Abstract
Transport of coral reef fish larvae is driven by advection in ocean currents and larval swimming. However, for swimming to be advantageous, larvae must use external stimuli as guides. One potential stimulus is “odor” emanating from settlement sites (e.g., coral reefs), signaling the upstream location of desirable settlement habitat. However, specific chemicals used by fish larvae have not been identified. Dimethyl sulfide (DMS) is produced in large quantities at coral reefs and may be important in larval orientation. In this study, a choice-chamber (shuttle box) was used to assess preference of 28 pre-settlement stage larvae from reef fish species for seawater with DMS. Swimming behavior was examined by video-tracking of larval swimming patterns in control and DMS seawater. We found common responses to DMS across reef fish taxa - a preference for water with DMS and change in swimming behavior - reflecting a switch to “exploratory behavior”. An open water species displayed no response to DMS. Affinity for and swimming response to DMS would allow a fish larva to locate its source and enhance its ability to find settlement habitat. Moreover, it may help them locate prey accumulating in fronts, eddies, and thin layers, where DMS is also produced.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
The early life history of most reef fishes is a multi-stage process during which they are capable of long-distance dispersal; however, the behaviors influencing dispersal are poorly understood. Immediately post-hatch, a larva possesses weak sensory capabilities and limited mobility, thus its transport is presumably passive1. As the larva develops it swims in an active2 and directed3 manner, utilizing its impressive swimming and sensory abilities4. Furthermore, biophysical models of larval transport show that the behaviorally-mediated swimming during this stage increases local retention and enhances overall settlement success5, 6. As a result, studying the complex behavioral responses of larvae to their sensory environment has emerged as a major component of understanding marine dispersal and population connectivity4, focusing on chemical7,8,9,10,11, auditory12, 13, and celestial14, 15 stimuli.
There is strong physiological and evolutionary evidence to suggest that olfaction is the most primitive and universal sense, and that it is intricately linked to odor-tracking and the use of a cognitive odor map16, 17. It is not always clear whether olfaction (e.g., chemoreception at the olfactory epithelium) or gustation (e.g., chemoreception on internal or external taste buds) is at work, but both chemosensory systems are well developed in fishes18. There is a long history of research on settlement-inducing chemoreception in marine invertebrates19, as well as on chemically-induced settlement in fish larvae20. Several marine fishes have even evolved the capacity to utilize distinct chemical signals at impressively large spatial scales, specifically salmon21, 22 and sharks23,24,25,26. Nonetheless, little is known about what chemicals play an important role, how perception of these chemicals affects swimming patterns, or mechanisms for their effective use in navigation27.
The literature suggesting that larval fish also utilize chemosensory systems for large scale navigation continues to grow. Researchers have hypothesized that “tidal halos” extending several kilometers from settlement sites, which are rich in chemical compounds carried from the reef, may provide important cues for navigation and settlement at varying spatial scales7. Population genetic substructure provides additional support for this hypothesis28, as does the activation of oriented swimming when larvae enter such a tidal plume10. Also, like salmon, some coral reef fish appear capable of imprinting as early as embryonic development and into the first 24 hours post-hatch29. The expression of this imprinting seems to vary throughout ontogeny, with chemical preference sharply transitioning from that of offshore waters to that of settlement habitat in one case30. It seems very clear that settlement-stage larvae of many reef fishes can and do discriminate chemical environments, preferring those of appropriate settlement habitat and of conspecifics9, 20, 31, 32. However, despite this breadth of literature, there is only a weak understanding of exactly what chemicals appeal to larvae (e.g., plant or coral “leachate”).
A logical candidate for a specific chemical involved in larval fish navigation is dimethyl sulfide (DMS), a particularly well-known “infochemical” in the marine environment. DMS has been well studied in the scientific community as far back as 1980’s for its potential role in climate regulation33. Since then it has gained attention in the biological community for its role as a chemical attractant34. And while most of this work has focused on marine air-breathers such as seabirds35, 36, penguins37, turtles38, and seals39, recent work has also demonstrated an affinity in whale sharks40. Dimethylsulfoniopropionate (DMSP), a precursor to DMS, has been shown to be a foraging cue for adult reef fishes41. Since many larval fishes have highly-developed olfactory systems even before juvenile metamorphosis42, it is reasonable to assume that larval fish would have an affinity for similar chemicals.
Moreover, high concentrations of DMS(P) are produced by zooxanthellae43, corals44, benthic algae45, and other coral reef residents. Surface concentrations of DMS(P) are higher near certain coral reefs than anywhere else in the marine environment (as high as nearly 19 μM [DMS] in one area), particularly in the mucus ropes and surface films exported from the reef as a part of the tidal halo46. Midwater samples of DMS(P) vary annually, but average from 20 nM to 120 nM47, with events such as coral spawning leading to temporal spikes in background concentrations. One further important consideration is that all methods for collecting water samples necessarily lead to an averaging of the DMS(P) concentration over the volume collected. In a chemically heterogeneous environment, chemical concentrations may be much higher at finer spatial scales, and small planktonic organisms are likely to perceive these higher concentrations48. In any case, it is clear that, despite considerable temporal and between-reef variation, concentrations are consistently higher around corals than in the pelagic environment. Thus, one would expect that an ability to detect and an affinity for DMS(P) would enhance the recruitment of larval fish at their reef destinations.
In the present study, we utilize a shuttle box (Loligo Systems) adapted to serve as a choice chamber, with DMS-containing seawater on one side and control seawater on the other. We hypothesized that the reef fish larvae would display a preference for the water containing DMS and, further, that their swimming behavior would be altered when they are inside of this water. Experiments were conducted on a range of different reef fish families to determine if observations were consistent for fish larvae with varying ecological traits. Further, in order to assess the relative importance of DMS in indicating reef habitat or food abundance, experiments were also conducted with larvae of mahi-mahi, Coryphaena hippurus, a tropical epipelagic fish that spends all its life cycle in the open ocean.
Results
Reef Fish Larvae
Of the 28 reef fish larvae tested, 23 (82.1%) displayed preference for the side containing DMS, including 16 of the 19 (84.2%) Stegastes spp larvae (Table 1). Pooling all trials, including those with a control-side preference, larvae spent 57.5% of their time on the odor side, 1.9x higher than the 30.3% spent on the control side (Fig. 1). The remaining 12.2% was spent inside of the center pathway.
The normalized count-difference variable was used to statistically asses the group-level preference for DMS odor. Figure 2 shows the relative probability distribution of this variable. The right-shifted mean and right-skewed distribution indicates that there was indeed a group preference for DMS odor, and this was statistically significant (n = 28, α = 0.05, p < 0.0001, t = 4.53, df = 27, one-tailed one-sample t-test). Data met assumptions of normality and independence.
There were also clear differences in behavior of reef fish larvae when in water containing DMS odor. First, we found differences in the activity level (i.e., mean of instantaneous velocities) of larvae while in odorous water (n = 28, α = 0.05, p = 0.02, t = −2.43, df = 27, paired t-test, Fig. 3); that is, larvae were swimming slower in water containing DMS. Patterns of swimming behavior were also different. Specifically, the normalized turning frequency was significantly higher (n = 28, α = 0.05, p = 0.0002, t = 4.29, df = 27, paired t-test) when larvae were inside of the water containing DMS (Fig. 4). Data met assumptions of normality and independence.
For analysis of group-level behavior, the 28 reef fish larvae of varying taxa were pooled. We justify this decision by the apparent consistency in preference for DMS across taxa. Statistics and complementary power analyses were conducted for the most common species, S. partitus; the most common genus, Stegastes spp; as well as the remaining taxa pooled (see Supplementary Figs S1, S2 and S3). We found statistical significance for all groups at even the α = 0.01 level (see Supplementary material). Further, we did a second analysis extracting only the mean value of the normalized count difference from each of the taxa tested (listed in Table 1). In this way each of the groups was weighted evenly. Results were statistically significant and consistent with the pooled data (Supplementary Figs S4, S5 and S6).
Finally, post-analysis inspection of the video data revealed some common patterns in the behavior of larvae while inside and outside of the odor (Video S7). While in control seawater, larvae tended to display either: (1) swimming directly from the center pathway to the opposite wall of the control chamber, making a 180° turn, and returning to the center pathway or to the other chamber; (2) swimming in a circular motion along the inner circumference of the control chamber. When in the chamber containing DMS odor, larvae displayed a zig-zagging behavior, a characteristic pattern of “search behavior”49, 50.
Mahi-mahi Larvae
Of the 23 mahi-mahi larvae tested, 14 (60.8%) displayed preference for the side containing DMS. Pooling all trials, including those with a control-side preference, larvae spent 56.2% of their time on the odor side, 1.3x higher than the 42.7% spent on the control side. The remaining 1.1% was spent inside of the center pathway.
The normalized count-difference variable was used to statistically asses the group-level preference for DMS odor. Figure 5 shows the relative probability distribution of this variable, which does not indicate a significant group-level preference for DMS (n = 23, V = 161.5, α = 0.05, p = 0.24, Wilcoxon signed rank test). There were also no differences in swimming behavior between mahi-mahi larvae in control seawater versus DMS containing water. Both activity level (n = 23, V = 84, α = 0.05, p = 0.188, Wilcoxon signed rank test, Fig. 6) and normalized turning frequency (n = 23, V = 68, α = 0.05, p = 0.6788, Wilcoxon signed rank test, Fig. 7) were similar in control water and water containing DMS. Nonparametric statistics were used, as the tested variables were not normally distributed.
Discussion
We demonstrate that reef fish larvae, across taxa, display a preference for seawater containing DMS. Furthermore, the presence of DMS in the water elicits a different swimming behavior than water without DMS, causing them to swim slower and make larger turns more frequently, consistent with observations in Paris et al.10 when larvae were inside of tidal halos. We did not, however, observe any preference for DMS in larvae of the epipelagic mahi-mahi, nor did we observe any change in behavior.
The conclusion that reef fish larvae generally possess an affinity for DMS is a significant one. Previous studies have been successful in demonstrating a preference for a chemical suite7, 10, 28 or in showing that a single species prefers a particular “leachate”9, but this is the first example of cross-taxon reef fish preference for a specific isolated compound. Over the past decade there is an impressive amount of literature displaying the potency of DMS as a universal marine infochemical35,36,37,38,39,40,41, and this study marks the addition of reef fish larvae to the growing list of attracted organisms.
This study also marks the first use of a shuttle box to assess chemical preference of an aquatic organism. Typically, preference experiments with larval fish have utilized a two-channel flume7, 11, 28. The primary benefit of this new method is the ability to simultaneously test the appeal of (or aversion to) a chemical and its effect on the movement patterns of a larva. In this way we are able to assess response to the odor in addition to preference for it. There are, however, a few important remarks regarding the use of a shuttle box for this application: (1) often larvae spend a considerable amount of time in the center pathway connecting the control seawater and the DMS containing seawater. This is likely due to the reduced flow in this region, something which, under stress, may be more important than chemosensory appeal; (2) certain larvae are unsuitable for testing with the shuttle box. Some Ostraciidae and Tetraodontidae larvae, for example, simply did not move. These larvae would stay on the side of the shuttle box they were placed in until the experiment ended, regardless of the length of the experiment. Similar behavior has been observed in subadult Lutjanus griseus inside of a shuttle box designed to investigate salinity preference51. Some Gerreidae and Clupeidae larvae, on the other hand, would energetically swim into the nearest wall as soon as they were placed into the shuttle box. This places a restriction on which taxa one can reasonably test using this device. We suggest this is also the case for other methods investigating chemical preference in larvae, based on personal communications, and that this is generally underreported. (3) Nearly all larvae spent some time in the control water, despite their significant preference for DMS water. This is likely a result of odor-induced search behavior, whereby the larvae were incited to find the source.
Like other organisms with DMS-affinity, fish larvae may use the presence of DMS as an indicator for nearby food sources34. Specific to fish larvae, high concentrations of DMS also suggest the nearby presence of suitable settlement habitat. It is unclear which of these two functions facilitated the evolution of DMS-preference, but it is noteworthy that the concentrations of DMS are higher in coral reef exports (i.e., mucus ropes and surface slicks) than anywhere else in the marine environment, and that the background concentrations are particularly low elsewhere in the tropics45, 46. For example, mucus ropes may contain anywhere from 60 to over 12000 times higher DMS concentrations (132 nM–17909 μM) than background (2.2 nM), suggesting DMS may be more useful in finding the reef than in finding prey. Our results here are also telling. While reef fish larvae displayed a strong preference for DMS, mahi mahi did not. This is not surprising, as DMS levels are substantially lower in the epipelagic environment than near reefs.
However, it is also possible that DMS actually serves both roles, either simultaneously or at different points in development of the larvae through its pelagic duration. Indeed, DMS is actually a product of the degradation of DMSP, a chemical which several species of adult fish have a documented preference for41. It is unclear whether DMS or DMSP is the “more potent attractant”, if they serve different roles, or if the ratio of the two chemicals is in some way important information for early or late stage larvae.
The idea of a widely-appealing chemical is relatively new and needs to be discussed in the context of perhaps the longest-standing hypothesis in the chemical ecology of larval fish: the imprinting hypothesis. There is strong evidence, both physiological29 and behavioral52, that supports the idea of chemical imprinting early in fish development, and this study does not intend to refute those findings. Indeed, imprinting on specific microhabitat odors likely plays a very important role in the selection of settlement habitat at a local scale. We speculate that DMS, which is more ubiquitously present at a reef, and would be more broadly exported from it, may be more important as a large-scale infochemical. However, we point out that damselfish of the Stegastes genus made up the bulk of our sample, demonstrating that benthic spawners, those most likely to imprint, still display an affinity for DMS. It is possible that DMS, despite its ubiquitous presence on the reef, is actually implicated in the process of imprinting, either in isolation or as one of a suite of chemicals.
This study is an important step forward in understanding the chemosensory navigation of fish larvae and allows us to confidently pursue additional questions. For example: (1) what is the threshold of detection for DMS? or (2) what is the mechanism for navigating using odor plumes? Clear evidence of chemical preferences certainly suggests a mechanism exists, but its nature is unknown. The spatial and temporal distributions of chemical concentrations are notoriously complex and composed of highly-concentrated filaments interspersed with fluid containing little or no chemical53. The structure depends on the dominant flow, turbulent diffusion, topography, etc.48 and creates a serious problem for traditional gradient-based odor-tracking strategies. Knowing the threshold of detection for DMS will allow observations of behavior to be compared to models of behavioral responses.
Which strategies are being used by minute fish larvae to find their settlement sites from the open ocean remains a mystery. Future studies should use DMS, at relevant concentrations above threshold of detection, to simultaneously record odor plume structure and larval fish kinematics. This will provide valuable insight on the mechanism being used to find the source of the odor. The identification of DMS as a universal infochemical for larval reef fish brings us closer to realizing this goal.
Methods
Study Organisms
Larval fish used in the experiment were obtained by one of two ways: (1) Wild-caught utilizing light traps, or (2) Reared in the University of Miami Experimental Hatchery (UMEH) at the Rosenstiel School of Marine and Atmospheric Science. Light traps (CARE, EcoOcean; BellaMare, LLC) were set on fore reef off the coast of Key Biscayne, FL, USA (25°70478N; 80°09790W and 25°67410N; 80°09420W). Traps were set at dusk and fish were retrieved from traps at dawn. All experiments were conducted within 24 hours of larval collection, and all larvae were drip-acclimated to the water from the seawater system in the Marine Technology & Life Sciences Seawater Research Building at University of Miami’s Rosenstiel School. Identifications were made using morphological key characteristics54 and incidental mortalities allowed certain specimens to be saved for detailed taxonomy. All other individuals were freed in the nearby bay following their behavioral trial, in accordance with the guidelines of University of Miami IACUC protocol #14-216. All methods were carried out in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations, defined in said protocol, approved by University of Miami’s Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee. A total of 28 larvae belonging to 10 taxa were caught and used in experiments, (Table 1). The two most common larvae were damselfish belonging to the Stegastes genus, Stegastes partitus (n = 11) and Stegastes diencaeus (n = 7). All larvae were wild-caught opportunistically from light-traps, and thus experiments were conducted on a wide array of fish families. Only one genus of damselfish, Stegastes, had substantial representation in our dataset (n = 17).
Mahi-mahi larvae were reared from fertilized eggs obtained from volitional spawning wild mahi-mahi broodstock maintained at the UMEH facility. Larval rearing protocols were similar to those described by Benetti et al.55 and Kraul (1993)56 for rearing of cobia (Rachycentron canadum) and mahi-mahi, respectively. Eggs were hatched and larvae were reared in 2.4 m3 cylindrical fiberglass tanks. Beginning on day 3 post hatch the rearing water was inoculated with Nannochloropsis microalgae paste (Reed Mariculture Inc.) at a level to maintain a slight green color in the water column (~250,000 cells mL−1). Enriched rotifers (Briachionus plicatilis) and Artemia nauplii were fed to the larvae over the course of the rearing period, and water exchange averaged 10 turnovers per day in the rearing tanks. Larvae used for this study were randomly removed from the larval rearing tanks using a submerged 1-L beaker to avoid physical contact and they were subsequently held in 15-L vessels containing UV-sterilized aerated seawater in the hours leading up to testing.
Shuttle box System
A novel methodology utilizing a shuttle box design (Loligo Systems, Fig. 8) was used in order to assess larval chemical preference. The system consists of two 10 cm diameter circular chambers connected by a short (<3 cm) narrow pathway allowing free movement of a larva. Each of the chambers is fitted with two tangential ports to attach inflow and outflow tubing. This positioning and the low flow rate (0.5 mL/s) produce smooth circular flow inside of the chambers and serves to greatly reduce mixing between the two sides. An iPumps i150 peristaltic pump with a B4 R6 four channel, 6 roller pump head (iPumps Limited) was used to ensure precise and equal flow rates at each of the inflows and outflows. Before any set of experiments a dye test was always conducted to ensure that there was no substantial mixing between the two chambers (Fig. 8).
Experimental Protocol
Odor preference experiments were conducted on one fish larvae at a time and lasted a total of 20 minutes. First, a solution of 340 nM DMS was prepared through serial dilution of a >99% standard (Sigma-Aldrich). Next, the shuttle box was filled with approximately 175 mL of water in each chamber and a larva was randomly introduced to one side. The experiment began when the pump was activated, introducing seawater with 340 nM DMS on side and seawater with 0 nM DMS on the other; sides were chosen at random. 340 nM was chosen as the stock concentration based on the maximum measurement of DMS in sediment pore waters at Kelso Reef, a mid-shelf oligotrophic patch reef in the central Great Barrier Reef46. Stock solutions for substantially larger aquatic organisms have varied from 100 nM for whale sharks40 and adult reef fishes41 in situ to 200 nM for adult reef fishes in lab flumes47. Our concentration was slightly higher, as a smaller organism should be able to sample higher concentrations at finer spatial scales. Exact ecologically-relevant concentrations are unknown, as there are no studies specifically addressing the spatial/temporal variation of DMS concentrations at varying distances from coral reefs, and we present a best guess at a relevant concentration given our current knowledge. The concentrations of both stock solutions were verified using vapor-generation and chemoluminescence-detection57 and a new stock DMS solution was created every other experiment to account for the naturally declining concentrations of DMS in solution over time.
The 20 minutes of each trial was split into 5 minutes of acclimation and 15 minutes of experiment. The acclimation period allowed the larvae to adjust to their new environment and also for the concentration of DMS to accumulate inside one of the chambers (Supplementary Fig. S6); at 5 minutes the concentration inside the chamber is roughly 200 nM, representing the mean concentration of the benthos near Kelso Reef46. No one was permitted to enter the experimental room during these 20 minutes, and the movement of the larva inside of the shuttle box was recorded by a video camera set to 30 frames per second. At the end of the trial the larva was removed, the shuttle box and tubing was thoroughly rinsed with ethanol and seawater, and the sides for larval introduction and odor were randomly chosen for the next experiment.
Movement Analysis
Video data was subsampled using VirtualDub (version 1.10.4, http://www.virtualdub.org) to record the position of the larva inside the shuttle box at 1 second intervals. Custom software was designed integrating R (R Core Team 2014, version 3.1.2, R Foundation for Statistical Computing, http://www.R-project.org) and ImageJ (W.S. Rasband, version 1.46r, NIH, Bethesda, MD; http://rsb.info.nih.gov/ih/) to process each image sequence. Briefly, the user manually selected the boundary of both chambers, used a portion of the shuttle box with known length to calibrate pixels to centimeters, and then selected the approximate position of the larva’s head in every frame. The program used these positional records to determine the amount of time spent in each chamber, instantaneous velocities, and turning angles. Records of larval position in the center pathway were excluded from analysis.
Statistical Analysis
A second level of statistical analysis was performed in R to examine group behavioral trends on both sides of the shuttle box. First, we investigated the average amount of time spent on the two sides of the chamber. Next, we calculated the “count difference”, which is, for each individual trial, the number of positional records in the odorous chamber minus the number of positional records in the control chamber. This count difference variable, if there is no overall preference for the odor, should be normally distributed with a mean of 0. A one-sample t-test (α = 0.01) was used to test the hypothesis that the mean was indeed equal to 0 when data met the assumptions of normality, independence, and random sampling. For data that was not normally distributed, a nonparametric Wilcoxon signed rank test was used. Because positional records where the larvae is in the center pathway were excluded, this variable was normalized to account for the different number of records between trials (i.e., some larva spend more time in the center pathway than others). This was accomplished by removing those records from the dataset and dividing the count difference by the number of records remaining.
In addition to side preference, behavioral differences while in each side of the chamber were assessed. Specifically, the mean swimming speed (which, for each trial, is the mean of the instantaneous speeds) and the variation in swimming speed were compared. The frequency of large turns (i.e., greater than 45°) was also compared. This latter variable was normalized to account for the fact that larva may have spent more time on one side than another, increasing the frequency of turns on that side without indicating a change in behavior. This was accomplished by dividing the turning frequency on each side by the total number of records on that side. A paired t-test (α = 0.01) was used to test the hypothesis that mean turning frequencies and activity levels were equal in seawater containing DMS and control seawater when our data met assumptions of normality, independence, and equal variance between groups, otherwise a nonparametric paired Wilcoxon signed rank test was used. Finally, while tracking was done without knowledge of the odorous side, a qualitative assessment of swimming behavior on each side was used to explain quantitative findings.
References
Leis, J. M. Are larvae of demersal fishes plankton or nekton? Adv. Mar. Biol. 51, 57–141 (2006).
Fisher, R. Swimming speeds of larval coral reef fishes: impacts on self-recruitment and dispersal. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 285, 223–232 (2005).
Leis, J. M. et al. In situ orientation of fish larvae can vary among regions. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 537, 191–203 (2015).
Leis, J. M., Siebeck, U. & Dixson, D. L. How Nemo finds home: the neuroecology of dispersal and of population connectivity in larvae of marine fishes. Integr. Comp. Biol. 51, 826–43 (2011).
Armsworth, P. R. Modelling the swimming response of late stage larval reef fish to different stimuli. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 195, 231–247 (2000).
Staaterman, E., Paris, C. B. & Helgers, J. Orientation behavior in fish larvae: a missing piece to Hjort’s critical period hypothesis. J. Theor. Biol. 304, 188–96 (2012).
Atema, J., Kingsford, M. & Gerlach, G. Larval reef fish could use odour for detection, retention and orientation to reefs. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 241, 151–160 (2002).
Kingsford, M. J. et al. Sensory Environments, Larval Abilities, and Local Self-Recruitment. Bull. Mar. Sci. 70, 309–340 (2002).
Dixson, D. L. et al. Terrestrial chemical cues help coral reef fish larvae locate settlement habitat surrounding islands. Ecol. Evol. 1, 586–95 (2011).
Paris, C. B. et al. Reef odor: a wake up call for navigation in reef fish larvae. PLoS One 8, e72808 (2013).
Dixson, D. L., Abrego, D. & Hay, M. E. Chemically mediated behavior of recruiting corals and fishes: A tipping point that may limit reef recovery. Science (80-.) 345, 892–897 (2014).
Simpson, S. D., Meekan, M., Montgomery, J., Mccauley, R. & Jeffs, A. Homeward Sound. Science (80-.) 308, 221 (2005).
Montgomery, J. C., Jeffs, A., Simpson, S. D., Meekan, M. & Tindle, C. Sound as an orientation cue for the pelagic larvae of reef fishes and decapod crustaceans. Adv. Mar. Biol. 51, 143–96 (2006).
Mouritsen, H., Atema, J., Kingsford, M. J. & Gerlach, G. Sun Compass Orientation Helps Coral Reef Fish Larvae Return to Their Natal Reef. PLoS One 8, e66039 (2013).
Berenshtein, I. et al. Polarized light sensitivity and orientation in coral reef fish post-larvae. PLoS One 9, e88468 (2014).
Jacobs, L. F. From chemotaxis to the cognitive map: the function of olfaction. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 109(Suppl), 10693–700 (2012).
Gomez-marin, A., Duistermars, B. J. & Frye, M. A. Mechanisms of odor-tracking: multiple sensors for enhanced perception and behavior Introduction Modes of Chemotaxis Evidence for the Use of Stereo Olfaction in Insects and. Front. Sci. 1–23 (2010).
Hara, T. J. Olfaction and gustation in fish: an overview. Acta Phsiol Scand 152, 207–217 (1994).
Pawlik, J. R. Chemical Ecology of the Settlement of Benthic Marine-Invertebrates. Oceanogr. Mar. Biol. 30, 273–335 (1992).
Lecchini, D., Shima, J., Banaigs, B. & Galzin, R. Larval sensory abilities and mechanisms of habitat selection of a coral reef fish during settlement. Oecologia 143, 326–34 (2005).
Dittman, A. H., Quinn, T. P. & Nevitt, G. A. Timing of imprinting to natural and artificial odors by coho salmon (Oncorhynchus kisutch). Can. J. Fish. Aquat. Sci. 53, 434–442 (1996).
Ueda, H. Physiological mechanisms of imprinting and homing migration in Pacific salmon Oncorhynchus spp. J. Fish Biol 81, 543–558 (2012).
Gardiner, J. M. & Atema, J. The function of bilateral odor arrival time differences in olfactory orientation of sharks. Curr. Biol. 20, 1187–91 (2010).
Gardiner, J. M., Atema, J., Hueter, R. E. & Motta, P. J. Multisensory Integration and Behavioral Plasticity in Sharks from Different Ecological Niches. PLoS One 9, e93036 (2014).
DeBose, J. L. & Nevitt, G. A. The use of odors at different spatial scales: comparing birds with fish. J. Chem. Ecol. 34, 867–81 (2008).
Nosal, A. P., Chao, Y., Farrara, J. D., Chai, F. & Hastings, P. A. Olfaction Contributes to Pelagic Navigation in a Coastal Shark. PLoS One 11, e0143758 (2016).
Staaterman, E. & Paris, C. B. Modelling larval fish navigation: the way forward. ICES J. Mar. Sci. 71, 918–924 (2013).
Gerlach, G., Atema, J., Kingsford, M. J., Black, K. P. & Miller-Sims, V. Smelling home can prevent dispersal of reef fish larvae. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 104, 858–63 (2007).
Arvedlund, M., Larsen, K. & Winsor, H. The embryonic development of the olfactory system in Amphiprion melanopus (Perciformes: Pomacentridae) related to the host imprinting hypothesis. J. Mar. Biol. Assoc. United Kingdon 80, 1103–1110 (2000).
Dixson, D. L., Munday, P. L., Pratchett, M. & Jones, G. P. Ontogenetic changes in responses to settlement cues by Anemonefish. Coral Reefs 30, 903–910 (2011).
Radford, C. A., Sim-Smith, C. J. & Jeffs, A. G. Can larval snapper, Pagrus auratus, smell their new home? Mar. Freshw. Res 63, 898 (2012).
Dixson, D. L. et al. Coral reef fish smell leaves to find island homes. Proc. Biol. Sci. 275, 2831–9 (2008).
Charlson, R. J., Lovelock, J. E., Andreae, M. O. & Warren, S. G. Oceanic phytoplankton, atmospheric sulphur, cloud albedo and climate. Nature 326, 655–661 (1987).
Nevitt, G. A. The Neuroecology of Dimethyl Sulfide: A Global-Climate Regulator Turned Marine Infochemical. Integr. Comp. Biol. 51, 819–825 (2011).
Savoca, M. S. & Nevitt, G. A. Evidence that dimethyl sulfide facilitates a tritrophic mutualism between marine primary producers and top predators. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 111, 4157–4161 (2014).
Nevitt, G. A. & Bonadonna, F. Sensitivity to dimethyl sulphide suggests a mechanism for olfactory navigation by seabirds. Biol. Lett. 1, 303–305 (2005).
Amo, L., Rodríguez-Gironés, M. & Barbosa, A. Olfactory detection of dimethyl sulphide in a krill-eating Antarctic penguin. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 474, 277–285 (2013).
Endres, C. S. & Lohmann, K. J. Perception of dimethyl sulfide (DMS) by loggerhead sea turtles: a possible mechanism for locating high-productivity oceanic regions for foraging. J. Exp. Biol. 215, 3535–3538 (2012).
Kowalewsky, S., Dambach, M., Mauck, B. & Dehnhardt, G. High olfactory sensitivity for dimethyl sulphide in harbour seals. Biol. Lett. 2, 106–109 (2006).
Dove, A. D. M. Foraging and Ingestive Behaviors of Whale Sharks, Rhincodon typus, in Response to Chemical Stimulus Cues. Biol. Bull. 228, 65–74 (2015).
DeBose, J. L., Lema, S. C. & Nevitt, G. A. Dimethylsulfoniopropionate as a foraging cue for reef fishes. Science 319, 1356 (2008).
Lara, M. R. Development of the nasal olfactory organs in the larvae, settlement-stages and some adults of 14 species of Caribbean reef fishes (Labridae, Scaridae, Pomacentridae). Mar. Biol. 154, 51–64 (2008).
Swan, H. B., Jones, G. B. & Deschaseaux, E. Dimethylsulfide, Climate and Coral Reef Ecosystems. Proc. 12th Int. Coral Reef Symp. 9–13 (2012).
Raina, J.-B. et al. DMSP biosynthesis by an animal and its role in coral thermal stress response. Nature 502, 677–680 (2013).
Broadbent, A. D., Jones, G. B. & Jones, R. J. DMSP in Corals and Benthic Algae from the Great Barrier Reef. Estuar. Coast. Shelf Sci. 55, 547–555 (2002).
Broadbent, A. D. & Jones, G. B. DMS and DMSP in mucus ropes, coral mucus, surface films and sediment pore waters from coral reefs in the Great Barrier Reef. Mar. Freshw. Res 55, 849–855 (2004).
DeBose, J. Investigating the Role of Dimethylsulfoniopropionate (DMSP) As an Aggregation Cue for Coral Reef and Reef-Associated Fishes (2007).
Moore, P. & Crimaldi, J. Odor landscapes and animal behavior: Tracking odor plumes in different physical worlds. J. Mar. Syst. 49, 55–64 (2004).
Kozlowski, C., Yopak, K., Voigt, R. & Atema, J. An initial study on the effects of signal intermittency on the odor plume tracking behavior of the American lobster, Homarus americanus. Biol. Bull. 201, 274–6 (2001).
Weissburg, M. J. & Zimmer-Faust, R. K. Odor Plumes and How Blue Crabs Use Them in Finding Prey. J. exp. Biol 197, 349–375 (1994).
Serrano, X., Grosell, M. & Serafy, J. E. Salinity selection and preference of the grey snapper Lutjanus griseus: Field and laboratory observations. J. Fish Biol. 76, 1592–1608 (2010).
Dixson, D. L. et al. Experimental evaluation of imprinting and the role innate preference plays in habitat selection in a coral reef fish. Oecologia 174, 99–107 (2014).
Crimaldi, J. P., Wiley, M. B. & Koseff, J. R. The relationship between mean and instantaneous structure in turbulent passive scalar plumes. J. Turbul. 3, 1–23 (2002).
Richards, W. J. Early Stages of Atlantic Fishes: An Identification Guide For the Western Central North Atlantic (Taylor & Franic Group, 2005).
Benetti, D. D. et al. Intensive larval husbandry and fingerling production of cobia Rachycentron canandum. Acquaculture 281, 22–27 (2008).
Kraul, S. Larviculture of the mahimahi Coryphaena hippurus in Hawaii, USA. J. World Acquacult. Soc. 24, 410–421 (1993).
Nagahata, T., Kajiwara, H., Ohira, S. I. & Toda, K. Simple field device for measurement of dimethyl sulfide and dimethylsulfoniopropionate in natural waters, based on vapor generation and chemiluminescence detection. Anal. Chem. 85, 4461–4467 (2013).
Acknowledgements
We thank Romain Chaput for his assistance collecting fish larvae, Andrew Kough and Joe Serafy for advice on statistical analysis and critical reading of the manuscript. M. Grosell is a Maytag professor of Ichthyology and is supported by a National Science Foundation (NSF) award (1146695). This research is supported by a NSF-OCE awards (1260424 and 1459156) to C. B. Paris.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
M.F. collected larvae, designed the experiment, conducted experiments, analyzed data, and wrote the manuscript. C.B.P. collected larvae, designed the experiment, funded the research, and critically reviewed the manuscript. M.G., J.S., and D.B. provided the lab-reared mahi-mahi larvae and critically reviewed the manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing Interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Electronic supplementary material
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Foretich, M.A., Paris, C.B., Grosell, M. et al. Dimethyl Sulfide is a Chemical Attractant for Reef Fish Larvae. Sci Rep 7, 2498 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-02675-3
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-02675-3
This article is cited by
-
Airborne dimethyl sulfide (DMS) cues dimethylsulfoniopropionate (DMSP) increases in the intertidal green alga Ulva fenestrata
Scientific Reports (2023)
-
Phylogeny and biogeography of the algal DMS-releasing enzyme in the global ocean
ISME Communications (2023)
-
Dimethyl sulfide mediates microbial predator–prey interactions between zooplankton and algae in the ocean
Nature Microbiology (2021)
-
Comparative volatilomics of coral endosymbionts from one- and comprehensive two-dimensional gas chromatography approaches
Marine Biology (2021)
-
Chemoattraction to dimethyl sulfide links the sulfur, iron, and carbon cycles in high-latitude oceans
Biogeochemistry (2018)
Comments
By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.