter males and females were singly housed. Two weeks before pairing Mini Mitter G2 E-Mitter transmitters (Starr Life Sciences Co., Oakmont, PA) were implanted in the intraperitoneal cavity under isoflurane anesthesia, followed by subcutaneous injections of 0.03 mg/kg buprenorphine in saline, administered every 12 h for 2 days after surgery. The transmitters were sutured to the ventral muscle wall to maintain consistent measurements and chronically recorded locomotor activity (LA) and core body temperature (CBT). The timeline of experimental procedures is presented in Fig. 1. A subset of Mini Mitter temperature and activity data from dams were communicated in a previous report40. All experiments were carried out in compliance with the NIH guide for the care and use of laboratory animals and in accordance with protocols approved by the Animal Care and Use Committee at The University of California, Berkeley.

Figure 1
figure 1

Experimental outline. Timeline (A) of experiment from maternal sensor implantation through end point for adult offspring. LD schedule for control animals (B) and DCCD animals (C) during the experiment, with advances every four days from the time of uterine implantation through weaning. Cartoons of behavioral tests administered to adult offspring during the time labeled “behavior” on the timeline (D).

Housing and Circadian Disruption

CBT profiles were utilized to monitor estrous cycles; mice were paired for 17 h on the day of estrus from 2 h before light offset (1600 h) to 3 h after light onset the following day (0900 h). A male was introduced to the female’s home cage, permitting specification of the day pregnancy was initiated. Pregnancies were confirmed by monitoring the daily CBT profile40. After each round of pairing, half of the females were moved to a separate light box and, on gestational day 6 (g6 – one day after uterine implantation) were exposed to DCCD consisting of 6 h advances in the light cycle every 4 days. DCCD continued until weaning at 22 days of age, at which point offspring were returned to a stable 12:12 LD cycle for the remainder of the study. The other half of the females remained in the static 12:12 LD cycle for the duration of the study. For cross-fostered litters, pups were switched with another litter within 24 h of birth. Six groups of litters were generated: “controls” (C) were neither shifted nor cross fostered; “shifted” (S) animals were exposed to DCCD but not cross-fostered; and all four permutations of control and shifted litters were generated (CC, CS, SC, SS, where the first letter denotes the condition in utero, and the second the condition post-partum). All mice were weighed at birth, daily from day postnatal day (p) 3-p7, at weaning (p22), midlife (p33), and in adulthood (p60). C and S offspring were weighed every 2–3 days after weaning until p60. The date of first vaginal opening was recorded. At weaning mice were ear-punched for identification and separated by sex within litters to a maximum of 4/cage. In litters with 5 same-sex siblings, animals were separated into groups of 3 and 2 to avoid isolation; offspring with litters of just 1 same-sex sibling were singly housed.

Examination of Maternal Behavior

Nest building was assessed prior to parturition, and pup retrieval during lactation. To measure nest building, nest material was removed from the cage two hours before dark onset two days before expected parturition40; pre-weighed fresh nesting material was placed on top of the cage lid, where it could be pulled down into the cage by the pregnant mouse. Four hours after the following light onset (18 h later), the nest material remaining on the cage lid, if any, was weighed. The nest was assessed for doming, consolidation, and neatness. Nests were scored from 1–4, with 1 representing no nest and 4 a complete, consolidated, domed nest. Nest building was only assessed in C and S groups, as it preceded cross-fostering.

To further examine maternal care, a pup retrieval test was conducted. Three hours after light onset on p3, the dam was placed into a 6 × 6′ container and pups were weighed and maintained under a warming lamp. After 5 min, 4 pups chosen at random were returned to the side of the home cage farthest from the nest. The dam was then placed on the nest and the time for each pup to be retrieved to the nest was recorded. This procedure was repeated daily from p3 through p7.

Offspring Behavioral Assessment

Beginning in adulthood, mice began a regimen of one behavioral test/day. In sequence, testing consisted of the elevated plus maze41, social approach behavior42, short-term novel object recognition (within day), and long-term novel object recognition (across days)43. The tests selected allowed assessment of exploration and anxiety-like behavior, social behavior, and learning, while minimizing both time spent in the apparatus and associated stress. These considerations were intended to minimize the impact of earlier tests on subsequent examination. The order of testing allowed novel arena exploration in the elevated plus maze without confounds of previous learning experience or possible stress from social encounters. Learning was assessed last as it required the longest duration of time away from the home cage, and potential anxiety in subsequent tests. All behavioral assessments were carried out in the middle six hours of the light phase. Mice were given 1 h to acclimate after being moved from their housing room to the testing room, after which they were moved through the assessments in an order blind to sex and condition. A video camera was suspended above the center of the testing areas and all relevant behaviors recorded for scoring in vCode, validated by at least two independent scorers.

Elevated plus maze

The mazes were constructed in house from ¼″ acrylic, laser-cut into four arms to fit together at 90° angles with a 2″ wide, 8″ long floor, two of which were enclosed on three sides to a height of 6″, joined in the center by a 2″ × 2″ square, and painted matte black. For testing, the maze was elevated 3’ off the ground on a tripod. Mice were placed in the center of the apparatus and allowed 4 minutes of exploration41. After each mouse completed the test, all fecal pellets were removed, the arena sprayed with ethanol, wiped with paper towels, and allowed to dry before the next mouse was tested. Mice were scored for % time in open vs. closed arms, for stereotyped anxiety-like movements41, taken as the sum of arm switches, rearing, full-body extension, and dipping of the head over the edge of the open arms.

Social approach arena

Two 3″ diameter cylindrical cages were set with centers 12″ apart in a 10″ × 18″ housing chamber with ¼″ bedding. The cages were centered relative to the walls of the chamber so that a mouse could move completely around each cage. Cages were constructed of clear plastic, with vertical ¼″ × 2″ slits every ½″ of the circumference so that mice could touch noses through the slits, but not move through the slits. One cage housed an age and sex-matched “stranger”, the other cage was empty, with the sides randomized on each trial (modified from ref. 42). Focal mice were placed into the center of the arena and given 1 min for familiarization. The subsequent 3 min were scored for % time the mouse moved more than 1″ toward the mouse-containing cage from the midline of the arena.

Novel object recognition, Day 1 (NOR1)

Mice went through three stages of novel object recognition (NOR) testing on day 1: For habituation, mice were placed in an empty 10″ × 18″ animal housing chamber. For familiarization, animals were placed in the same arena with 2 identical objects separated by 12″, centered with respect to the shorter arena dimension. For novel object recognition, one of the familiar objects was replaced with a novel object of similar size and complexity. Objects consisted of a 1.5″ wing nut, a 1.5″ bolt, or the wing nut fitted to the end of the bolt. Side placement and order of exposure were randomized. Each mouse was tested for 4 min per stage, and scored for time per half (for habituation), time attending to each object (scored when the object was within the front 90° of the mouse’s head and within one body length of the mouse’s head), and time touching each object with its face or paw. A 30–60 min interval separated testing between the stages43.

Novel object recognition, Day 2 (NOR2)

This test consisted of a single stage on the day following NOR1. The arena was arranged as for novel object exploration on day 1, but the novel object was different than the object used in day 1. Replacement side was randomized as was order of object introduction across days.

Examination of GR and BDNF Gene Methylation

Hippocampi were dissected from bisected snap-frozen brains. DNA was extracted using a protocol modified from the DNEasy Blood and Tissue kit (Qiagen, Hilden Germany, product # 69504), wherein samples were incubated with proteinase K overnight. Extracted DNA was quantified using a Nanodrop 1000 kit (Thermo Fischer Scientific, Waltham, MA) and 500 ng-1 ug of DNA were bisulfite converted using a Qiagen Epitect Bisulfite Kit (product # 59104) according to the manufacturer’s instructions. Methylation occurs largely at cytosines followed by guanines (termed CpG sites). Bisulfite treatment selectively replaces un-methylated cytosines with uracil, allowing methylated and un-methylated sites to be distinguished by Sanger Sequencing.

Bisulfite specific primers were designed using MethPrimer (Li Lab, Chinese Academy for Medical Sciences, Peking, China) and Bisulfite Primer Seeker (Zymo, Irvine, CA). Regions of BDNF and GR were chosen based on a consistent literature showing hypermethylation of the region 4 promoter32, 33, 44 and exon 17 promoter30, 31, 44, 45 respectively. PCR was carried out using a ZymoTaq HotStart Polymerase kit as follows: 10 min at 95 °C denaturation, followed by 35 cycles of 30 s at 95 °C, 30 s at annealing temperature, 30 s at 72 °C, and a 7 min 72 °C final extension. Primer sequences used are listed in Table 1.

Table 1 Primer sequences for methylation screens.

Primer bias, annealing temperatures, amplicon location, and CpG sites analyzed are presented in supplementary Fig. S1. PCR products were cleaned and sequenced by the Barker Sequencing Facility at University of California, Berkeley, using Axyprep MagPCR cleanup reagent according to the manufacturer’s instructions. All samples were bisulfite-converted and sequenced in duplicate.

Methylation was quantified as the nucleotide bases’ C/(C + T) peak height ratio as previously described46. Only samples with a conversion efficiency >88% were included in the analysis. Responders and non-responders were chosen based on the severity of sociality and weight phenotypes, (i.e., lowest weight and least social vs highest weight and most social for each group). Both sexes were examined, and no sex differences were apparent. For comparisons to the literature values of fold-change in methylation induced by early life stress, fold-changes in methylation were extracted from studies examining amplicons within GR 17 29, 30, 47 and BDNF IV32,33,34. The mean fold change between control and treatment groups across studies for each region was applied to our control rates to determine predicted methylation levels from our samples had they been obtained under early-life stress conditions.

Statistical Analysis

Statistical analysis was carried out in Matlab 2016a. Weight progression, methylation state, and all behaviors were assessed by Wilcoxon rank-sum non-parametric comparisons (reported with p-value only) or Kruskal-Wallis non-parametric tests for group effect (reported with group statistic and p-value), as appropriate. Post hoc tests were carried out with Dunn’s comparisons, with alpha value set to p < 0.05, reported only as above or below threshold. N’s for each condition were as follows for females and males respectively: C: 19, 20; CC: 44, 39; CS: 22, 21; SC: 19, 16; SS: 18, 21; S: 24, 21. Maternal behavior N’s were as follows: C: 10; CC: 11; CS: 7; SC: 7; SS: 8; S: 11. Clusters were constructed using kernel based distribution inference for each group, and visualized with contours over the projected surfaces at the level of 50% inclusion and 0% inclusion (the surface apex, which defines the cluster centroid). Cluster dissimilarity was calculated with a Euclidean distance technique48, using a Kruskal-Wallis test for effect of group on the distances of all C cluster points with its centroid versus the distances from all points in each other cluster to the C centroid. Figures were produced in Matlab 2016a and formatted in Adobe Photoshop CS5.

Results

Maternal care is compromised by the light environment and in utero condition of offspring

For dams exposed to repeated 6 h phase advances, physiological rhythms and locomotor activity showed chronic misalignment as females attempted to realign to the shifting LD cycle. This disruption is apparent in CBT (Fig. 2a,b) and LA (Supplemental Fig. 2a,b) raster plots. Despite the fact that the DCCD paradigm exposes dams to 2 additional subjective days during their pregnancy (48 h accumulated shifting), the number of calendar days for completion of pregnancy, as revealed by CBT, remains the same as for control mice (Fig. 2c,d).

Figure 2
figure 2

DCCD impacts maternal physiological rhythms. Color maps of intensity (A) reveal stable daily rhythms core body temperature (CBT) in sensor-implanted dams from control (B) and shifting (C) dams before onset of DCCD. Double-plotted actograms, with light-phase overlaid on the right half. After onset of DCCD, shifting dams show expected advances in their CBT rhythms in response to advances, which overall give shifting dams 2 additional daily cycles during their pregnancies (48 h accumulated advance). Nevertheless, pregnancies are of the same duration for control (D) and shifting (E) dams, as revealed by the stereotyped profile of CBT changes across pregnancy.

Maternal care was negatively affected by light environment and in utero condition of offspring. Whereas all C animals built complete nests and used all nesting material, 75% of S dams built incomplete nests (p = 9.3 × 10−3) and 33% did not use all the nesting material provided (p = 0.3, not significant) (Fig. 3a). After birth, pup retrieval times were also significantly increased by DCCD (χ2 = 20.85, p = 9 × 10−4) (Fig. 3b,c). Specifically, the in utero condition of the pups predicted the mother’s retrieval scores (Fig. 3b). Dams rearing pups that were shifted in utero took significantly longer to retrieve both the first pup (χ2 = 6.37, p = 0.01) and last pup (χ2 = 12.78, p = 4 × 10−4), regardless of maternal shifting history (Fig. 3c). Shifted mothers cross fostering pups from control mothers (SC dams) did not differ from SS and S dams on the first day of retrieval testing (p = 0.49), but took significantly longer than C and CC (p = 0.047). By contrast, CS dams took longer to retrieve pups than C and CC (p = 0.02), but did not differ from S and SS (p = 0.26). However, by day 5, CS pup retrievals were not significantly different from those of either control groups (C, CC, p = 0.06) or shifted groups (S, SS, p = 0.06). To summarize, pups that were shifted in utero start with a maternal retrieval disadvantage that progressively worsens over days, regardless of the postnatal maternal condition. Pups from control mothers eventually experience longer retrieval times if exposed to DCCD early in postnatal life, indicating that S and SS pups are negatively impacted by both circadian disruption in utero and poorer maternal care in early life.

Figure 3
figure 3

Maternal Behavior is altered by prenatal and postnatal DCCD. Before giving birth, 75% of S dams create nests with major structural deficits, and 33% do not use all nest material provided over the night of testing. By contrast, 0% of C dams exhibit these deficits in nest building (A). The condition of the pup in utero affects maternal pup retrieval behavior (BD). A 3-dimenstional plot showing the time interval from retrieval of 1st to 4th pup (labeled as such on S data), which defines a region shaded by condition (B, legend in E) across 5 sequential days of retrieval trials. Pups experiencing in utero disruption (S, SS, SC) show longer retrieval times than pups from in utero control conditions (C, CC, CS), regardless of rearing-dam’s condition. As a group, dams retrieving pups that experienced in utero disruption take longer to retrieve the first (C, left) and last (C, right) pup, and this effect is present across all five trial days. A significant effect is also found in total amount of time spent retrieving pups across all 5 days (D), with in utero-disrupted pups taking longer to be retrieved than pups without in utero disruption.

Postnatal disruption and poor maternal care lead to growth deficits but do not alter puberty onset

For C and S offspring, the week before midlife and the last week of maturation were chosen to assess adult growth. As expected, females weighed less than males regardless of condition both at p33 (p = 2.4 × 10−4) and in adulthood (p = 7.0 × 10−11). DCCD did not induce significant differences in body weight by p33 for either sex (females: p = 0.59, males: p = 0.53), but did cause lower adult weights in both females and males (p = 2.1 × 10−3, p = 2.2 × 10−3, respectively) (Fig. 4). Cross-fostered offspring were weighed only at p60 to assess adult weight (Table 2). An effect of group was also observed in cross-fostered litters (χ2 = 20.12, p = 0.0012 for males, χ2 = 11.11, p = 0.049 for females). In these cases, shifts during rearing more consistently lead to lower adult body mass than shifts in utero2 = 10.01, p = 0.0016 for males by rearing condition, χ2 = 4.63, p = 0.03 for males as C & CC vs. conditions with shifting; χ2 = 6.3, p = 0.01 for females by rearing condition; χ2 = 2.1, p = 0.15 for females as C & CC vs. conditions with shifting). No difference was found in the date of first vaginal opening (mean +/ s.d., C: day 38 +/ 6; S: day 37 +/ 4).

Figure 4
figure 4

DCCD negatively affects offspring morphological development. Male and female offspring do not differ in body mass half-way through postnatal development (A), but by onset of adulthood (p60) S (red), offspring are significantly underweight in both males (dashed lines) and females (solid lines). Life-time growth curve show in (A, inset).

Table 2 Body mass at p60, mean and standard deviation.

Early life circadian disruption leads to adult deficits in social and anxiety behaviors

In the Social Approach task, shifted offspring (S, SS, SC, and CS) spent significantly less time near the novel mouse than control offspring (C and CC) (χ2 = 80.99, p = 2.89 × 10−16; Fig. 5a). Post hoc analysis reveals that CS offspring were significantly more social than S, but less than C or CC (p < 0.05). No sex effect is apparent (p > 0.05). For the elevated plus maze, no difference was found for time in open vs. closed arms (mean time in open/closed arms: mean +/ s.d., C: 0.08 +/ 0.1, S: 0.2 +/ 0.4). There was, however, a significant effect of group in the frequency of stereotyped anxiety-like movements while in the maze (χ2 = 87.35, p = 2.42 × 10−17; Fig. 5b). No effect of sex was observed (p > 0.05). Post hoc analysis revealed that, generally circadian disruption whether pre- or postnatal, was associated with increased anxiety-like activity; S, SC CS, and SS offspring exhibited significantly more anxiety-like movements than C or CC animals, although CS and SS animals were significantly less active than S mice (p < 0.05). Finally, during both days of novel object recognition, all groups attended to the novel object more than 50% of the trial, and no effect of group or sex reached significance (% time near novel object: mean +/ s.d., Day 1: C: 63 +/ 18, S: 63 +/ 25; Day 2: C: 73 +/ 11, S: 67 +/ 17, Table 3).

Figure 5
figure 5

DCCD induces adult behavioral abnormalities. Mice exposed to DCCD exhibit a significant decrease in % time spent exploring a same-sex, same-age unfamiliar mouse (median +/ standard error) (A). DCCD offspring are hyperactive when exploring a novel elevated plus maze (median +/ standard error) (B). Note that SC offspring exhibit significantly greater separation from C and CC than do CS offspring. Individual socializing and hyperactivity scores plotted against each other reveal phenotypic separation by condition (C). Contours describing the 50% inclusion level for S and C are shaded. Contours for all groups (D) illustrate the significant separation of DCCD phenotypes from C phenotype. Groups with significant differences indicated by letters on bars; colors are consistent for groups across all plots.

Table 3 Novel object recognition % attending novel object, mean and standard deviation.

Combined behavioral phenotyping reveals novel effects of DCCD on adult behavior

Plotting elevated plus scores against social approach scores creates two-dimensional scatter plots for all individuals. Treatment groups cluster within this scatter, and comparison of distance between clusters revealed a separation of S and C populations, with cross-fostered groups exhibiting an intermediate phenotype (overlapping clusters, Fig. 5c). Cluster similarity tests reveal a significant effect of group on cluster separation (χ2 = 45.79, p = 1 × 10−8). Probing this effect with a post hoc analysis indicated that CC mice were not significantly separable from C mice, but CS, SC, SS, and S were all in significantly separate clusters from C mice (p < 0.05) (Fig. 5d), indicating that prenatal and postnatal shifting have additive, detrimental effects on adult behavior.

DCCD Does Not Mimic Early Life Stress

Birth weight, number of pups per litter, number of pups cannibalized, and cannibalization rate did not differ between C and S dams (p = 0.80, p = 0.86, p = 0.70, and p = 0.69, respectively). SS and CC mice did not differ in methylation rate on the promotor regions of BDNF IV (χ2 = 1.12, p = 0.77) or GR 172 = 1.97, p = 0.58) (Fig. 6a,b,d,e), with non-significant median changes of 3% and 11% overall, as compared to average reported increases for the same regions resulting from early-life stress of 95%32,33,34 and 135%29, 30, 47, respectively (Fig. 6c,f).

Figure 6
figure 6

DCCD does not lead to differential methylation patterns of the GR and BDNF genes. Birthweight for S and C litters do not differ (A). Litter size (B), number of pups cannibalized by the mother (C), or percent of litters cannibalized (D) are not affected by circadian disruption. Hypermethylation of BDNF V (E) and GR17 (G) regulatory regions of the genome is also absent in SS offspring. High and low responders were chosen from each group, and no apparent effect of phenotype on methylation rates.

Discussion

Our findings establish that DCCD leads to lasting changes in somatic development and adult behavior. Additionally, these effects depend on the developmental life stage at which disruption occurs. In utero disruptions induce more pronounced behavioral changes in adult offspring, regardless of neonatal or maternal condition. In contrast, adult body mass is influenced more by neonatal than in utero condition, suggesting actions of DCCD on multiple targets across development. Notably, we did not observe learning deficits in DCCD offspring, consistent with some earlier reports e.g., ref. 14. Our results establish that early life circadian disruption exerts life-stage-dependent effects, and that these effects are additive, suggesting that maternal and early life circadian rhythms play complex organizational roles in normal development.

Many types of learning show circadian modulation49. In the present study, behavioral testing was conducted during the day, which may affect how the mice responded to the stress of novel tests50. Although unlikely, it is possible that testing at this time masks subtle DCCD-induced learning deficits and testing in the early dark phase might permit unmasking49. Other studies have found learning deficits following gestation under constant light15, 16. In these cases, testing was conducted at variable times of day. Together, these findings suggest that the nature of the circadian disruption applied developmentally, and the time of day of testing, may influence those deficits that manifest.

Given that circadian disruptions can activate the stress axis, we speculated that ELS mechanisms may contribute to any observed changes. To explore this possibility, we examined markers of ELS in dams and offspring, but detected no signs of stress in any of the markers in any condition. DCCD dams did not have smaller or underweight litters and did not cannibalize their pups at an increased rate. Furthermore, CC and SS offspring exhibited levels of methylation of BDNF IV and GR 17 comparable to control mice, and control values in the literature29,30,31,32, 34, 47, 51, 52. Specifically, SS shows non-significant 3% and 11% increases from CC, respectively, as compared to 95% and 135% increases seen with ELS for the same regions29,30,31,32, 34, 47, 51, 52. The failure to implicate ELS mechanisms, taken together with a limited role of melatonin in this strain of mice, points to a unique mechanism by which DCCD negatively affects offspring growth and adult behavior.

The present findings also indicate that both in utero and early life development are uniquely vulnerable, with different downstream consequences. Specifically, disruptions in utero lead to markedly modified behavioral phenotypes that are not rescued by quality maternal care; by contrast, early postnatal circadian disruption induces nearly equivalent behavioral changes and a more marked body mass deficit. Differences in body mass do not appear to be due to differential maternal care as DCCD offspring diverge from control animals after weaning. The present study did not examine body composition or food intake, providing opportunity for future studies to determine specific systems impacted and potential changes in feeding and metabolism resulting from DCCD. The failure of stable maternal rhythms to rescue offspring outcomes are in contrast with both the ELS and maternal melatonin literature, in which neonatal interventions rescue circadian-22, 53,54,55 and ELS-induced effects in offspring56. In the present study, use of BALB/c mice minimizes the role of melatonin in all groups, suggesting that circadian disruption can negatively affect development independently of alterations in this hormone26, 27. Mothers communicate rhythmic information to developing pups through melatonin in their milk20, 22. Whether a dam producing robust melatonin rhythms would rescue the impact of prenatal DCCD is not clear. Our findings suggest that even when dams lack robust melatonin rhythms, DCCD still has a pronounced effect compared to controls. A more complete understanding is therefore needed of the mechanisms contributing to the effects of DCCD, both in utero and in postnatal development.

Endogenous circadian rhythms are not manifest until late in fetal development57 and developing offspring rely on metabolic and hormonal information communicated from mother to fetuses17, 20, 58. Misalignment of these cues may disrupt the organization of pup physiology. As fetuses develop, they become sensitive to light cues penetrating through the mother’s abdomen59. Thus, pups may not only experience circadian disruptions through mistimed maternal signals, but also through their own internal misalignment to the light:dark environment. Importantly, temporal structure governing behavior in the mother, and likely in the fetus, is provided by ultradian (rhythms < 24 h) as well as circadian mechanisms. Several physiological systems exhibit ultradian rhythms (URs) in the 1–3 h range, including the HPG axis60,61,62,63,64,65, the HPA axis66,67,68,69,70,71,72, and the suprachiasmatic nucleus73, 74 – the principal orchestrating brain area for circadian rhythms. The central dopaminergic axis exhibits URs in the 1–3 h range75, and may influence CBT as well through modulation of activity and appetitive behaviors, such as eating and drinking (reviewed in ref. 76). We recently documented that maternal UR power early in pregnancy is predictive of pregnancy success40. Therefore, it is possible that URs play important roles in normal pup development. Fetal URs are understudied but do exist77 and may provide a substrate through which temporal disruptions manifest as disrupted signaling within the developing fetal physiological network78.

In summary, chronic circadian disruption during gestation affects maternal behavior and the behavioral phenotype of their offspring in adulthood, independent of ELS. These effects depend on the timing of developmental circadian disruption. Circadian disruptions are ubiquitous in modern society, affected by the modern lighting environment and negatively impacting health from in utero development through old age79,80,81. As of 2004, 12.6% of women over the age of 16 in the US work force were shift workers82. Given the lasting negative impacts on female reproductive health83, circadian disruption represents a highly relevant environmental threat to both women and their children. Our behavioral findings of hyperactivity and social avoidance are consistent with patterns seen in human adolescents. Sleep disruption in adolescents is associated with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder84 and autism spectrum disorder85. Diagnoses for both have been on the rise over the past several decades86,87,88. Our findings support the hypothesis that increased circadian disruption early in life, as in portable electronic devices and light pollution, may play a role in these increases in incidence. Large datasets of mothers and newborns (i.e., nuMoM2b89) are excellent potential sources of information about how DCCD may affect children. The present findings underscore the need for increased investigation of the effects of temporal disruption on development, both in model systems and in humans.