Background & Summary

Well over two years after the official outbreak1, it is evident that the COVID-19 pandemic has affected all domains of human life, including the economic and social fabric of societies2 as well as people’s physical and mental health3. At the time of writing, the world reached 850 million confirmed infections and up to 18 million deaths4. The detrimental effects of the pandemic extend beyond physical health with evidence of increased stress levels5 and suicide rates6, along with deterioration of general well-being7. Such findings reflect the cautionary warnings by Taylor8 that the psychological and societal effects are “likely to be more pronounced, more widespread, and longer-lasting than the purely somatic effects of the infection”8, p.23.

In the early stages of the pandemic, when vaccines were not yet available, governments introduced non-pharmaceutical interventions to reduce the spread of the SARS-CoV-2 virus9. Various contact-restricting policies (e.g., stay-at-home recommendations, curfews, police hours, partial or complete lock-downs) were enacted, and citizens were advised to adhere to public health recommendations (e.g., hand washing, face masks, and spatial distancing). It quickly became clear that behavioural science had a major role to play10.

On April 11th, a team of researchers launched a call for international collaboration in social and moral psychology. The initiative quickly gained momentum, gathering a consortium of over 250 academics worldwide. The aim of this project was to collect data from as many countries as possible to serve as a public good for the scientific community by allowing future research to draw on this broad database collected during this early phase of the COVID-19 pandemic. The survey, developed by the initial team, was circulated among the national teams, who provided feedback, translated it into 32 languages, and disseminated it online. The project concluded with responses from a total of 51,404 participants from 69 countries, 77 samples, between April 22nd and June 3rd, 2020.

A key goal of the project was to test the hypothesis that national identity predicts support for public health measures during the COVID-19 pandemic, which has since been confirmed11,12. In addition to collecting variables to test this hypothesis, we collected data on a variety of other social and moral constructs to make of our multi-country large-scale survey a rich resource for future research. The survey focused on the following areas: on a) COVID-19 beliefs and compliance behaviours (COVID-19 public health support, COVID-19 risk perception, COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs, and COVID-19 testing behaviour); b) identity and social attitudes (national identification, national narcissism, and social belonging); c) ideology (political ideology); d) health and well-being (subjective physical health, a wealth ladder ranking, and psychological well-being); e) moral beliefs and motivation (generosity, morality as cooperation, moral identity, and moral circle); f) personality traits and dispositions (open-mindedness, self-esteem, trait optimism, trait self-control, narcissism, and cognitive reflection); and g) demographic variables (i.e., sex, age, marital status, number of children, and employment status).

Using this dataset, project team members have pre-registered a variety of secondary hypotheses (see icsmp-covid19.netlify.app/preregistration), several of which have already been published11,12,13,14,15,16,17,18,19,20,21,22,23. In this paper, we present the complete ICSMP datasets to facilitate its findability, accessibility, interoperability, and reuse (FAIR;24,25) and maximize its educational impact26,27,28.

Methods

When possible, we used articles published in Nature Scientific Data presenting social sciences data as blueprints5,29. Given the urgent call for COVID-19 research, this study received an umbrella ethical approval from the University of Kent (see osf.io/ce638) but also complied with local ethics, norms, and regulations in the countries where the data were collected.

Participants

A total of 51,404 individuals from 77 samples across 69 countries participated in our survey. The inclusion criteria were the following: being 18 years of age and older, and giving informed consent (although researchers were encouraged to, ideally, recruit representative samples regarding age and gender). Data were collected between April 22nd and June 3rd, 2020. Figure 1 displays where the data were collected, coloured according to national sample size. Figure 2 displays the proportion of respondents in relation to the full sample. Figure 3 shows when the data were collected in each country.

Fig. 1
figure 1

A world map visualizing the number of participants in each surveyed country. Note: This heat map shows the number of respondents from each country. The gray areas are the countries that are not covered by the data, and the colour scale shows the size of the sample in accordance with the scale on the lower left side.

Fig. 2
figure 2

International Collaboration on the Social and Moral Psychology of COVID-19: Investigated constructs, items and variables.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Gantt Chart illustrating the data collection periods for each surveyed country.

Demographic variables across countries are summarised in several tables: Tables 1, 2 show the number of participants, the mean proportion of non-missing ‘valid’ answers, and age. Tables 3, 4 illustrate the distribution of gender; Tables 5, 6 show employment status; and Tables 79 show marital status and number of children. When multiple samples were collected within the same country, data were split into numbered subgroups (e.g., for Brazil, which has three samples, they were flagged as Brazil_1, Brazil_2 and Brazil_3). Note that in the tables above, we kept country subsamples separated to highlight they were collected by different teams, often using different sampling methodologies or languages, which impact their characteristics (e.g., representativeness).

Table 1 Sample size, average proportion of valid answers, age of respondents and the number of data collections in 69 countries (A-M).
Table 2 Sample size, average proportion of valid answers, age of respondents and the number of data collections in 69 countries (N-V).
Table 3 Distribution of sex in 69 countries (A-M).
Table 4 Distribution of sex in 69 countries (N-V).
Table 5 Distribution of employment status in 69 countries (A-M).
Table 6 Distribution of employment status in 69 countries (N-V).
Table 7 Distribution of marital status and number of children in 69 countries (A-H).
Table 8 Distribution of marital status and number of children in 69 countries (I-R).
Table 9 Distribution of marital status and number of children in 69 countries (S-V).

For the most part, participants were recruited via professional survey research companies and were incentivised to participate. In countries that, to our knowledge, did not possess polling infrastructure30, incentivising participants was not feasible. To collect data in these countries, leaders of national teams relied on online volunteers recruited via media appeals, mailing lists, advertisements on news aggregators, local communities and bloggers, and private messaging apps such as WhatsApp or WeChat.

Materials

The measures we used are illustrated in Figs. 4, 5 along with the specific items listed for each measure. In most cases, participants’ responses were collected on a scale from 0 = ‘strongly disagree’ to 10 = ‘strongly agree’, with 5 = ‘neither disagree nor agree’. In some cases, when more appropriate, we used other response scales (e.g., the generosity measure, where a 0–100% response scale was applied to hypothetical donations). In total, we collected 98 unique variables and meta-data. To ensure participants’ anonymity, no data that would allow their identification were collected.

Fig. 4
figure 4

International Collaboration on the Social and Moral Psychology of COVID-19: Investigated constructs, items and variables.

Fig. 5
figure 5

International Collaboration on the Social and Moral Psychology of COVID-19: Investigated constructs, items and variables.

COVID-19 Beliefs and compliance

Four constructs: COVID-19 public health support, COVID-19 risk perception, COVID-19 conspiracy theory beliefs, and COVID-19 testing behaviour. The public health support construct, in turn, is composed of three measures: spatial distancing, physical hygiene, and policy support. These are ad-hoc scales that we developed ourselves.

Identity and social attitudes

Three constructs: national identification31, national narcissism32, and social belonging33.

Ideology

One construct: political ideology. Participants self-reported their political orientation according to a single item on a scale from 0 (“Very left-leaning”) to 10 (“Very right-leaning”). This measure has been shown to account for a significant proportion of the variance in voting intentions in American presidential elections between 1972 and 200434 and 201635,36,37. In fact, using a single-item scale to measure political ideology has been a common practice in political psychology literature, providing substantive evidence for the validity of the measure both across national and international research38,39. However, even if the symbolic ideology can be a useful and parsimonious instrument to study political attitudes, when interpreting results, users should be attentive to the political and cultural applicability, psychometric validity, and generalisability of measures of political ideology40,41,42.

Health and well-being

Three constructs: subjective physical health, wealth ladder, and psychological well-being. Each of these scales relied on well-validated instruments43,44,45.

Moral beliefs and motivation

Four constructs: generosity46, morality as cooperation47, moral identity48, and moral circle49.

Personality traits

Six constructs: open-mindedness50, self-esteem51, trait optimism52, trait self-control53, narcissism54, and cognitive reflection55.

Demographics

Six questions: age, number of children, employment status, marital status, gender, and urbanicity.

Metadata and attention check

An attention check was used to mitigate negative impact on data quality from potential non-human responses and the likelihood of biasing data and subsequent analysis of low base-rate outcomes—such as endorsement of COVID-19 conspiracies. We collected typical questionnaire metadata (e.g., start, record, and end dates, duration, and language). In addition, we created an internal participant ID, added ISO2 and ISO3 country codes, and sample representativeness.

Translation

The survey instrument was drafted in English and translated into other languages using the standard forward-backward method (i.e., members of national teams were advised to split members into forward-translating the survey into the local language and back-translating it into English, and then have the two groups discuss and resolve discrepancies). In total, the survey instrument was translated into 32 languages, including adaptations of region-specific dialects or vernaculars. Specifically, from English into Arabic, Bengali, Bulgarian, Croatian, Danish, Dutch, Finnish, French, German, Greek, Hebrew, Hungary, Italian, Japanese, Korean, Kurdish, Latvian, Macedonian, Mandarin simplified, Mandarin traditional, Nepali, Norwegian, Polish, Portuguese, Romanian, Russian, Serbian, Slovak, Spanish, Swedish, Turkish, and Ukrainian (see osf.io/tfsza at sub-folder Translations).

Data cleaning

We received individual data files from each national team. To merge these raw data, minor modifications were introduced, which we delineate in this section. First, we renamed columns to match across data sets, reordered variables alphabetically, and standardised variable labels. Furthermore, all missing values and values denoting the absence of a response were converted to NAs (not available). When ambiguous date formats were found (e.g., on start date, end date, and record date), we manually specified the correct format and standardised them. At the second stage, we introduced multiple modifications to clean the data for research. Some modifications were introduced to every national data set, while others were introduced to specific national data sets (both of which are thoroughly reported in the Data Records section). To each national data set, we recoded the attention check (attcheck) into pass (1) or fail (0); standardised generosity items (generosity1–3), recoded CRT items into intuitive (2), correct (1), and incorrect (0); converted the number of children (children) into a variable with a fixed range from zero to ten or more; recoded all participants declaring being older than 100 years old as 100; and we excluded all duplicates (i.e., in case multiple participants were recorded with identical inputs within a national database, only the first input was retained).

Data Records

All materials associated with the ICSMP COVID-19 project can be found on the project’s repository (comprising five folders) hosted by the Open Science Framework (OSF, https://doi.org/10.17605/osf.io/tfsza)56,57. The folder named Code includes an R Markdown document (ICSMP official data.Rmd; osf.io/dwpng) that loads multiple data files (from each national team), cleans them up, merges them into a single data file, generates a data-driven code-book, and saves all outputs. It also includes a reproducible report with all reported numbers, analyses and graphs in this article (Analyses-SciData.html; osf.io/s5c4p; Analyses SciData.Rmd; osf.io/9suyb). The folder named Data includes three sub-folders. The Raw data sub-folder contains the original and unmodified data files from each national team (country data files.zip; osf.io/dqmut). The sub-folder named Cleaned data contains the merged and cleaned dataset, which is provided in a non-proprietary (ICSMP_cleaned_data.csv; osf.io/ypkrc) and a labelled (ICSMP_cleaned_data.sav; at osf.io/8tyj9) file formats. In addition, we included in a sub-folder a dataset that removes observations failing the attention check or filled out less than 50% of the items, both in a non-proprietary (ICSMP_cleaned_data_nobots.csv; osf.io/98fex) and a labelled (ICSMP_cleaned_data_nobots.sav; at osf.io/3yjga) file formats. The Metadata sub-folder provides a thorough itemised description of the data cleaning process in both text (Data Cleaning.docx; osf.io/7udpt) and human-readable change-log (human-readable change log ICSMP.xlsx; osf.io/fydx2).

We also provide a data-driven codebook detailing how each measure was collected—e.g., listing variable names, variable labels, and label values (dt.codebook.xlsx; osf.io/ecva2). The IRB folder contains both the Internal Review Board Ethics application (ICSMP Kent Ethics application full.pdf; osf.io/xt9gr) and Ethics approval (ICSMP Kent Ethics approval.pdf; osf.io/ce638). The folder Sample Type & Representativeness includes the documentation for an internal survey conducted with national team leaders about the employed survey methodology for the data provided (Sample Type & Representativeness.zip; osf.io/fj5xn). The folder Survey Instrument contains the initial English version of our survey instrument along with its Qualtrics.qsf for reproducibility (Survey Instrument.zip; osf.io/nf48q). In the sub-folder Translations, we archived all 32 translated survey instruments along with a report on the languages of conducted surveys per country (i.e., several countries had their surveys in multiple languages per country; Country and language.xlsx; osf.io/wj7d2).

Potential for future research

The data contains four measures of COVID-19 beliefs and compliance, 17 social and moral psychological constructs, and six sociodemographic characteristics, amounting to 27 socially-relevant variables. To quantify the potential of this dataset—and assuming a typical research paper uses between three to five key main constructs plus sociodemographics and controls—we calculated the number of combinations of 17 constructs, taken three, four, and five at a time, yielding a grand total of 9248 possible unique designs. As a demonstration of the broad scope of the ICSMP data, published studies cover a broad range of psychological disciplines, including social psychology13,14, cognitive psychology15,17, political psychology16, moral psychology16,18, economic psychology19 and health sciences20, among others. They explore different populations in reference to the COVID-19 pandemic in terms of age (e.g., older adults see21, marital status19 or nationality (e.g., for a study on the Spanish population, see;22 for Swedish and Chinese population see23), and other socio-demographic characteristics. These all attest to the great potential of the ICSMP data to inspire further research. In sum, the present dataset affords numerous opportunities for cross-cultural research on a plethora of hypotheses. We encourage researchers who consider reusing ICSMP data to examine the list of pre-registrations before beginning a new project so as to avoid duplication (see icsmp-covid19.netlify.app/preregistration).

Data visualisation interface

In addition to the raw data, a dedicated Web application was developed to provide a general overview of the dataset (icsmp.shinyapps.io/icsmp_covid19). The application is based on an R shiny server (rstudio.com/products/shiny), together with the leaflet58 and ggplot259 graphical libraries to generate dynamic plots. All the generated figures can be exported as .png files, and all tables can be exported as .csv files. The Web application allows easy and dynamic generation of illustrations like the figures with maps for each construct with zoomable world maps and static figures and plots for sample and country characteristics. In addition, all tables are embedded with dynamic features for sorting and filtering. To make it more accessible for the readers, both tables and figures are downloadable. The Shiny app has two tabs giving general information about the project and the international consortium. The first tab contains sample descriptions such as sample size, missing data, and attention checks for each country with a Gantt chart showing the dates of data collection. The second tab displays world maps of spatial distancing, policy support, national identity, conspiracy beliefs, national narcissism and morality as cooperation as well as all tables reported in dynamic formats.

Technical Validation

To support the technical quality of the dataset, we conducted an analysis to showcase its reliability (and its diverse applicability to research questions in social sciences and beyond). For completeness, in the analyses that follow, we examined all samples-including those with very few observations, such as Puerto Rico (N = 2), Brazil_3 (N = 6), and Panama (N = 12).

We evaluated the adopted survey methodology utilised by national teams by conducting an internal survey to ensure the accuracy of reported sample types. The inspection showed that 28 samples were quota-based nationally representative samples (36%), 6 used post hoc weights to achieve an approximate level of national representation (8%) which nonetheless should be seen as convenience samples, and 43 were convenience samples (56%), many of which were from low and middle-income countries60. We codified the results of this survey into the cleaned data as the variable ‘sample_coding’ and present a summary in Table 10. National representativeness for the 28 quota-based samples relate to an approximation of the demographic characteristics of age and gender only for each country.

Table 10 Overview of the samples.

Regarding individual-level data quality, Fig. 6 shows a world map of the 69 countries from which data were collected, coloured according to overall percentages of missing data (overall mean = 6.0%). Overall, 95.6% of participants had less than 50% missing data, 92.8% participants had less than 10% missing data, and 24.7% of participants had 0% missing data. Another indicator of data quality is the rate of attention check fails per country. On the last screen of the survey, participants were given the following instructions: “Help us get rid of bots: Please write the number 213 into the comment box.” Participants who wrote “213” were coded as passing the attention check, participants who wrote anything else were coded as failing the attention check, and those who did not reach this screen of the survey were coded as missing data. Figure 6 also shows (bottom plot) a world map coloured according to the rate of attention-check fails across countries. Overall, 90.1% of participants passed the attention check (1.0% failed), and 8.0% did not reach the final screen with the attention check.

Fig. 6
figure 6

Data quality indicators for each surveyed country. Note: The percentage of missing data considered all the questions in the survey (i.e., all sociodemographics and psychological scales”). We calculated, for each country, the mean of the participants’ proportion of missing data across all survey questions, including sociodemographics (this information is also provided in our reproducible report of Fig. 6, where the R code is provided).

The full dataset presents N = 51,404 cases across 69 countries (from 77 samples, 28 of which are quota-based nationally representative), with an average sample size of 745 (SD = 549) and a proportion of valid answers of 95%. The mean age of respondents was 42.93 (SD = 16.04) years, and 50.9% were women (44% males, 0.3% others, and 4.8% unreported). The employment status breakdown shows 44.8% employed full-time, 10.6% part-time, 8.1% unemployed, 10% students, 10.1% retired, 11% other, and 5.3% unreported. The overall marital status shows 33% of respondents were single, 18.7% in a relationship, 42.7% married, and 5.5% unreported. The majority of our participants reported having no children (41.6%), with 16.7% having one child, 20.1%, 9.2%, and 3.9% with two, three and four children, respectively, and 1.7% had five or more children (6.9% unreported). We break down these aggregated results per country. Tables 1, 2 show the number of cases and valid answers, Table 3,4 summarises the distribution of sex, Tables 5, 6 display employment status, and Tables 79 illustrate both marital statuses and the number of children.

We also examined cross-cultural differences in conspiracy beliefs, morality as cooperation, spatial distancing, national narcissism, national identification, and policy support for preventative measures across 69 countries in Fig. 7. Additionally, we showcase patterns of associations between these moral and psychological constructs across gender, ideology and age in Figs. 8, 9. For the association pattern analysis, we excluded samples with less than 490 respondents as recommended for stable correlations61, as well as for the subsequent consistency measure analysis.

Fig. 7
figure 7

Cross-cultural differences in Social & Moral Psychology of COVID-19 across 69 countries. Note: Each world heat map in the figure shows the means score, at the country level, for constructs in the survey. Conspiracy Beliefs - participant’s beliefs in conspiracy theories regarding COVID-19; Morality as Cooperation - participant’s moral concern based on the morality-as-cooperation theory; Spatial Distancing - participant’s support for spatial distancing as a strategy against COVID-19; Collective Narcissism - participant’s narcissism, i.e., an inflated view regarding their ingroup (in this research we focused on nationality); National Identity - participant’s identity attached to belonging to a nation; Policy Support - participant’s support to public policies (e.g., closing parks or schools) as a strategy against COVID-19.

Fig. 8
figure 8

Cross-cultural differences in associations of Social & Moral Psychology of COVID-19 across sex and ideology in 69 countries.

Fig. 9
figure 9

Cross-cultural differences in associations of Social & Moral Psychology of COVID-19 across age in 69 countries.

To examine internal consistency for the main scales, we calculated Cronbach’s Alpha, Omega, Guttman split-half reliability, and proportion of variance explained by a unidimensional factor. This table is available at osf.io/ed7yg and shows indices of internal consistency by country for measures of conspiracy beliefs, morality as cooperation, spatial distancing, national narcissism, national identification, and policy support for preventative measures, respectively. We found that the spatial distancing construct, on average, has the lowest Cronbach’s alpha, followed by morality as cooperation. On average, conspiracy beliefs have the highest Cronbach’s alpha, followed by policy support. These patterns hold for the Omega measures, but when considering Guttman’s split-half reliability, collective narcissism and national identity yield the lowest values. Figures 915 show these patterns visually.

Fig. 10
figure 10

Support for policies in 69 countries.

Fig. 11
figure 11

Collective narcissism in 69 countries.

Fig. 12
figure 12

Spatial distancing in 69 countries.

Fig. 13
figure 13

Morality as cooperation in 69 countries.

Fig. 14
figure 14

National identity in 69 countries.

Fig. 15
figure 15

Cross-cultural differences in Internal Consistency Coefficients (Cronbach’s alpha, McDonald’s Omega, Guttman Split-Half), and variance explained of Social & Moral Psychology Constructs in 69 countries. Note: internal consistency typically refers to correlations between different items on the same test to evaluate the extent to which latent indicators comprising the scale measure the same construct.

Usage Notes

The datasets are shared, cleaned, and ready for analysis. We recommend that interested researchers use the cleaned version of the data (available at https://doi.org/10.17605/osf.io/tfsza)56. The use of the labelled data is also suggested for convenience as it has all variable levels encoded, thus eliminating the need to consult the codebook when using the.csv format.

The Data were imported and cleaned using the R software for statistical analysis62 and packages readr63, haven64, readxl65, dplyr66, psych67, htmltools68, mime69, xfun70, labelled71, sjlabelled72, codebook73, lubridate74.

As previously noted5, those wishing to approximate national representativeness can apply the appropriate survey weights to demographic and countries of interest when random sampling is used (e.g., sex: https://ourworldindata.org/gender-ratio; age: http://data.un.org/Data.aspx?d=POP&f=tableCode%3A22; education: https://ourworldindata.org/global-education; marital status: https://ourworldindata.org/marriages-and-divorces).

To minimize misclassification of text-based responses to the cognitive reflection test (CRT) and the attention check, we used multiple steps of data cleaning using REGEX (regular expressions) as fully detailed in (ICSMP official data.Rmd; osf.io/dwpng) located in the folder named Code. First, we coded the predefined numerical and text values as correct (in the case of CRT, also the values predefined as intuitive). Then, iteratively, we screened the remaining responses and, using REGEX, updated answers. Remaining responses were recoded as incorrect.