Abstract
Humans require a shared conceptualization of others’ emotions for adaptive social functioning. A concept is a mental blueprint that gives our brains parameters for predicting what will happen next. Emotion concepts undergo refinement with development, but it is not known whether their neural representations change in parallel. Here, in a sample of 5–15-year-old children (n = 823), we show that the brain represents different emotion concepts distinctly throughout the cortex, cerebellum and caudate. Patterns of activation to each emotion changed little across development. Using a model-free approach, we show that activation patterns were more similar between older children than between younger children. Moreover, scenes that required inferring negative emotional states elicited higher default mode network activation similarity in older children than younger children. These results suggest that representations of emotion concepts are relatively stable by mid to late childhood and synchronize between individuals during adolescence.
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution
Access options
Access Nature and 54 other Nature Portfolio journals
Get Nature+, our best-value online-access subscription
$29.99 / 30 days
cancel any time
Subscribe to this journal
Receive 12 print issues and online access
$209.00 per year
only $17.42 per issue
Rent or buy this article
Prices vary by article type
from$1.95
to$39.95
Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout






Data availability
MRI data: The first nine releases of the Healthy Brain Network Biobank, an open dataset, were used in these analyses. These data are available at http://fcon_1000.projects.nitrc.org/indi/cmi_healthy_brain_network/sharing_neuro.html.
Video codes: The codes for the videos obtained using the EmoCodes system are available at https://emocodes.org/datasets/.
Code availability
Pre-processing was carried out using the Human Connectome Project minimal processing pipeline (available at https://github.com/Washington-University/HCPpipelines). Additional processing, analyses and plotting were carried out using custom scripts written in Python 3.7.2 (available at https://github.com/catcamacho/hbn_analysis) using the numpy version 1.21.6, scipy version 1.7.3, nibabel version 3.2.1 and pandas version 1.1.2 libraries. Analyses were carried out using the pliers version 0.4.1, statsmodels version 0.13.2 and scikit-learn version 0.24.2 libraries. Plotting was carried out using the seaborn version 0.11.1 and matplotlib version 3.4.2 libraries.
Change history
27 July 2023
A Correction to this paper has been published: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-023-01420-6
References
Alba, J. W. & Hasher, L. Is memory schematic? Psychol. Bull. 93, 203–231 (1983).
Masís-Obando, R., Norman, K. A. & Baldassano, C. Schema representations in distinct brain networks support narrative memory during encoding and retrieval. eLife 11, e70445 (2022).
van Kesteren, M. T. R., Ruiter, D. J., Fernández, G. & Henson, R. N. How schema and novelty augment memory formation. Trends Neurosci. 35, 211–219 (2012).
Darley, J. M. & Fazio, R. H. Expectancy confirmation processes arising in the social interaction sequence. Am. Psychol. 35, 867–881 (1980).
Ruba, A. L. & Pollak, S. D. The development of emotion reasoning in infancy and early childhood. Annu. Rev. Dev. Psychol. 2, 503–531 (2020).
Mills, K. L. et al. Structural brain development between childhood and adulthood: convergence across four longitudinal samples. Neuroimage 141, 273–281 (2016).
Giedd, J. N. & Rapoport, J. L. Structural MRI of pediatric brain development: what have we learned and where are we going? Neuron 67, 728–734 (2010).
Grayson, D. S. & Fair, D. A. Development of large-scale functional networks from birth to adulthood: a guide to the neuroimaging literature. Neuroimage 160, 15–31 (2017).
Malsert, J., Palama, A. & Gentaz, E. Emotional facial perception development in 7, 9 and 11 year-old children: the emergence of a silent eye-tracked emotional other-race effect. PLoS ONE 15, e0233008 (2020).
Batty, M. & Taylor, M. J. The development of emotional face processing during childhood. Dev. Sci. 9, 207–220 (2006).
Lemerise, E. A. & Arsenio, W. F. An integrated model of emotion processes and cognition in social information processing. Child Dev. 71, 107–118 (2000).
Crick, N. R. & Dodge, K. A. A review and reformulation of social information-processing mechanisms in children’s social adjustment. Psychol. Bull. 115, 74–101 (1994).
Mishkin, M., Ungerleider, L. G. & Macko, K. A. Object vision and spatial vision: two cortical pathways. Trends Neurosci. 6, 414–417 (1983).
Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Hertz-Pannier, L. & Dubois, J. Nature and nurture in language acquisition: anatomical and functional brain-imaging studies in infants. Trends Neurosci. 29, 367–373 (2006).
Corbetta, M. & Shulman, G. L. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 215–229 (2002).
Sadaghiani, S. & D’Esposito, M. Functional characterization of the cingulo-opercular network in the maintenance of tonic alertness. Cereb. Cortex 25, 2763–2773 (2015).
Barrett, L. F. & Satpute, A. B. Large-scale brain networks in affective and social neuroscience: towards an integrative functional architecture of the brain. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 23, 361–372 (2013).
Buckner, R. L. & DiNicola, L. M. The brain’s default network: updated anatomy, physiology and evolving insights. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 20, 593–608 (2019).
Satpute, A. B. & Lindquist, K. A. The default mode network’s role in discrete emotion. Trends Cogn. Sci. 23, 851–864 (2019).
Marek, S. & Dosenbach, N. U. F. The frontoparietal network: function, electrophysiology, and importance of individual precision mapping. Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 20, 133–140 (2018).
Lindquist, K. A. & Barrett, L. F. A functional architecture of the human brain: emerging insights from the science of emotion. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 533–540 (2012).
Kragel, P. A., Reddan, M. C., LaBar, K. S. & Wager, T. D. Emotion schemas are embedded in the human visual system. Sci. Adv. 5, eaaw4358 (2019).
Park, A. T. et al. Early stressful experiences are associated with reduced neural responses to naturalistic emotional and social content in children. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 57, 101152 (2022).
Gruskin, D. C., Rosenberg, M. D. & Holmes, A. J. Relationships between depressive symptoms and brain responses during emotional movie viewing emerge in adolescence. Neuroimage 216, 116217 (2020).
Camacho, M. C., Karim, H. T. & Perlman, S. B. Neural architecture supporting active emotion processing in children: a multivariate approach. Neuroimage 188, 171–180 (2019).
Widen, S. C. Children’s interpretation of facial expressions: the long path from valence-based to specific discrete categories. Emot. Rev. 5, 72–77 (2013).
Nook, E. C., Sasse, S. F., Lambert, H. K., McLaughlin, K. A. & Somerville, L. H. The nonlinear development of emotion differentiation: granular emotional experience is low in adolescence. Psychol. Sci. 29, 1346–1357 (2018).
Nook, E. C. et al. Charting the development of emotion comprehension and abstraction from childhood to adulthood using observer-rated and linguistic measures. Emotion 20, 773–792 (2020).
Motta-Mena, N. V. & Scherf, K. S. Pubertal development shapes perception of complex facial expressions. Dev. Sci. 20, e12451 (2017).
Thomas, K. M. et al. Amygdala response to facial expressions in children and adults. Biol. Psychiatry 49, 309–316 (2001).
Wiggins, J. L. et al. Developmental differences in the neural mechanisms of facial emotion labeling. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 11, 172–181 (2016).
Marusak, H. A., Carré, J. M. & Thomason, M. E. The stimuli drive the response: an fMRI study of youth processing adult or child emotional face stimuli. Neuroimage 83, 679–689 (2013).
Ladouceur, C. D., Schlund, M. W. & Segreti, A.-M. Positive reinforcement modulates fronto-limbic systems subserving emotional interference in adolescents. Behav. Brain Res. 338, 109–117 (2018).
Hoehl, S., Brauer, J., Brasse, G., Striano, T. & Friederici, A. D. Children’s processing of emotions expressed by peers and adults: an fMRI study. Soc. Neurosci. 5, 543–559 (2010).
Haller, S. P. et al. Reliability of neural activation and connectivity during implicit face emotion processing in youth. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 31, 67–73 (2018).
Lobaugh, N. J., Gibson, E. & Taylor, M. J. Children recruit distinct neural systems for implicit emotional face processing. Neuroreport 17, 215–219 (2006).
Guyer, A. E. et al. A developmental examination of amygdala response to facial expressions. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 20, 1565–1582 (2008).
Pagliaccio, D. et al. Functional brain activation to emotional and nonemotional faces in healthy children: evidence for developmentally undifferentiated amygdala function during the school-age period. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 13, 771–789 (2013).
Hare, T. A. et al. Biological substrates of emotional reactivity and regulation in adolescence during an emotional go-nogo task. Biol. Psychiatry 63, 927–934 (2008).
Somerville, L. H., Fani, N. & McClure-Tone, E. B. Behavioral and neural representation of emotional facial expressions across the lifespan. Dev. Neuropsychol. 36, 408–428 (2011).
Widen, S. C. & Russell, J. A. Children acquire emotion categories gradually. Cogn. Dev. 23, 291–312 (2008).
Widen, S. C. & Russell, J. A. Children’s scripts for social emotions: causes and consequences are more central than are facial expressions. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 28, 565–581 (2010).
Wu, Y., Schulz, L. E., Frank, M. C. & Gweon, H. Emotion as information in early social learning. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 30, 468–475 (2021).
Cantlon, J. F. The balance of rigor and reality in developmental neuroscience. Neuroimage 216, 116464 (2020).
Vanderwal, T., Eilbott, J. & Castellanos, F. X. Movies in the magnet: naturalistic paradigms in developmental functional neuroimaging. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 36, 100600 (2019).
Eickhoff, S. B., Milham, M. & Vanderwal, T. Towards clinical applications of movie fMRI. Neuroimage 217, 116860 (2020).
Glasser, M. F. et al. The minimal preprocessing pipelines for the Human Connectome Project. Neuroimage 80, 105–124 (2013).
Camacho, M. C. et al. EmoCodes: a standardized coding system for socio-emotional content in complex video stimuli. Affect. Sci. 3, 168–181 (2022).
Sander, D., Grandjean, D. & Scherer, K. R. An appraisal-driven componential approach to the emotional brain. Emot. Rev. 10, 219–231 (2018).
Pessoa, L. Understanding emotion with brain networks. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 19, 19–25 (2018).
Lindquist, K. A. & MacCormack, J. K. Comment: Constructionism is a multilevel framework for affective science. Emot. Rev. 6, 134–135 (2014).
Skerry, A. E. & Saxe, R. Neural representations of emotion are organized around abstract event features. Curr. Biol. 25, 1945–1954 (2015).
Tracy, J. L. & Randles, D. Four models of basic emotions: a review of Ekman and Cordaro, Izard, Levenson, and Panksepp and Watt. Emot. Rev. 3, 397–405 (2011).
Panksepp, J. & Watt, D. What is basic about basic emotions? Lasting lessons from affective neuroscience. Emot. Rev. 3, 387–396 (2011).
Ogren, M. & Johnson, S. P. Factors facilitating early emotion understanding development: contributions to individual differences. Hum. Dev. 64, 108–118 (2020).
Ogren, M. & Sandhofer, C. M. Emotion words link faces to emotional scenarios in early childhood. Emotion 22, 167–178 (2022).
Camras, L. A. & Allison, K. Children’s understanding of emotional facial expressions and verbal labels. J. Nonverbal Behav. 9, 84–94 (1985).
Lawrence, K., Campbell, R. & Skuse, D. Age, gender, and puberty influence the development of facial emotion recognition. Front. Psychol. 6, 761 (2015).
Keulers, E. H. H., Evers, E. A. T., Stiers, P. & Jolles, J. Age, sex, and pubertal phase influence mentalizing about emotions and actions in adolescents. Dev. Neuropsychol. 35, 555–569 (2010).
Dai, J. & Scherf, K. S. Puberty and functional brain development in humans: convergence in findings? Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 39, 100690 (2019).
Hoemann, K., Xu, F. & Barrett, L. F. Emotion words, emotion concepts, and emotional development in children: a constructionist hypothesis. Dev. Psychol. 55, 1830–1849 (2019).
Richardson, H., Lisandrelli, G., Riobueno-Naylor, A. & Saxe, R. Development of the social brain from age three to twelve years. Nat. Commun. 9, 1027 (2018).
Fan, F. et al. Development of the default-mode network during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal resting-state fMRI study. Neuroimage 226, 117581 (2021).
Gordon, E. M. et al. Three distinct sets of connector hubs integrate human brain function. Cell Rep. 24, 1687–1695 (2018).
Brandman, T., Malach, R. & Simony, E. The surprising role of the default mode network in naturalistic perception. Commun. Biol. 4, 79 (2021).
da Silva, P. H. R., Rondinoni, C. & Leoni, R. F. Non-classical behavior of the default mode network regions during an information processing task. Brain Struct. Funct. 225, 2553–2562 (2020).
Gordon, E. M. et al. Default-mode network streams for coupling to language and control systems. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 117, 17308–17319 (2020).
Kaiser, R. H., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Wager, T. D. & Pizzagalli, D. A. Large-scale network dysfunction in major depressive disorder. JAMA Psychiatry 72, 603 (2015).
Vargas, C., López-Jaramillo, C. & Vieta, E. A systematic literature review of resting state network—functional MRI in bipolar disorder. J. Affect. Disord. 150, 727–735 (2013).
Broyd, S. J. et al. Default-mode brain dysfunction in mental disorders: a systematic review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 33, 279–296 (2009).
Williams, L. M. Defining biotypes for depression and anxiety based on large-scale circuit dysfunction: a theoretical review of the evidence and future directions for clinical translation. Depress. Anxiety 34, 9–24 (2017).
Lettieri, G. et al. Emotionotopy in the human right temporo-parietal cortex. Nat. Commun. 10, 5568 (2019).
Lettieri, G. et al. Default and control network connectivity dynamics track the stream of affect at multiple timescales. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 17, 461–469 (2022).
Kragel, P. A. & LaBar, K. S. Multivariate neural biomarkers of emotional states are categorically distinct. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 10, 1437–1448 (2015).
Kragel, P. A., Knodt, A. R., Hariri, A. R. & Labar, K. S. Decoding spontaneous emotional states in the human brain. PLoS Biol. 14, e2000106 (2016).
Kragel, P. A. & Labar, K. S. Decoding the nature of emotion in the brain. Trends Cogn. Sci. 20, 444–455 (2016).
Chang, L. J. et al. Endogenous variation in ventromedial prefrontal cortex state dynamics during naturalistic viewing reflects affective experience. Sci. Adv. 7, eabf7129 (2021).
Alexander, L. M. et al. An open resource for transdiagnostic research in pediatric mental health and learning disorders. Sci. Data 4, 170181 (2017).
Nook, E. C., Sasse, S. F., Lambert, H. K., McLaughlin, K. A. & Somerville, L. H. The nonlinear development of emotion differentiation: granular emotional experience is low in adolescence. Psychol. Sci. 29, 1346–1357 (2018).
Nook, E. C. et al. Charting the development of emotion comprehension and abstraction from childhood to adulthood using observer-rated and linguistic measures. Emotion 20, 773–792 (2020).
Motta-Mena, N. V. & Scherf, K. S. Pubertal development shapes perception of complex facial expressions. Dev. Sci. 20, e12451 (2017).
Thomas, L. A., De Bellis, M. D., Graham, R. & LaBar, K. S. Development of emotional facial recognition in late childhood and adolescence. Dev. Sci. 10, 547–558 (2007).
Durand, K., Gallay, M., Seigneuric, A., Robichon, F. & Baudouin, J.-Y. The development of facial emotion recognition: the role of configural information. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 97, 14–27 (2007).
Wu, M. et al. Age-related changes in amygdala-frontal connectivity during emotional face processing from childhood into young adulthood. Hum. Brain Mapp. 37, 1684–1695 (2016).
Harris, C. R. et al. Array programming with NumPy. Nature 585, 357–362 (2020).
Virtanen, P. et al. SciPy 1.0: fundamental algorithms for scientific computing in Python. Nat. Methods 17, 261–272 (2020).
Andersson, J. L. R. & Sotiropoulos, S. N. An integrated approach to correction for off-resonance effects and subject movement in diffusion MR imaging. Neuroimage 125, 1063–1078 (2016).
Fischl, B. FreeSurfer. Neuroimage 62, 774–781 (2012).
Robinson, E. C. et al. Multimodal surface matching with higher-order smoothness constraints. Neuroimage 167, 453–465 (2018).
Siegel, J. S. et al. Statistical improvements in functional magnetic resonance imaging analyses produced by censoring high-motion data points. Hum. Brain Mapp. 35, 1981–1996 (2014).
Siegel, J. S. et al. Data quality influences observed links between functional connectivity and behavior. Cereb. Cortex 27, 4492–4502 (2017).
Greene, D. J. et al. Behavioral interventions for reducing head motion during MRI scans in children. Neuroimage 171, 234–245 (2018).
Vanderwal, T., Kelly, C., Eilbott, J., Mayes, L. C. & Castellanos, F. X. Inscapes: a movie paradigm to improve compliance in functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroimage 122, 222–232 (2015).
Gordon, E. M. et al. Generation and evaluation of a cortical area parcellation from resting-state correlations. Cereb. Cortex 26, 288–303 (2016).
Seitzman, B. A. et al. A set of functionally-defined brain regions with improved representation of the subcortex and cerebellum. Neuroimage 206, 116290 (2020).
Camacho, M. C. et al. EmoCodes: a standardized coding system for socio-emotional content in complex video stimuli. Affect. Sci. 3, 168–181 (2022).
McNamara, Q., De La Vega, A. & Yarkoni, T. Developing a comprehensive framework for multimodal feature extraction. In Proc. 23rd ACM SIGKDD International Conference on Knowledge Discovery and Data Mining (eds Matwin, S. et al.) 1567–1574 (Association for Computing Machinery, 2017).
Nastase, S. A., Gazzola, V., Hasson, U. & Keysers, C. Measuring shared responses across subjects using intersubject correlation. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 14, 669–687 (2019).
Hasson, U., Nir, Y., Levy, I., Fuhrmann, G. & Malach, R. Intersubject synchronization of cortical activity during natural vision. Science 303, 1634–1640 (2004).
Aviyente, S., Bernat, E. M., Evans, W. S. & Sponheim, S. R. A phase synchrony measure for quantifying dynamic functional integration in the brain. Hum. Brain Mapp. 32, 80–93 (2011).
Glerean, E., Salmi, J., Lahnakoski, J. M., Jääskeläinen, I. P. & Sams, M. Functional magnetic resonance imaging phase synchronization as a measure of dynamic functional connectivity. Brain Connect. 2, 91–101 (2012).
Petersen, A. C., Crockett, L., Richards, M. & Boxer, A. A self-report measure of pubertal status: reliability, validity, and initial norms. J. Youth Adolesc. 17, 117–133 (1988).
Finn, E. S. et al. Idiosynchrony: from shared responses to individual differences during naturalistic neuroimaging. Neuroimage 215, 116828 (2020).
Mantel, N. The detection of disease clustering and a generalized regression approach. Cancer Res. 27, 209–220 (1967).
Benjamini, Y. & Hochberg, Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Stat. Soc. B 57, 289–300 (1995).
Acknowledgements
We thank A. Witherspoon for assistance with video coding, the families who participated in the Healthy Brain Network study and the Child Mind Institute for sharing the data publicly. This work was funded by the National Science Foundation (DGE-1745038 to M.C.C.) and the National Institutes of Health (HD102156 to M.C.C. and MH109589 to D.M.B.).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
M.C.C.: conceptualization, methodology, software, validation, formal analysis, data curation, writing—original draft, writing—review and editing, visualization and project administration. A.N.N.: conceptualization, methodology, supervision and writing—review and editing. D.B.: conceptualization, investigation and writing—review and editing. E.F.: conceptualization, writing—original draft and writing—review and editing. D.C.S.: conceptualization, investigation and writing—review and editing. L.F.: conceptualization, investigation and writing—review and editing. J.P.C.: conceptualization, supervision and writing—review and editing. C.M.S.: conceptualization, supervision and writing—review and editing. D.M.B.: conceptualization, methodology, resources, supervision, writing—review and editing and funding acquisition.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Peer review
Peer review information
Nature Neuroscience thanks Erik Nook, Heini Saarimäki and the other, anonymous, reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work.
Additional information
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Extended data
Extended Data Fig. 1 Emotion-specific activation differences maps.
(a) Difference maps for general emotions (column minus row). Mean activation maps are shown on the diagonal. (b) Difference maps for specific emotions (column minus row). Mean activation maps are shown on the diagonal.
Extended Data Fig. 2 Parcel-wise Pearson Correlations between chronological age and activation to each emotion.
Age explained at most 3.8% of the variance in parcel-level activation (Age-Activation r2 range: Negative 0-0.017, Positive 0-0.030, Anger 0-0.020, Happy 0-0.026, Sad 0-0.023, Excited 0-0.028, Fearful 0-0.023; Puberty-Activation r2 range: Negative 0-0.029, Positive 0-0.030, Anger 0-0.024, Happy 0-0.038, Sad 0-0.014, Excited 0-0.030, Fearful 0-0.021).
Extended Data Fig. 3 Significant parcels and similarity coefficients for each model of maturation after FDR correction.
(a) Average inter-subject correlations for each parcel for each movie. Significant parcels (one-sided Pearson’s r; permutation based and FDR-corrected p < 0.05) are outlined in black. (b) Significant parcels and their coefficient magnitudes for the Convergence similarity model of maturation. (c) Significant parcels for the Nearest Neighbor and (d) Divergence models of maturation across chronological age and puberty.
Extended Data Fig. 4 Full sample dynamic analysis results for The Present.
(a) Dynamic synchrony across the full sample with replicating peaks in synchrony shaded purple. Peaks at least 5 seconds wide and with a prominence higher than the 95th percentile value for that network (permutation-based p < 0.001). Parcels were limited to those that were significantly correlated across the sample at the group level after FDR correction. (b) Video feature means within the peaks were compared to features outside of the peaks using a two-sided t-test to test if specific video features elicited increased synchronization. Plotted features were those that were significantly different (FDR-corrected p < 0.05). Bars are plotted at the mean with dots indicating each measure used in computing the mean. (C) Violin plots of mean synchrony values by network. White dots indicate median value, the box the 50% interquartile range, and the whiskers each the upper and lower 25%. The dashed horizontal line indicates the value at permuted p < 0.05 after FDR correction.
Extended Data Fig. 5 Dynamic Synchrony analysis results in the oldest children for The Present.
(a) Network dynamic activation similarity (synchrony) for each the oldest and youngest children in the sample. Included parcels are shown to the left of each trace. Shaded areas denote significant increases in synchrony (1.5 standard deviations above the mean) in the oldest children. (b) Bar plots of the mean value show the results from the video feature analysis comparing portions of the video within peaks of inter-subject synchrony to outside the peaks. Dots overlaid on the bar plots indicate each measure used in computing the mean Only features that significantly differed after FDR-correction are plotted.
Extended Data Fig. 6 Network-wise dynamic similarity plots for 3 age groups: oldest, middle, and youngest, for each movie. Included parcels are shown to the left of each trace.
Shaded areas denote significant increases in synchrony (1.5 standard deviations above the mean) in the oldest children.
Supplementary information
Supplementary Information
Supplementary Methods and Analyses (Appendix A and Appendix B) and Figures and Tables (Appendix C).
Rights and permissions
Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.
About this article
Cite this article
Camacho, M.C., Nielsen, A.N., Balser, D. et al. Large-scale encoding of emotion concepts becomes increasingly similar between individuals from childhood to adolescence. Nat Neurosci 26, 1256–1266 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-023-01358-9
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-023-01358-9